Language Experience in the Second Year of Life and Language Outcomes in Late Childhood
Quantity of talk and interaction in the home during early childhood is correlated with socioeconomic status (SES) and can be used to predict early language and cognitive outcomes. We tested the effectiveness of automated early language environment estimates for children 2 to 36 months old to predict...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Pediatrics (Evanston) 2018-10, Vol.142 (4), p.e20174276 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , , , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | |
---|---|
container_issue | 4 |
container_start_page | e20174276 |
container_title | Pediatrics (Evanston) |
container_volume | 142 |
creator | Gilkerson, Jill Richards, Jeffrey A Warren, Steven F Oller, D Kimbrough Russo, Rosemary Vohr, Betty |
description | Quantity of talk and interaction in the home during early childhood is correlated with socioeconomic status (SES) and can be used to predict early language and cognitive outcomes. We tested the effectiveness of automated early language environment estimates for children 2 to 36 months old to predict cognitive and language skills 10 years later and examined effects for specific developmental age periods.
Daylong audio recordings for 146 infants and toddlers were completed monthly for 6 months, and the total number of daily adult words and adult-child conversational turnswere automatically estimated with Language Environment Analysis software. Follow-up evaluations at 9 to 14 years of age included language and cognitive testing. Language exposure for 3 age groups was assessed: 2 to 17 months, 18 to 24 months, and ≥25 months. Pearson correlations and multiple linear regression analyses were conducted.
Conversational turn counts at 18 to 24 months of age accounted for 14% to 27% of the variance in IQ, verbal comprehension, and receptive and/or expressive vocabulary scores 10 years later after controlling for SES. Adult word counts between 18 and 24 months were correlated with language outcomes but were considerably weakened after controlling for SES.
These data support the hypothesis that early talk and interaction, particularly during the relatively narrow developmental window of 18 to 24 months of age, can be used to predict school-age language and cognitive outcomes. With these findings, we underscore the need for effective early intervention programs that support parents in creating an optimal early language learning environment in the home. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1542/peds.2017-4276 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>gale_proqu</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_2102339332</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><galeid>A561217486</galeid><sourcerecordid>A561217486</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c401t-4c84bdfc1ce39b709ab997a8e52dcf6c8ced91a76d9f329a4323cddf5c9b1d1f3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNpdkUtr4zAURsXQYZJmZttlEXTTjTN62bKWJaQPMGTRB3QlZOkqcXCs1LKh8-9HJm0XXUmI84nv3oPQBSVLmgv29wguLhmhMhNMFj_QnBJVTvf8DM0J4TQThOQzdB7jnhAicsl-oRknKVIwMUcvlem2o9kCXr8foW-gs4CbDg87wI9gQ-fwK5geB4-rxgM26eErshkHGw4Qp0BlBsCrXdO6XQjuN_rpTRvhz8e5QM-366fVfVZt7h5WN1VmBaFDJmwpaucttcBVLYkytVLSlJAzZ31hSwtOUSMLpzxnygjOuHXO51bV1FHPF-j69O-xD28jxEEfmmihbU0HYYyaUcI4VzzlFujqG7oPY9-ldolKCymplGWishO1NS3opksbGOA9jdm2sAWdyq82-iYvKKNSlEXilyfe9iHGHrw-9s3B9P80JXoypCdDejKkJ0MpcPlRY6wP4L7wTyX8P5sNipw</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2130281778</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Language Experience in the Second Year of Life and Language Outcomes in Late Childhood</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>EZB-FREE-00999 freely available EZB journals</source><source>Alma/SFX Local Collection</source><creator>Gilkerson, Jill ; Richards, Jeffrey A ; Warren, Steven F ; Oller, D Kimbrough ; Russo, Rosemary ; Vohr, Betty</creator><creatorcontrib>Gilkerson, Jill ; Richards, Jeffrey A ; Warren, Steven F ; Oller, D Kimbrough ; Russo, Rosemary ; Vohr, Betty</creatorcontrib><description>Quantity of talk and interaction in the home during early childhood is correlated with socioeconomic status (SES) and can be used to predict early language and cognitive outcomes. We tested the effectiveness of automated early language environment estimates for children 2 to 36 months old to predict cognitive and language skills 10 years later and examined effects for specific developmental age periods.
Daylong audio recordings for 146 infants and toddlers were completed monthly for 6 months, and the total number of daily adult words and adult-child conversational turnswere automatically estimated with Language Environment Analysis software. Follow-up evaluations at 9 to 14 years of age included language and cognitive testing. Language exposure for 3 age groups was assessed: 2 to 17 months, 18 to 24 months, and ≥25 months. Pearson correlations and multiple linear regression analyses were conducted.
Conversational turn counts at 18 to 24 months of age accounted for 14% to 27% of the variance in IQ, verbal comprehension, and receptive and/or expressive vocabulary scores 10 years later after controlling for SES. Adult word counts between 18 and 24 months were correlated with language outcomes but were considerably weakened after controlling for SES.
These data support the hypothesis that early talk and interaction, particularly during the relatively narrow developmental window of 18 to 24 months of age, can be used to predict school-age language and cognitive outcomes. With these findings, we underscore the need for effective early intervention programs that support parents in creating an optimal early language learning environment in the home.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0031-4005</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1098-4275</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1542/peds.2017-4276</identifier><identifier>PMID: 30201624</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: American Academy of Pediatrics</publisher><subject>Adolescent ; Adult-child interactions ; Age ; Analysis ; Child ; Child, Preschool ; Children ; Children & youth ; Cognition ; Cognitive ability ; Cognitive development ; Comprehension ; Conversation ; Developmental stages ; Effectiveness ; Female ; Follow-Up Studies ; Humans ; Infant ; Infants ; Language ; Language acquisition ; Language Development ; Language Tests ; Language thought relationship ; Learning environment ; Linear models (Statistics) ; Linear regression models ; Male ; Native language acquisition ; Parent and child ; Parent-Child Relations ; Pediatrics ; Social Class ; Social classes ; Social Environment ; Socioeconomic status ; Socioeconomics ; Turn taking ; Wechsler Scales</subject><ispartof>Pediatrics (Evanston), 2018-10, Vol.142 (4), p.e20174276</ispartof><rights>Copyright © 2018 by the American Academy of Pediatrics.</rights><rights>Copyright American Academy of Pediatrics Oct 2018</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c401t-4c84bdfc1ce39b709ab997a8e52dcf6c8ced91a76d9f329a4323cddf5c9b1d1f3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c401t-4c84bdfc1ce39b709ab997a8e52dcf6c8ced91a76d9f329a4323cddf5c9b1d1f3</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,27924,27925</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30201624$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Gilkerson, Jill</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Richards, Jeffrey A</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Warren, Steven F</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Oller, D Kimbrough</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Russo, Rosemary</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Vohr, Betty</creatorcontrib><title>Language Experience in the Second Year of Life and Language Outcomes in Late Childhood</title><title>Pediatrics (Evanston)</title><addtitle>Pediatrics</addtitle><description>Quantity of talk and interaction in the home during early childhood is correlated with socioeconomic status (SES) and can be used to predict early language and cognitive outcomes. We tested the effectiveness of automated early language environment estimates for children 2 to 36 months old to predict cognitive and language skills 10 years later and examined effects for specific developmental age periods.
Daylong audio recordings for 146 infants and toddlers were completed monthly for 6 months, and the total number of daily adult words and adult-child conversational turnswere automatically estimated with Language Environment Analysis software. Follow-up evaluations at 9 to 14 years of age included language and cognitive testing. Language exposure for 3 age groups was assessed: 2 to 17 months, 18 to 24 months, and ≥25 months. Pearson correlations and multiple linear regression analyses were conducted.
Conversational turn counts at 18 to 24 months of age accounted for 14% to 27% of the variance in IQ, verbal comprehension, and receptive and/or expressive vocabulary scores 10 years later after controlling for SES. Adult word counts between 18 and 24 months were correlated with language outcomes but were considerably weakened after controlling for SES.
These data support the hypothesis that early talk and interaction, particularly during the relatively narrow developmental window of 18 to 24 months of age, can be used to predict school-age language and cognitive outcomes. With these findings, we underscore the need for effective early intervention programs that support parents in creating an optimal early language learning environment in the home.</description><subject>Adolescent</subject><subject>Adult-child interactions</subject><subject>Age</subject><subject>Analysis</subject><subject>Child</subject><subject>Child, Preschool</subject><subject>Children</subject><subject>Children & youth</subject><subject>Cognition</subject><subject>Cognitive ability</subject><subject>Cognitive development</subject><subject>Comprehension</subject><subject>Conversation</subject><subject>Developmental stages</subject><subject>Effectiveness</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Follow-Up Studies</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Infant</subject><subject>Infants</subject><subject>Language</subject><subject>Language acquisition</subject><subject>Language Development</subject><subject>Language Tests</subject><subject>Language thought relationship</subject><subject>Learning environment</subject><subject>Linear models (Statistics)</subject><subject>Linear regression models</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Native language acquisition</subject><subject>Parent and child</subject><subject>Parent-Child Relations</subject><subject>Pediatrics</subject><subject>Social Class</subject><subject>Social classes</subject><subject>Social Environment</subject><subject>Socioeconomic status</subject><subject>Socioeconomics</subject><subject>Turn taking</subject><subject>Wechsler Scales</subject><issn>0031-4005</issn><issn>1098-4275</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2018</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><recordid>eNpdkUtr4zAURsXQYZJmZttlEXTTjTN62bKWJaQPMGTRB3QlZOkqcXCs1LKh8-9HJm0XXUmI84nv3oPQBSVLmgv29wguLhmhMhNMFj_QnBJVTvf8DM0J4TQThOQzdB7jnhAicsl-oRknKVIwMUcvlem2o9kCXr8foW-gs4CbDg87wI9gQ-fwK5geB4-rxgM26eErshkHGw4Qp0BlBsCrXdO6XQjuN_rpTRvhz8e5QM-366fVfVZt7h5WN1VmBaFDJmwpaucttcBVLYkytVLSlJAzZ31hSwtOUSMLpzxnygjOuHXO51bV1FHPF-j69O-xD28jxEEfmmihbU0HYYyaUcI4VzzlFujqG7oPY9-ldolKCymplGWishO1NS3opksbGOA9jdm2sAWdyq82-iYvKKNSlEXilyfe9iHGHrw-9s3B9P80JXoypCdDejKkJ0MpcPlRY6wP4L7wTyX8P5sNipw</recordid><startdate>201810</startdate><enddate>201810</enddate><creator>Gilkerson, Jill</creator><creator>Richards, Jeffrey A</creator><creator>Warren, Steven F</creator><creator>Oller, D Kimbrough</creator><creator>Russo, Rosemary</creator><creator>Vohr, Betty</creator><general>American Academy of Pediatrics</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7T9</scope><scope>7TS</scope><scope>7U9</scope><scope>H94</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>M7N</scope><scope>NAPCQ</scope><scope>U9A</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>201810</creationdate><title>Language Experience in the Second Year of Life and Language Outcomes in Late Childhood</title><author>Gilkerson, Jill ; Richards, Jeffrey A ; Warren, Steven F ; Oller, D Kimbrough ; Russo, Rosemary ; Vohr, Betty</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c401t-4c84bdfc1ce39b709ab997a8e52dcf6c8ced91a76d9f329a4323cddf5c9b1d1f3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2018</creationdate><topic>Adolescent</topic><topic>Adult-child interactions</topic><topic>Age</topic><topic>Analysis</topic><topic>Child</topic><topic>Child, Preschool</topic><topic>Children</topic><topic>Children & youth</topic><topic>Cognition</topic><topic>Cognitive ability</topic><topic>Cognitive development</topic><topic>Comprehension</topic><topic>Conversation</topic><topic>Developmental stages</topic><topic>Effectiveness</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Follow-Up Studies</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Infant</topic><topic>Infants</topic><topic>Language</topic><topic>Language acquisition</topic><topic>Language Development</topic><topic>Language Tests</topic><topic>Language thought relationship</topic><topic>Learning environment</topic><topic>Linear models (Statistics)</topic><topic>Linear regression models</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Native language acquisition</topic><topic>Parent and child</topic><topic>Parent-Child Relations</topic><topic>Pediatrics</topic><topic>Social Class</topic><topic>Social classes</topic><topic>Social Environment</topic><topic>Socioeconomic status</topic><topic>Socioeconomics</topic><topic>Turn taking</topic><topic>Wechsler Scales</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Gilkerson, Jill</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Richards, Jeffrey A</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Warren, Steven F</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Oller, D Kimbrough</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Russo, Rosemary</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Vohr, Betty</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)</collection><collection>Physical Education Index</collection><collection>Virology and AIDS Abstracts</collection><collection>AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C)</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Premium</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Pediatrics (Evanston)</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Gilkerson, Jill</au><au>Richards, Jeffrey A</au><au>Warren, Steven F</au><au>Oller, D Kimbrough</au><au>Russo, Rosemary</au><au>Vohr, Betty</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Language Experience in the Second Year of Life and Language Outcomes in Late Childhood</atitle><jtitle>Pediatrics (Evanston)</jtitle><addtitle>Pediatrics</addtitle><date>2018-10</date><risdate>2018</risdate><volume>142</volume><issue>4</issue><spage>e20174276</spage><pages>e20174276-</pages><issn>0031-4005</issn><eissn>1098-4275</eissn><abstract>Quantity of talk and interaction in the home during early childhood is correlated with socioeconomic status (SES) and can be used to predict early language and cognitive outcomes. We tested the effectiveness of automated early language environment estimates for children 2 to 36 months old to predict cognitive and language skills 10 years later and examined effects for specific developmental age periods.
Daylong audio recordings for 146 infants and toddlers were completed monthly for 6 months, and the total number of daily adult words and adult-child conversational turnswere automatically estimated with Language Environment Analysis software. Follow-up evaluations at 9 to 14 years of age included language and cognitive testing. Language exposure for 3 age groups was assessed: 2 to 17 months, 18 to 24 months, and ≥25 months. Pearson correlations and multiple linear regression analyses were conducted.
Conversational turn counts at 18 to 24 months of age accounted for 14% to 27% of the variance in IQ, verbal comprehension, and receptive and/or expressive vocabulary scores 10 years later after controlling for SES. Adult word counts between 18 and 24 months were correlated with language outcomes but were considerably weakened after controlling for SES.
These data support the hypothesis that early talk and interaction, particularly during the relatively narrow developmental window of 18 to 24 months of age, can be used to predict school-age language and cognitive outcomes. With these findings, we underscore the need for effective early intervention programs that support parents in creating an optimal early language learning environment in the home.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>American Academy of Pediatrics</pub><pmid>30201624</pmid><doi>10.1542/peds.2017-4276</doi><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0031-4005 |
ispartof | Pediatrics (Evanston), 2018-10, Vol.142 (4), p.e20174276 |
issn | 0031-4005 1098-4275 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_2102339332 |
source | MEDLINE; EZB-FREE-00999 freely available EZB journals; Alma/SFX Local Collection |
subjects | Adolescent Adult-child interactions Age Analysis Child Child, Preschool Children Children & youth Cognition Cognitive ability Cognitive development Comprehension Conversation Developmental stages Effectiveness Female Follow-Up Studies Humans Infant Infants Language Language acquisition Language Development Language Tests Language thought relationship Learning environment Linear models (Statistics) Linear regression models Male Native language acquisition Parent and child Parent-Child Relations Pediatrics Social Class Social classes Social Environment Socioeconomic status Socioeconomics Turn taking Wechsler Scales |
title | Language Experience in the Second Year of Life and Language Outcomes in Late Childhood |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-29T13%3A25%3A14IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-gale_proqu&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Language%20Experience%20in%20the%20Second%20Year%20of%20Life%20and%20Language%20Outcomes%20in%20Late%20Childhood&rft.jtitle=Pediatrics%20(Evanston)&rft.au=Gilkerson,%20Jill&rft.date=2018-10&rft.volume=142&rft.issue=4&rft.spage=e20174276&rft.pages=e20174276-&rft.issn=0031-4005&rft.eissn=1098-4275&rft_id=info:doi/10.1542/peds.2017-4276&rft_dat=%3Cgale_proqu%3EA561217486%3C/gale_proqu%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2130281778&rft_id=info:pmid/30201624&rft_galeid=A561217486&rfr_iscdi=true |