Calling strategies of male túngara frogs in response to dynamic playback
AbstractTúngara frogs produce calls of varying complexity that consist of a whine followed by 0–7 chucks. In previous studies using static playbacks, males increase chuck number in response to calls with 1 versus 0 chucks but not in response to 2 or 3 versus 1 chuck. Here we use dynamic playback in...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Behaviour 2010, Vol.147 (1), p.65-83 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 83 |
---|---|
container_issue | 1 |
container_start_page | 65 |
container_title | Behaviour |
container_volume | 147 |
creator | Goutte, S Kime, N.M Argo IV, T.F Ryan, M.J |
description | AbstractTúngara frogs produce calls of varying complexity that consist of a whine followed by 0–7 chucks. In previous studies using static playbacks, males increase chuck number in response to calls with 1 versus 0 chucks but not in response to 2 or 3 versus 1 chuck. Here we use dynamic playback in which an automated interaction program counts the number of chucks present in a call and responds according to a specified calling strategy which determines the number of chucks in the response call. Males added progressively more chucks in response to the models' strategies of de-escalate, match or escalate; there was a significant difference in the focal males' responses to the de-escalate versus the escalate strategies. Similarly, males changed their calling strategy in response to the de-escalate strategy of the model. There was no evidence of change in calling patterns, as estimated by entropy, among treatments and between experimental and field data. Males produced significantly more chucks in these experiments than in the field, but in both contexts the mean chuck number is low and never approaches the maximum observed in nature. These data suggest that males are cost-sensitive when it comes to adding chucks and that they are more influenced by vocal competitors that de-escalate rather than escalate number of chucks. These are patterns of calling not previously revealed in studies using static playbacks, and this study is another demonstration of the usefulness of dynamic playback in studies of animal communication. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1163/000579509X12483520922205 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>jstor_proqu</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_21406781</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><jstor_id>40599656</jstor_id><sourcerecordid>40599656</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-b452t-91faed3415136df1532f07d3bf1226796f762e1085e3b83c23e9c36d8dd1e0a3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkMtKAzEUhoMoWKuPIGTlbjSXSWay1OKlKlqwi-ImZGaSYdp0MiZTsK_m1hczZaQ7cXU4fN9_OPwAQIwuMeb0CiHEMsGQWGCS5pQRJAghiB2AEWY8TxgVi0Mw2mlJ9MQxOAlhGdeMUTYC04mytmlrGHqvel03OkBn4FpZDfvvr7ZWXkHjXR1g00KvQ-faEJGD1bZV66aEnVXbQpWrU3BklA367HeOwfzudj55SJ5f76eT6-ekSBnpE4GN0hVNMcOUVwYzSgzKKloYTAjPBDcZJxqjnGla5LQkVIsymnlVYY0UHYOL4Wzn3cdGh16um1Bqa1Wr3SZIglPEsxxHMR_E0rsQvDay881a-a3ESO6qk39VF6PnQ3QZeuf3uRQxITjjkScDb0KvP_dc-ZXkGc2YfHyay7cXMkMz_C5vos8Hv_CNtXLpNr6NFf3_yA_wS4n_</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>21406781</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Calling strategies of male túngara frogs in response to dynamic playback</title><source>Jstor Complete Legacy</source><creator>Goutte, S ; Kime, N.M ; Argo IV, T.F ; Ryan, M.J</creator><creatorcontrib>Goutte, S ; Kime, N.M ; Argo IV, T.F ; Ryan, M.J</creatorcontrib><description>AbstractTúngara frogs produce calls of varying complexity that consist of a whine followed by 0–7 chucks. In previous studies using static playbacks, males increase chuck number in response to calls with 1 versus 0 chucks but not in response to 2 or 3 versus 1 chuck. Here we use dynamic playback in which an automated interaction program counts the number of chucks present in a call and responds according to a specified calling strategy which determines the number of chucks in the response call. Males added progressively more chucks in response to the models' strategies of de-escalate, match or escalate; there was a significant difference in the focal males' responses to the de-escalate versus the escalate strategies. Similarly, males changed their calling strategy in response to the de-escalate strategy of the model. There was no evidence of change in calling patterns, as estimated by entropy, among treatments and between experimental and field data. Males produced significantly more chucks in these experiments than in the field, but in both contexts the mean chuck number is low and never approaches the maximum observed in nature. These data suggest that males are cost-sensitive when it comes to adding chucks and that they are more influenced by vocal competitors that de-escalate rather than escalate number of chucks. These are patterns of calling not previously revealed in studies using static playbacks, and this study is another demonstration of the usefulness of dynamic playback in studies of animal communication.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0005-7959</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1568-539X</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1163/000579509X12483520922205</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>The Netherlands: Brill</publisher><subject>ACOUSTIC BEHAVIOUR ; Amphibians ; ANIMAL COMMUNICATION ; Animals ; Anura ; Broadcasting industry ; Capital costs ; DYNAMIC PLAYBACK ; Entropy ; EVOKED VOCAL RESPONSE ; Freshwater ; FROG CALLS ; Frogs ; INTERACTIVE PLAYBACK ; Male animals ; Modeling ; Null hypothesis ; TÚNGARA FROG</subject><ispartof>Behaviour, 2010, Vol.147 (1), p.65-83</ispartof><rights>2010 Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, The Netherlands</rights><rights>Copyright 2010 Koninklijke Brill NV</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-b452t-91faed3415136df1532f07d3bf1226796f762e1085e3b83c23e9c36d8dd1e0a3</citedby></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.jstor.org/stable/pdf/40599656$$EPDF$$P50$$Gjstor$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.jstor.org/stable/40599656$$EHTML$$P50$$Gjstor$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,799,4010,27900,27901,27902,57992,58225</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Goutte, S</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kime, N.M</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Argo IV, T.F</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Ryan, M.J</creatorcontrib><title>Calling strategies of male túngara frogs in response to dynamic playback</title><title>Behaviour</title><addtitle>BEH</addtitle><description>AbstractTúngara frogs produce calls of varying complexity that consist of a whine followed by 0–7 chucks. In previous studies using static playbacks, males increase chuck number in response to calls with 1 versus 0 chucks but not in response to 2 or 3 versus 1 chuck. Here we use dynamic playback in which an automated interaction program counts the number of chucks present in a call and responds according to a specified calling strategy which determines the number of chucks in the response call. Males added progressively more chucks in response to the models' strategies of de-escalate, match or escalate; there was a significant difference in the focal males' responses to the de-escalate versus the escalate strategies. Similarly, males changed their calling strategy in response to the de-escalate strategy of the model. There was no evidence of change in calling patterns, as estimated by entropy, among treatments and between experimental and field data. Males produced significantly more chucks in these experiments than in the field, but in both contexts the mean chuck number is low and never approaches the maximum observed in nature. These data suggest that males are cost-sensitive when it comes to adding chucks and that they are more influenced by vocal competitors that de-escalate rather than escalate number of chucks. These are patterns of calling not previously revealed in studies using static playbacks, and this study is another demonstration of the usefulness of dynamic playback in studies of animal communication.</description><subject>ACOUSTIC BEHAVIOUR</subject><subject>Amphibians</subject><subject>ANIMAL COMMUNICATION</subject><subject>Animals</subject><subject>Anura</subject><subject>Broadcasting industry</subject><subject>Capital costs</subject><subject>DYNAMIC PLAYBACK</subject><subject>Entropy</subject><subject>EVOKED VOCAL RESPONSE</subject><subject>Freshwater</subject><subject>FROG CALLS</subject><subject>Frogs</subject><subject>INTERACTIVE PLAYBACK</subject><subject>Male animals</subject><subject>Modeling</subject><subject>Null hypothesis</subject><subject>TÚNGARA FROG</subject><issn>0005-7959</issn><issn>1568-539X</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2010</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><recordid>eNqFkMtKAzEUhoMoWKuPIGTlbjSXSWay1OKlKlqwi-ImZGaSYdp0MiZTsK_m1hczZaQ7cXU4fN9_OPwAQIwuMeb0CiHEMsGQWGCS5pQRJAghiB2AEWY8TxgVi0Mw2mlJ9MQxOAlhGdeMUTYC04mytmlrGHqvel03OkBn4FpZDfvvr7ZWXkHjXR1g00KvQ-faEJGD1bZV66aEnVXbQpWrU3BklA367HeOwfzudj55SJ5f76eT6-ekSBnpE4GN0hVNMcOUVwYzSgzKKloYTAjPBDcZJxqjnGla5LQkVIsymnlVYY0UHYOL4Wzn3cdGh16um1Bqa1Wr3SZIglPEsxxHMR_E0rsQvDay881a-a3ESO6qk39VF6PnQ3QZeuf3uRQxITjjkScDb0KvP_dc-ZXkGc2YfHyay7cXMkMz_C5vos8Hv_CNtXLpNr6NFf3_yA_wS4n_</recordid><startdate>2010</startdate><enddate>2010</enddate><creator>Goutte, S</creator><creator>Kime, N.M</creator><creator>Argo IV, T.F</creator><creator>Ryan, M.J</creator><general>Brill</general><general>BRILL</general><scope>BSCLL</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>F1W</scope><scope>H95</scope><scope>L.G</scope></search><sort><creationdate>2010</creationdate><title>Calling strategies of male túngara frogs in response to dynamic playback</title><author>Goutte, S ; Kime, N.M ; Argo IV, T.F ; Ryan, M.J</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-b452t-91faed3415136df1532f07d3bf1226796f762e1085e3b83c23e9c36d8dd1e0a3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2010</creationdate><topic>ACOUSTIC BEHAVIOUR</topic><topic>Amphibians</topic><topic>ANIMAL COMMUNICATION</topic><topic>Animals</topic><topic>Anura</topic><topic>Broadcasting industry</topic><topic>Capital costs</topic><topic>DYNAMIC PLAYBACK</topic><topic>Entropy</topic><topic>EVOKED VOCAL RESPONSE</topic><topic>Freshwater</topic><topic>FROG CALLS</topic><topic>Frogs</topic><topic>INTERACTIVE PLAYBACK</topic><topic>Male animals</topic><topic>Modeling</topic><topic>Null hypothesis</topic><topic>TÚNGARA FROG</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Goutte, S</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kime, N.M</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Argo IV, T.F</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Ryan, M.J</creatorcontrib><collection>Istex</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>ASFA: Aquatic Sciences and Fisheries Abstracts</collection><collection>Aquatic Science & Fisheries Abstracts (ASFA) 1: Biological Sciences & Living Resources</collection><collection>Aquatic Science & Fisheries Abstracts (ASFA) Professional</collection><jtitle>Behaviour</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Goutte, S</au><au>Kime, N.M</au><au>Argo IV, T.F</au><au>Ryan, M.J</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Calling strategies of male túngara frogs in response to dynamic playback</atitle><jtitle>Behaviour</jtitle><addtitle>BEH</addtitle><date>2010</date><risdate>2010</risdate><volume>147</volume><issue>1</issue><spage>65</spage><epage>83</epage><pages>65-83</pages><issn>0005-7959</issn><eissn>1568-539X</eissn><abstract>AbstractTúngara frogs produce calls of varying complexity that consist of a whine followed by 0–7 chucks. In previous studies using static playbacks, males increase chuck number in response to calls with 1 versus 0 chucks but not in response to 2 or 3 versus 1 chuck. Here we use dynamic playback in which an automated interaction program counts the number of chucks present in a call and responds according to a specified calling strategy which determines the number of chucks in the response call. Males added progressively more chucks in response to the models' strategies of de-escalate, match or escalate; there was a significant difference in the focal males' responses to the de-escalate versus the escalate strategies. Similarly, males changed their calling strategy in response to the de-escalate strategy of the model. There was no evidence of change in calling patterns, as estimated by entropy, among treatments and between experimental and field data. Males produced significantly more chucks in these experiments than in the field, but in both contexts the mean chuck number is low and never approaches the maximum observed in nature. These data suggest that males are cost-sensitive when it comes to adding chucks and that they are more influenced by vocal competitors that de-escalate rather than escalate number of chucks. These are patterns of calling not previously revealed in studies using static playbacks, and this study is another demonstration of the usefulness of dynamic playback in studies of animal communication.</abstract><cop>The Netherlands</cop><pub>Brill</pub><doi>10.1163/000579509X12483520922205</doi><tpages>19</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0005-7959 |
ispartof | Behaviour, 2010, Vol.147 (1), p.65-83 |
issn | 0005-7959 1568-539X |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_21406781 |
source | Jstor Complete Legacy |
subjects | ACOUSTIC BEHAVIOUR Amphibians ANIMAL COMMUNICATION Animals Anura Broadcasting industry Capital costs DYNAMIC PLAYBACK Entropy EVOKED VOCAL RESPONSE Freshwater FROG CALLS Frogs INTERACTIVE PLAYBACK Male animals Modeling Null hypothesis TÚNGARA FROG |
title | Calling strategies of male túngara frogs in response to dynamic playback |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-14T04%3A18%3A03IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-jstor_proqu&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Calling%20strategies%20of%20male%20t%C3%BAngara%20frogs%20in%20response%20to%20dynamic%20playback&rft.jtitle=Behaviour&rft.au=Goutte,%20S&rft.date=2010&rft.volume=147&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=65&rft.epage=83&rft.pages=65-83&rft.issn=0005-7959&rft.eissn=1568-539X&rft_id=info:doi/10.1163/000579509X12483520922205&rft_dat=%3Cjstor_proqu%3E40599656%3C/jstor_proqu%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=21406781&rft_id=info:pmid/&rft_jstor_id=40599656&rfr_iscdi=true |