Noise, Economy, and the Emergence of Information Structure in a Laboratory Language

The acceptability of sentences in natural language is constrained not only grammaticality, but also by the relationship between what is being conveyed and such factors as context and the beliefs of interlocutors. In many languages the critical element in a sentence (its focus) must be given grammati...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Cognitive science 2019-03, Vol.43 (2), p.e12717-n/a
Hauptverfasser: Stevens, Jon S., Roberts, Gareth
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page n/a
container_issue 2
container_start_page e12717
container_title Cognitive science
container_volume 43
creator Stevens, Jon S.
Roberts, Gareth
description The acceptability of sentences in natural language is constrained not only grammaticality, but also by the relationship between what is being conveyed and such factors as context and the beliefs of interlocutors. In many languages the critical element in a sentence (its focus) must be given grammatical prominence. There are different accounts of the nature of focus marking. Some researchers treat it as the grammatical realization of a potentially arbitrary feature of universal grammar and do not provide an explicit account of its origins; others have argued, however, that focus marking is a (grammaticalized) functional solution to the problem of efficiently transmitting information via a noisy channel. By adding redundancy to highlight critical elements in particular, focus protects key parts of the message from noise. If this information‐theoretic account is true, then we should expect focus‐like behavior to emerge even in non‐linguistic communication systems given sufficient noise and pressures for efficiency. We tested this in an experiment in which participants played a simple communication game in which they had to click cells on a grid to communicate one of two line figures drawn across the grid. We manipulated the noise, available time, and required effort, and measured patterns of redundancy. Because the lines in many cases overlapped, meaning that only some parts of each line could be used to distinguish it from the other, we were able to compare the extent to which effort was expended on adding redundancy to critical (non‐overlapping) and non‐critical (overlapping) parts of the message. The results supported the information‐theoretic account of focus and shed light on the emergence of information structure in language.
doi_str_mv 10.1111/cogs.12717
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_2186151887</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><ericid>EJ1206726</ericid><sourcerecordid>2186151887</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c3877-187ecffc4244ef56874c05a8dcfb426e3253f6b25c55cc97eed01c93c6bdf0fb3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9kDFv2zAQRomiQe04Xbq34BgUUcIjRVIeA8NNExjJ4HYWqNPRVWGJKSkh8L-PHKUZc8sd8D18BzzGvoC4hHGuMOzSJUgL9gObg9aQGSuWH9lcKJNnQoKasdOU_gohjFHLT2ymRCGUAJiz7X1oEl3wNYYutIcL7rqa93-Ir1uKO-qQePD8tvMhtq5vQse3fRywHyLxpuOOb1wVoutDPIxntxvcjs7YiXf7RJ9f94L9_rH-tfqZbR5ublfXmwxVYW0GhSX0HnOZ5-S1KWyOQruiRl_l0pCSWnlTSY1aIy4tUS0AlwpNVXvhK7Vg51PvYwz_Bkp92TYJab93HYUhlRIKAxqKwo7o9wnFGFKK5MvH2LQuHkoQ5VFieZRYvkgc4W-vvUPVUv2G_rc2Al8ngGKDb_H6DqQwVpoxhyl_avZ0eOdVuXq42U5PnwHfF4Xe</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2186151887</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Noise, Economy, and the Emergence of Information Structure in a Laboratory Language</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>Access via Wiley Online Library</source><source>EBSCOhost Education Source</source><source>Wiley Online Library (Open Access Collection)</source><creator>Stevens, Jon S. ; Roberts, Gareth</creator><creatorcontrib>Stevens, Jon S. ; Roberts, Gareth</creatorcontrib><description>The acceptability of sentences in natural language is constrained not only grammaticality, but also by the relationship between what is being conveyed and such factors as context and the beliefs of interlocutors. In many languages the critical element in a sentence (its focus) must be given grammatical prominence. There are different accounts of the nature of focus marking. Some researchers treat it as the grammatical realization of a potentially arbitrary feature of universal grammar and do not provide an explicit account of its origins; others have argued, however, that focus marking is a (grammaticalized) functional solution to the problem of efficiently transmitting information via a noisy channel. By adding redundancy to highlight critical elements in particular, focus protects key parts of the message from noise. If this information‐theoretic account is true, then we should expect focus‐like behavior to emerge even in non‐linguistic communication systems given sufficient noise and pressures for efficiency. We tested this in an experiment in which participants played a simple communication game in which they had to click cells on a grid to communicate one of two line figures drawn across the grid. We manipulated the noise, available time, and required effort, and measured patterns of redundancy. Because the lines in many cases overlapped, meaning that only some parts of each line could be used to distinguish it from the other, we were able to compare the extent to which effort was expended on adding redundancy to critical (non‐overlapping) and non‐critical (overlapping) parts of the message. The results supported the information‐theoretic account of focus and shed light on the emergence of information structure in language.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0364-0213</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1551-6709</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12717</identifier><identifier>PMID: 30803011</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: Wiley-Blackwell</publisher><subject>Behavior Patterns ; Communication ; Communication (Thought Transfer) ; Comprehension ; Cultural evolution ; Difficulty Level ; Experimental semiotics ; Humans ; Information structure ; Information Theory ; Laboratory languages ; Linguistic focus ; Noise ; Pragmatics ; Psycholinguistics ; Semantics ; Speech Perception ; Time</subject><ispartof>Cognitive science, 2019-03, Vol.43 (2), p.e12717-n/a</ispartof><rights>2019 Cognitive Science Society, Inc.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c3877-187ecffc4244ef56874c05a8dcfb426e3253f6b25c55cc97eed01c93c6bdf0fb3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c3877-187ecffc4244ef56874c05a8dcfb426e3253f6b25c55cc97eed01c93c6bdf0fb3</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/pdf/10.1111%2Fcogs.12717$$EPDF$$P50$$Gwiley$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1111%2Fcogs.12717$$EHTML$$P50$$Gwiley$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,1417,1433,27924,27925,45574,45575,46409,46833</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ1206726$$DView record in ERIC$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30803011$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Stevens, Jon S.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Roberts, Gareth</creatorcontrib><title>Noise, Economy, and the Emergence of Information Structure in a Laboratory Language</title><title>Cognitive science</title><addtitle>Cogn Sci</addtitle><description>The acceptability of sentences in natural language is constrained not only grammaticality, but also by the relationship between what is being conveyed and such factors as context and the beliefs of interlocutors. In many languages the critical element in a sentence (its focus) must be given grammatical prominence. There are different accounts of the nature of focus marking. Some researchers treat it as the grammatical realization of a potentially arbitrary feature of universal grammar and do not provide an explicit account of its origins; others have argued, however, that focus marking is a (grammaticalized) functional solution to the problem of efficiently transmitting information via a noisy channel. By adding redundancy to highlight critical elements in particular, focus protects key parts of the message from noise. If this information‐theoretic account is true, then we should expect focus‐like behavior to emerge even in non‐linguistic communication systems given sufficient noise and pressures for efficiency. We tested this in an experiment in which participants played a simple communication game in which they had to click cells on a grid to communicate one of two line figures drawn across the grid. We manipulated the noise, available time, and required effort, and measured patterns of redundancy. Because the lines in many cases overlapped, meaning that only some parts of each line could be used to distinguish it from the other, we were able to compare the extent to which effort was expended on adding redundancy to critical (non‐overlapping) and non‐critical (overlapping) parts of the message. The results supported the information‐theoretic account of focus and shed light on the emergence of information structure in language.</description><subject>Behavior Patterns</subject><subject>Communication</subject><subject>Communication (Thought Transfer)</subject><subject>Comprehension</subject><subject>Cultural evolution</subject><subject>Difficulty Level</subject><subject>Experimental semiotics</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Information structure</subject><subject>Information Theory</subject><subject>Laboratory languages</subject><subject>Linguistic focus</subject><subject>Noise</subject><subject>Pragmatics</subject><subject>Psycholinguistics</subject><subject>Semantics</subject><subject>Speech Perception</subject><subject>Time</subject><issn>0364-0213</issn><issn>1551-6709</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2019</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><recordid>eNp9kDFv2zAQRomiQe04Xbq34BgUUcIjRVIeA8NNExjJ4HYWqNPRVWGJKSkh8L-PHKUZc8sd8D18BzzGvoC4hHGuMOzSJUgL9gObg9aQGSuWH9lcKJNnQoKasdOU_gohjFHLT2ymRCGUAJiz7X1oEl3wNYYutIcL7rqa93-Ir1uKO-qQePD8tvMhtq5vQse3fRywHyLxpuOOb1wVoutDPIxntxvcjs7YiXf7RJ9f94L9_rH-tfqZbR5ublfXmwxVYW0GhSX0HnOZ5-S1KWyOQruiRl_l0pCSWnlTSY1aIy4tUS0AlwpNVXvhK7Vg51PvYwz_Bkp92TYJab93HYUhlRIKAxqKwo7o9wnFGFKK5MvH2LQuHkoQ5VFieZRYvkgc4W-vvUPVUv2G_rc2Al8ngGKDb_H6DqQwVpoxhyl_avZ0eOdVuXq42U5PnwHfF4Xe</recordid><startdate>201903</startdate><enddate>201903</enddate><creator>Stevens, Jon S.</creator><creator>Roberts, Gareth</creator><general>Wiley-Blackwell</general><scope>7SW</scope><scope>BJH</scope><scope>BNH</scope><scope>BNI</scope><scope>BNJ</scope><scope>BNO</scope><scope>ERI</scope><scope>PET</scope><scope>REK</scope><scope>WWN</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>201903</creationdate><title>Noise, Economy, and the Emergence of Information Structure in a Laboratory Language</title><author>Stevens, Jon S. ; Roberts, Gareth</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c3877-187ecffc4244ef56874c05a8dcfb426e3253f6b25c55cc97eed01c93c6bdf0fb3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2019</creationdate><topic>Behavior Patterns</topic><topic>Communication</topic><topic>Communication (Thought Transfer)</topic><topic>Comprehension</topic><topic>Cultural evolution</topic><topic>Difficulty Level</topic><topic>Experimental semiotics</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Information structure</topic><topic>Information Theory</topic><topic>Laboratory languages</topic><topic>Linguistic focus</topic><topic>Noise</topic><topic>Pragmatics</topic><topic>Psycholinguistics</topic><topic>Semantics</topic><topic>Speech Perception</topic><topic>Time</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Stevens, Jon S.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Roberts, Gareth</creatorcontrib><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Ovid)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>ERIC( SilverPlatter )</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>Education Resources Information Center (ERIC)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Cognitive science</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Stevens, Jon S.</au><au>Roberts, Gareth</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><ericid>EJ1206726</ericid><atitle>Noise, Economy, and the Emergence of Information Structure in a Laboratory Language</atitle><jtitle>Cognitive science</jtitle><addtitle>Cogn Sci</addtitle><date>2019-03</date><risdate>2019</risdate><volume>43</volume><issue>2</issue><spage>e12717</spage><epage>n/a</epage><pages>e12717-n/a</pages><issn>0364-0213</issn><eissn>1551-6709</eissn><abstract>The acceptability of sentences in natural language is constrained not only grammaticality, but also by the relationship between what is being conveyed and such factors as context and the beliefs of interlocutors. In many languages the critical element in a sentence (its focus) must be given grammatical prominence. There are different accounts of the nature of focus marking. Some researchers treat it as the grammatical realization of a potentially arbitrary feature of universal grammar and do not provide an explicit account of its origins; others have argued, however, that focus marking is a (grammaticalized) functional solution to the problem of efficiently transmitting information via a noisy channel. By adding redundancy to highlight critical elements in particular, focus protects key parts of the message from noise. If this information‐theoretic account is true, then we should expect focus‐like behavior to emerge even in non‐linguistic communication systems given sufficient noise and pressures for efficiency. We tested this in an experiment in which participants played a simple communication game in which they had to click cells on a grid to communicate one of two line figures drawn across the grid. We manipulated the noise, available time, and required effort, and measured patterns of redundancy. Because the lines in many cases overlapped, meaning that only some parts of each line could be used to distinguish it from the other, we were able to compare the extent to which effort was expended on adding redundancy to critical (non‐overlapping) and non‐critical (overlapping) parts of the message. The results supported the information‐theoretic account of focus and shed light on the emergence of information structure in language.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>Wiley-Blackwell</pub><pmid>30803011</pmid><doi>10.1111/cogs.12717</doi><tpages>33</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0364-0213
ispartof Cognitive science, 2019-03, Vol.43 (2), p.e12717-n/a
issn 0364-0213
1551-6709
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_2186151887
source MEDLINE; Access via Wiley Online Library; EBSCOhost Education Source; Wiley Online Library (Open Access Collection)
subjects Behavior Patterns
Communication
Communication (Thought Transfer)
Comprehension
Cultural evolution
Difficulty Level
Experimental semiotics
Humans
Information structure
Information Theory
Laboratory languages
Linguistic focus
Noise
Pragmatics
Psycholinguistics
Semantics
Speech Perception
Time
title Noise, Economy, and the Emergence of Information Structure in a Laboratory Language
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-29T15%3A21%3A59IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Noise,%20Economy,%20and%20the%20Emergence%20of%20Information%20Structure%20in%20a%20Laboratory%20Language&rft.jtitle=Cognitive%20science&rft.au=Stevens,%20Jon%20S.&rft.date=2019-03&rft.volume=43&rft.issue=2&rft.spage=e12717&rft.epage=n/a&rft.pages=e12717-n/a&rft.issn=0364-0213&rft.eissn=1551-6709&rft_id=info:doi/10.1111/cogs.12717&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E2186151887%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2186151887&rft_id=info:pmid/30803011&rft_ericid=EJ1206726&rfr_iscdi=true