Language Experience in the Second Year of Life and Language Outcomes in Late Childhood

Quantity of talk and interaction in the home during early childhood is correlated with socioeconomic status (SES) and can be used to predict early language and cognitive outcomes. We tested the effectiveness of automated early language environment estimates for children 2 to 36 months old to predict...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Pediatrics (Evanston) 2018-10, Vol.142 (4), p.e20174276
Hauptverfasser: Gilkerson, Jill, Richards, Jeffrey A, Warren, Steven F, Oller, D Kimbrough, Russo, Rosemary, Vohr, Betty
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page
container_issue 4
container_start_page e20174276
container_title Pediatrics (Evanston)
container_volume 142
creator Gilkerson, Jill
Richards, Jeffrey A
Warren, Steven F
Oller, D Kimbrough
Russo, Rosemary
Vohr, Betty
description Quantity of talk and interaction in the home during early childhood is correlated with socioeconomic status (SES) and can be used to predict early language and cognitive outcomes. We tested the effectiveness of automated early language environment estimates for children 2 to 36 months old to predict cognitive and language skills 10 years later and examined effects for specific developmental age periods. Daylong audio recordings for 146 infants and toddlers were completed monthly for 6 months, and the total number of daily adult words and adult-child conversational turnswere automatically estimated with Language Environment Analysis software. Follow-up evaluations at 9 to 14 years of age included language and cognitive testing. Language exposure for 3 age groups was assessed: 2 to 17 months, 18 to 24 months, and ≥25 months. Pearson correlations and multiple linear regression analyses were conducted. Conversational turn counts at 18 to 24 months of age accounted for 14% to 27% of the variance in IQ, verbal comprehension, and receptive and/or expressive vocabulary scores 10 years later after controlling for SES. Adult word counts between 18 and 24 months were correlated with language outcomes but were considerably weakened after controlling for SES. These data support the hypothesis that early talk and interaction, particularly during the relatively narrow developmental window of 18 to 24 months of age, can be used to predict school-age language and cognitive outcomes. With these findings, we underscore the need for effective early intervention programs that support parents in creating an optimal early language learning environment in the home.
doi_str_mv 10.1542/peds.2017-4276
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>gale_proqu</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_2102339332</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><galeid>A561217486</galeid><sourcerecordid>A561217486</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c401t-4c84bdfc1ce39b709ab997a8e52dcf6c8ced91a76d9f329a4323cddf5c9b1d1f3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNpdkUtr4zAURsXQYZJmZttlEXTTjTN62bKWJaQPMGTRB3QlZOkqcXCs1LKh8-9HJm0XXUmI84nv3oPQBSVLmgv29wguLhmhMhNMFj_QnBJVTvf8DM0J4TQThOQzdB7jnhAicsl-oRknKVIwMUcvlem2o9kCXr8foW-gs4CbDg87wI9gQ-fwK5geB4-rxgM26eErshkHGw4Qp0BlBsCrXdO6XQjuN_rpTRvhz8e5QM-366fVfVZt7h5WN1VmBaFDJmwpaucttcBVLYkytVLSlJAzZ31hSwtOUSMLpzxnygjOuHXO51bV1FHPF-j69O-xD28jxEEfmmihbU0HYYyaUcI4VzzlFujqG7oPY9-ldolKCymplGWishO1NS3opksbGOA9jdm2sAWdyq82-iYvKKNSlEXilyfe9iHGHrw-9s3B9P80JXoypCdDejKkJ0MpcPlRY6wP4L7wTyX8P5sNipw</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2130281778</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Language Experience in the Second Year of Life and Language Outcomes in Late Childhood</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>EZB-FREE-00999 freely available EZB journals</source><source>Alma/SFX Local Collection</source><creator>Gilkerson, Jill ; Richards, Jeffrey A ; Warren, Steven F ; Oller, D Kimbrough ; Russo, Rosemary ; Vohr, Betty</creator><creatorcontrib>Gilkerson, Jill ; Richards, Jeffrey A ; Warren, Steven F ; Oller, D Kimbrough ; Russo, Rosemary ; Vohr, Betty</creatorcontrib><description>Quantity of talk and interaction in the home during early childhood is correlated with socioeconomic status (SES) and can be used to predict early language and cognitive outcomes. We tested the effectiveness of automated early language environment estimates for children 2 to 36 months old to predict cognitive and language skills 10 years later and examined effects for specific developmental age periods. Daylong audio recordings for 146 infants and toddlers were completed monthly for 6 months, and the total number of daily adult words and adult-child conversational turnswere automatically estimated with Language Environment Analysis software. Follow-up evaluations at 9 to 14 years of age included language and cognitive testing. Language exposure for 3 age groups was assessed: 2 to 17 months, 18 to 24 months, and ≥25 months. Pearson correlations and multiple linear regression analyses were conducted. Conversational turn counts at 18 to 24 months of age accounted for 14% to 27% of the variance in IQ, verbal comprehension, and receptive and/or expressive vocabulary scores 10 years later after controlling for SES. Adult word counts between 18 and 24 months were correlated with language outcomes but were considerably weakened after controlling for SES. These data support the hypothesis that early talk and interaction, particularly during the relatively narrow developmental window of 18 to 24 months of age, can be used to predict school-age language and cognitive outcomes. With these findings, we underscore the need for effective early intervention programs that support parents in creating an optimal early language learning environment in the home.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0031-4005</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1098-4275</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1542/peds.2017-4276</identifier><identifier>PMID: 30201624</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: American Academy of Pediatrics</publisher><subject>Adolescent ; Adult-child interactions ; Age ; Analysis ; Child ; Child, Preschool ; Children ; Children &amp; youth ; Cognition ; Cognitive ability ; Cognitive development ; Comprehension ; Conversation ; Developmental stages ; Effectiveness ; Female ; Follow-Up Studies ; Humans ; Infant ; Infants ; Language ; Language acquisition ; Language Development ; Language Tests ; Language thought relationship ; Learning environment ; Linear models (Statistics) ; Linear regression models ; Male ; Native language acquisition ; Parent and child ; Parent-Child Relations ; Pediatrics ; Social Class ; Social classes ; Social Environment ; Socioeconomic status ; Socioeconomics ; Turn taking ; Wechsler Scales</subject><ispartof>Pediatrics (Evanston), 2018-10, Vol.142 (4), p.e20174276</ispartof><rights>Copyright © 2018 by the American Academy of Pediatrics.</rights><rights>Copyright American Academy of Pediatrics Oct 2018</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c401t-4c84bdfc1ce39b709ab997a8e52dcf6c8ced91a76d9f329a4323cddf5c9b1d1f3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c401t-4c84bdfc1ce39b709ab997a8e52dcf6c8ced91a76d9f329a4323cddf5c9b1d1f3</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,27924,27925</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30201624$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Gilkerson, Jill</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Richards, Jeffrey A</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Warren, Steven F</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Oller, D Kimbrough</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Russo, Rosemary</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Vohr, Betty</creatorcontrib><title>Language Experience in the Second Year of Life and Language Outcomes in Late Childhood</title><title>Pediatrics (Evanston)</title><addtitle>Pediatrics</addtitle><description>Quantity of talk and interaction in the home during early childhood is correlated with socioeconomic status (SES) and can be used to predict early language and cognitive outcomes. We tested the effectiveness of automated early language environment estimates for children 2 to 36 months old to predict cognitive and language skills 10 years later and examined effects for specific developmental age periods. Daylong audio recordings for 146 infants and toddlers were completed monthly for 6 months, and the total number of daily adult words and adult-child conversational turnswere automatically estimated with Language Environment Analysis software. Follow-up evaluations at 9 to 14 years of age included language and cognitive testing. Language exposure for 3 age groups was assessed: 2 to 17 months, 18 to 24 months, and ≥25 months. Pearson correlations and multiple linear regression analyses were conducted. Conversational turn counts at 18 to 24 months of age accounted for 14% to 27% of the variance in IQ, verbal comprehension, and receptive and/or expressive vocabulary scores 10 years later after controlling for SES. Adult word counts between 18 and 24 months were correlated with language outcomes but were considerably weakened after controlling for SES. These data support the hypothesis that early talk and interaction, particularly during the relatively narrow developmental window of 18 to 24 months of age, can be used to predict school-age language and cognitive outcomes. With these findings, we underscore the need for effective early intervention programs that support parents in creating an optimal early language learning environment in the home.</description><subject>Adolescent</subject><subject>Adult-child interactions</subject><subject>Age</subject><subject>Analysis</subject><subject>Child</subject><subject>Child, Preschool</subject><subject>Children</subject><subject>Children &amp; youth</subject><subject>Cognition</subject><subject>Cognitive ability</subject><subject>Cognitive development</subject><subject>Comprehension</subject><subject>Conversation</subject><subject>Developmental stages</subject><subject>Effectiveness</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Follow-Up Studies</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Infant</subject><subject>Infants</subject><subject>Language</subject><subject>Language acquisition</subject><subject>Language Development</subject><subject>Language Tests</subject><subject>Language thought relationship</subject><subject>Learning environment</subject><subject>Linear models (Statistics)</subject><subject>Linear regression models</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Native language acquisition</subject><subject>Parent and child</subject><subject>Parent-Child Relations</subject><subject>Pediatrics</subject><subject>Social Class</subject><subject>Social classes</subject><subject>Social Environment</subject><subject>Socioeconomic status</subject><subject>Socioeconomics</subject><subject>Turn taking</subject><subject>Wechsler Scales</subject><issn>0031-4005</issn><issn>1098-4275</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2018</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><recordid>eNpdkUtr4zAURsXQYZJmZttlEXTTjTN62bKWJaQPMGTRB3QlZOkqcXCs1LKh8-9HJm0XXUmI84nv3oPQBSVLmgv29wguLhmhMhNMFj_QnBJVTvf8DM0J4TQThOQzdB7jnhAicsl-oRknKVIwMUcvlem2o9kCXr8foW-gs4CbDg87wI9gQ-fwK5geB4-rxgM26eErshkHGw4Qp0BlBsCrXdO6XQjuN_rpTRvhz8e5QM-366fVfVZt7h5WN1VmBaFDJmwpaucttcBVLYkytVLSlJAzZ31hSwtOUSMLpzxnygjOuHXO51bV1FHPF-j69O-xD28jxEEfmmihbU0HYYyaUcI4VzzlFujqG7oPY9-ldolKCymplGWishO1NS3opksbGOA9jdm2sAWdyq82-iYvKKNSlEXilyfe9iHGHrw-9s3B9P80JXoypCdDejKkJ0MpcPlRY6wP4L7wTyX8P5sNipw</recordid><startdate>201810</startdate><enddate>201810</enddate><creator>Gilkerson, Jill</creator><creator>Richards, Jeffrey A</creator><creator>Warren, Steven F</creator><creator>Oller, D Kimbrough</creator><creator>Russo, Rosemary</creator><creator>Vohr, Betty</creator><general>American Academy of Pediatrics</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7T9</scope><scope>7TS</scope><scope>7U9</scope><scope>H94</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>M7N</scope><scope>NAPCQ</scope><scope>U9A</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>201810</creationdate><title>Language Experience in the Second Year of Life and Language Outcomes in Late Childhood</title><author>Gilkerson, Jill ; Richards, Jeffrey A ; Warren, Steven F ; Oller, D Kimbrough ; Russo, Rosemary ; Vohr, Betty</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c401t-4c84bdfc1ce39b709ab997a8e52dcf6c8ced91a76d9f329a4323cddf5c9b1d1f3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2018</creationdate><topic>Adolescent</topic><topic>Adult-child interactions</topic><topic>Age</topic><topic>Analysis</topic><topic>Child</topic><topic>Child, Preschool</topic><topic>Children</topic><topic>Children &amp; youth</topic><topic>Cognition</topic><topic>Cognitive ability</topic><topic>Cognitive development</topic><topic>Comprehension</topic><topic>Conversation</topic><topic>Developmental stages</topic><topic>Effectiveness</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Follow-Up Studies</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Infant</topic><topic>Infants</topic><topic>Language</topic><topic>Language acquisition</topic><topic>Language Development</topic><topic>Language Tests</topic><topic>Language thought relationship</topic><topic>Learning environment</topic><topic>Linear models (Statistics)</topic><topic>Linear regression models</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Native language acquisition</topic><topic>Parent and child</topic><topic>Parent-Child Relations</topic><topic>Pediatrics</topic><topic>Social Class</topic><topic>Social classes</topic><topic>Social Environment</topic><topic>Socioeconomic status</topic><topic>Socioeconomics</topic><topic>Turn taking</topic><topic>Wechsler Scales</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Gilkerson, Jill</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Richards, Jeffrey A</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Warren, Steven F</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Oller, D Kimbrough</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Russo, Rosemary</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Vohr, Betty</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)</collection><collection>Physical Education Index</collection><collection>Virology and AIDS Abstracts</collection><collection>AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts</collection><collection>ProQuest Health &amp; Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C)</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Premium</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Pediatrics (Evanston)</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Gilkerson, Jill</au><au>Richards, Jeffrey A</au><au>Warren, Steven F</au><au>Oller, D Kimbrough</au><au>Russo, Rosemary</au><au>Vohr, Betty</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Language Experience in the Second Year of Life and Language Outcomes in Late Childhood</atitle><jtitle>Pediatrics (Evanston)</jtitle><addtitle>Pediatrics</addtitle><date>2018-10</date><risdate>2018</risdate><volume>142</volume><issue>4</issue><spage>e20174276</spage><pages>e20174276-</pages><issn>0031-4005</issn><eissn>1098-4275</eissn><abstract>Quantity of talk and interaction in the home during early childhood is correlated with socioeconomic status (SES) and can be used to predict early language and cognitive outcomes. We tested the effectiveness of automated early language environment estimates for children 2 to 36 months old to predict cognitive and language skills 10 years later and examined effects for specific developmental age periods. Daylong audio recordings for 146 infants and toddlers were completed monthly for 6 months, and the total number of daily adult words and adult-child conversational turnswere automatically estimated with Language Environment Analysis software. Follow-up evaluations at 9 to 14 years of age included language and cognitive testing. Language exposure for 3 age groups was assessed: 2 to 17 months, 18 to 24 months, and ≥25 months. Pearson correlations and multiple linear regression analyses were conducted. Conversational turn counts at 18 to 24 months of age accounted for 14% to 27% of the variance in IQ, verbal comprehension, and receptive and/or expressive vocabulary scores 10 years later after controlling for SES. Adult word counts between 18 and 24 months were correlated with language outcomes but were considerably weakened after controlling for SES. These data support the hypothesis that early talk and interaction, particularly during the relatively narrow developmental window of 18 to 24 months of age, can be used to predict school-age language and cognitive outcomes. With these findings, we underscore the need for effective early intervention programs that support parents in creating an optimal early language learning environment in the home.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>American Academy of Pediatrics</pub><pmid>30201624</pmid><doi>10.1542/peds.2017-4276</doi><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0031-4005
ispartof Pediatrics (Evanston), 2018-10, Vol.142 (4), p.e20174276
issn 0031-4005
1098-4275
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_2102339332
source MEDLINE; EZB-FREE-00999 freely available EZB journals; Alma/SFX Local Collection
subjects Adolescent
Adult-child interactions
Age
Analysis
Child
Child, Preschool
Children
Children & youth
Cognition
Cognitive ability
Cognitive development
Comprehension
Conversation
Developmental stages
Effectiveness
Female
Follow-Up Studies
Humans
Infant
Infants
Language
Language acquisition
Language Development
Language Tests
Language thought relationship
Learning environment
Linear models (Statistics)
Linear regression models
Male
Native language acquisition
Parent and child
Parent-Child Relations
Pediatrics
Social Class
Social classes
Social Environment
Socioeconomic status
Socioeconomics
Turn taking
Wechsler Scales
title Language Experience in the Second Year of Life and Language Outcomes in Late Childhood
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-29T13%3A25%3A14IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-gale_proqu&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Language%20Experience%20in%20the%20Second%20Year%20of%20Life%20and%20Language%20Outcomes%20in%20Late%20Childhood&rft.jtitle=Pediatrics%20(Evanston)&rft.au=Gilkerson,%20Jill&rft.date=2018-10&rft.volume=142&rft.issue=4&rft.spage=e20174276&rft.pages=e20174276-&rft.issn=0031-4005&rft.eissn=1098-4275&rft_id=info:doi/10.1542/peds.2017-4276&rft_dat=%3Cgale_proqu%3EA561217486%3C/gale_proqu%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2130281778&rft_id=info:pmid/30201624&rft_galeid=A561217486&rfr_iscdi=true