Art expertise in construing meaning of representational and abstract artworks
Aesthetic appraisal of artwork can present the observer with visual problems to solve in the process of grasping its meaning and ‘visual rightness’ (i.e. “good” structure; Locher, 2003), with an elaboration on perceptual, semantic and affective dimensions (e.g. Marković, 2011). Thus observer's...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Acta psychologica 2019-01, Vol.192, p.11-22 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 22 |
---|---|
container_issue | |
container_start_page | 11 |
container_title | Acta psychologica |
container_volume | 192 |
creator | Bimler, David L. Snellock, Megan Paramei, Galina V. |
description | Aesthetic appraisal of artwork can present the observer with visual problems to solve in the process of grasping its meaning and ‘visual rightness’ (i.e. “good” structure; Locher, 2003), with an elaboration on perceptual, semantic and affective dimensions (e.g. Marković, 2011). Thus observer's expertise is a factor in aesthetic appraisal. To examine the influence of art training on the aesthetic response, and to clarify the nature of the Representational/Abstract distinction, 30 Experts and 33 Non-experts (Art and Psychology students, respectively) were asked to rate 24 paintings on six affective and affective-evaluative semantic differential scales. Stimuli were images of paintings from the period 1900–1935, 12 broadly Representational and 12 broadly Abstract. Relative to Non-experts, Experts rated Abstract artworks as more Interesting, Beautiful, Informative and Sophisticated, distinguishing them less markedly from Representational artworks. Aggregate Expert and Non-expert ratings, processed by factor analysis, resulted in a two-factor solution. The first factor, contrasting Abstract and Representational artworks, appeared more salient for Non-experts. The second factor, Cool–Warm, separating vibrantly-colored paintings from those with a blue-dominated/dull palette, was more salient for Experts. While Non-experts exaggerated differences between Abstract and Representational paintings, Experts appraised these two types of art similarly, attending more to artwork collative properties. We conclude that appreciation of art by Experts involves ‘cognitive mastery’ (Leder, Belke, Oeberst, & Augustin, 2004), i.e. more complex, cues-based visual schemata which equip them with more sophisticated strategies for analysing collative properties and semantics of an artwork while parsing ‘visual rightness’ to unfold its visual meaning. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.10.012 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_2129532062</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><els_id>S0001691816303298</els_id><sourcerecordid>2190997970</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c390t-fd4612561c02ac7d3cdc1fa3f53f2d16d054afdd383aac9835cc5a168e3d12b33</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9kMtKLDEQQIMoOlf9A5EGN256TCX9ykYQUe8FxY2uQyaplowzSZukffy9aUZduLiroopTr0PIEdA5UGjOlnOl0xA_5oxCl0tzCmyLzKBredkw0W6TGaUUykZAt0f-xLjMaQUCdskep1zQisGM3F2EVOD7gCHZiIV1hfYupjBa91SsUbkp-r4IOASM6JJK1ju1KpQzhVpkMl9RqJDefHiOB2SnV6uIh19xnzxeXz1c_i1v72_-XV7cljrvTWVvqgZY3YCmTOnWcG009Ir3Ne-ZgcbQulK9MbzjSmnR8VrrWkHTITfAFpzvk9PN3CH4lxFjkmsbNa5WyqEfo2TARM0ZbVhGT36hSz-G_MFECSpEK1qaqWpD6eBjDNjLIdi1Ch8SqJx0y6Xc6JaT7qmadee246_h42KN5qfp228GzjcAZhuvFoOM2qLTaGxAnaTx9v8bPgH51JNf</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2190997970</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Art expertise in construing meaning of representational and abstract artworks</title><source>Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals Complete</source><source>Periodicals Index Online</source><creator>Bimler, David L. ; Snellock, Megan ; Paramei, Galina V.</creator><creatorcontrib>Bimler, David L. ; Snellock, Megan ; Paramei, Galina V.</creatorcontrib><description>Aesthetic appraisal of artwork can present the observer with visual problems to solve in the process of grasping its meaning and ‘visual rightness’ (i.e. “good” structure; Locher, 2003), with an elaboration on perceptual, semantic and affective dimensions (e.g. Marković, 2011). Thus observer's expertise is a factor in aesthetic appraisal. To examine the influence of art training on the aesthetic response, and to clarify the nature of the Representational/Abstract distinction, 30 Experts and 33 Non-experts (Art and Psychology students, respectively) were asked to rate 24 paintings on six affective and affective-evaluative semantic differential scales. Stimuli were images of paintings from the period 1900–1935, 12 broadly Representational and 12 broadly Abstract. Relative to Non-experts, Experts rated Abstract artworks as more Interesting, Beautiful, Informative and Sophisticated, distinguishing them less markedly from Representational artworks. Aggregate Expert and Non-expert ratings, processed by factor analysis, resulted in a two-factor solution. The first factor, contrasting Abstract and Representational artworks, appeared more salient for Non-experts. The second factor, Cool–Warm, separating vibrantly-colored paintings from those with a blue-dominated/dull palette, was more salient for Experts. While Non-experts exaggerated differences between Abstract and Representational paintings, Experts appraised these two types of art similarly, attending more to artwork collative properties. We conclude that appreciation of art by Experts involves ‘cognitive mastery’ (Leder, Belke, Oeberst, & Augustin, 2004), i.e. more complex, cues-based visual schemata which equip them with more sophisticated strategies for analysing collative properties and semantics of an artwork while parsing ‘visual rightness’ to unfold its visual meaning.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0001-6918</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1873-6297</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.10.012</identifier><identifier>PMID: 30390421</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Netherlands: Elsevier B.V</publisher><subject>Abstract/Representational ; Aesthetic appraisal ; Art expertise ; Cognitive ability ; Cognitive mastery ; Experts ; Factor analysis ; Semantic differential ; Semantics ; Visual stimuli</subject><ispartof>Acta psychologica, 2019-01, Vol.192, p.11-22</ispartof><rights>2018 Elsevier B.V.</rights><rights>Copyright © 2018. Published by Elsevier B.V.</rights><rights>Copyright Elsevier Science Ltd. Jan 2019</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c390t-fd4612561c02ac7d3cdc1fa3f53f2d16d054afdd383aac9835cc5a168e3d12b33</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c390t-fd4612561c02ac7d3cdc1fa3f53f2d16d054afdd383aac9835cc5a168e3d12b33</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.10.012$$EHTML$$P50$$Gelsevier$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>315,782,786,3552,27876,27931,27932,46002</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30390421$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Bimler, David L.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Snellock, Megan</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Paramei, Galina V.</creatorcontrib><title>Art expertise in construing meaning of representational and abstract artworks</title><title>Acta psychologica</title><addtitle>Acta Psychol (Amst)</addtitle><description>Aesthetic appraisal of artwork can present the observer with visual problems to solve in the process of grasping its meaning and ‘visual rightness’ (i.e. “good” structure; Locher, 2003), with an elaboration on perceptual, semantic and affective dimensions (e.g. Marković, 2011). Thus observer's expertise is a factor in aesthetic appraisal. To examine the influence of art training on the aesthetic response, and to clarify the nature of the Representational/Abstract distinction, 30 Experts and 33 Non-experts (Art and Psychology students, respectively) were asked to rate 24 paintings on six affective and affective-evaluative semantic differential scales. Stimuli were images of paintings from the period 1900–1935, 12 broadly Representational and 12 broadly Abstract. Relative to Non-experts, Experts rated Abstract artworks as more Interesting, Beautiful, Informative and Sophisticated, distinguishing them less markedly from Representational artworks. Aggregate Expert and Non-expert ratings, processed by factor analysis, resulted in a two-factor solution. The first factor, contrasting Abstract and Representational artworks, appeared more salient for Non-experts. The second factor, Cool–Warm, separating vibrantly-colored paintings from those with a blue-dominated/dull palette, was more salient for Experts. While Non-experts exaggerated differences between Abstract and Representational paintings, Experts appraised these two types of art similarly, attending more to artwork collative properties. We conclude that appreciation of art by Experts involves ‘cognitive mastery’ (Leder, Belke, Oeberst, & Augustin, 2004), i.e. more complex, cues-based visual schemata which equip them with more sophisticated strategies for analysing collative properties and semantics of an artwork while parsing ‘visual rightness’ to unfold its visual meaning.</description><subject>Abstract/Representational</subject><subject>Aesthetic appraisal</subject><subject>Art expertise</subject><subject>Cognitive ability</subject><subject>Cognitive mastery</subject><subject>Experts</subject><subject>Factor analysis</subject><subject>Semantic differential</subject><subject>Semantics</subject><subject>Visual stimuli</subject><issn>0001-6918</issn><issn>1873-6297</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2019</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>K30</sourceid><recordid>eNp9kMtKLDEQQIMoOlf9A5EGN256TCX9ykYQUe8FxY2uQyaplowzSZukffy9aUZduLiroopTr0PIEdA5UGjOlnOl0xA_5oxCl0tzCmyLzKBredkw0W6TGaUUykZAt0f-xLjMaQUCdskep1zQisGM3F2EVOD7gCHZiIV1hfYupjBa91SsUbkp-r4IOASM6JJK1ju1KpQzhVpkMl9RqJDefHiOB2SnV6uIh19xnzxeXz1c_i1v72_-XV7cljrvTWVvqgZY3YCmTOnWcG009Ir3Ne-ZgcbQulK9MbzjSmnR8VrrWkHTITfAFpzvk9PN3CH4lxFjkmsbNa5WyqEfo2TARM0ZbVhGT36hSz-G_MFECSpEK1qaqWpD6eBjDNjLIdi1Ch8SqJx0y6Xc6JaT7qmadee246_h42KN5qfp228GzjcAZhuvFoOM2qLTaGxAnaTx9v8bPgH51JNf</recordid><startdate>20190101</startdate><enddate>20190101</enddate><creator>Bimler, David L.</creator><creator>Snellock, Megan</creator><creator>Paramei, Galina V.</creator><general>Elsevier B.V</general><general>Elsevier Science Ltd</general><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>ICWRT</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20190101</creationdate><title>Art expertise in construing meaning of representational and abstract artworks</title><author>Bimler, David L. ; Snellock, Megan ; Paramei, Galina V.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c390t-fd4612561c02ac7d3cdc1fa3f53f2d16d054afdd383aac9835cc5a168e3d12b33</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2019</creationdate><topic>Abstract/Representational</topic><topic>Aesthetic appraisal</topic><topic>Art expertise</topic><topic>Cognitive ability</topic><topic>Cognitive mastery</topic><topic>Experts</topic><topic>Factor analysis</topic><topic>Semantic differential</topic><topic>Semantics</topic><topic>Visual stimuli</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Bimler, David L.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Snellock, Megan</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Paramei, Galina V.</creatorcontrib><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 28</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Acta psychologica</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Bimler, David L.</au><au>Snellock, Megan</au><au>Paramei, Galina V.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Art expertise in construing meaning of representational and abstract artworks</atitle><jtitle>Acta psychologica</jtitle><addtitle>Acta Psychol (Amst)</addtitle><date>2019-01-01</date><risdate>2019</risdate><volume>192</volume><spage>11</spage><epage>22</epage><pages>11-22</pages><issn>0001-6918</issn><eissn>1873-6297</eissn><abstract>Aesthetic appraisal of artwork can present the observer with visual problems to solve in the process of grasping its meaning and ‘visual rightness’ (i.e. “good” structure; Locher, 2003), with an elaboration on perceptual, semantic and affective dimensions (e.g. Marković, 2011). Thus observer's expertise is a factor in aesthetic appraisal. To examine the influence of art training on the aesthetic response, and to clarify the nature of the Representational/Abstract distinction, 30 Experts and 33 Non-experts (Art and Psychology students, respectively) were asked to rate 24 paintings on six affective and affective-evaluative semantic differential scales. Stimuli were images of paintings from the period 1900–1935, 12 broadly Representational and 12 broadly Abstract. Relative to Non-experts, Experts rated Abstract artworks as more Interesting, Beautiful, Informative and Sophisticated, distinguishing them less markedly from Representational artworks. Aggregate Expert and Non-expert ratings, processed by factor analysis, resulted in a two-factor solution. The first factor, contrasting Abstract and Representational artworks, appeared more salient for Non-experts. The second factor, Cool–Warm, separating vibrantly-colored paintings from those with a blue-dominated/dull palette, was more salient for Experts. While Non-experts exaggerated differences between Abstract and Representational paintings, Experts appraised these two types of art similarly, attending more to artwork collative properties. We conclude that appreciation of art by Experts involves ‘cognitive mastery’ (Leder, Belke, Oeberst, & Augustin, 2004), i.e. more complex, cues-based visual schemata which equip them with more sophisticated strategies for analysing collative properties and semantics of an artwork while parsing ‘visual rightness’ to unfold its visual meaning.</abstract><cop>Netherlands</cop><pub>Elsevier B.V</pub><pmid>30390421</pmid><doi>10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.10.012</doi><tpages>12</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0001-6918 |
ispartof | Acta psychologica, 2019-01, Vol.192, p.11-22 |
issn | 0001-6918 1873-6297 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_2129532062 |
source | Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals Complete; Periodicals Index Online |
subjects | Abstract/Representational Aesthetic appraisal Art expertise Cognitive ability Cognitive mastery Experts Factor analysis Semantic differential Semantics Visual stimuli |
title | Art expertise in construing meaning of representational and abstract artworks |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-05T00%3A48%3A15IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Art%20expertise%20in%20construing%20meaning%20of%20representational%20and%20abstract%20artworks&rft.jtitle=Acta%20psychologica&rft.au=Bimler,%20David%20L.&rft.date=2019-01-01&rft.volume=192&rft.spage=11&rft.epage=22&rft.pages=11-22&rft.issn=0001-6918&rft.eissn=1873-6297&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.10.012&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E2190997970%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2190997970&rft_id=info:pmid/30390421&rft_els_id=S0001691816303298&rfr_iscdi=true |