The emergence of systematicity: How environmental and communicative factors shape a novel communication system

Where does linguistic structure come from? We suggest that systematicity in language evolves adaptively in response to environmental and contextual affordances associated with the practice of communication itself. In two experiments, we used a silent gesture referential game paradigm to investigate...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Cognition 2018-12, Vol.181, p.93-104
Hauptverfasser: Nölle, Jonas, Staib, Marlene, Fusaroli, Riccardo, Tylén, Kristian
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 104
container_issue
container_start_page 93
container_title Cognition
container_volume 181
creator Nölle, Jonas
Staib, Marlene
Fusaroli, Riccardo
Tylén, Kristian
description Where does linguistic structure come from? We suggest that systematicity in language evolves adaptively in response to environmental and contextual affordances associated with the practice of communication itself. In two experiments, we used a silent gesture referential game paradigm to investigate environmental and social factors promoting the propagation of systematicity in a novel communication system. We found that structure in the emerging communication systems evolve contingent on structural properties of the environment. More specifically, interlocutors spontaneously relied on structural features of the referent stimuli they communicated about to motivate systematic aspects of the evolving communication system even when idiosyncratic iconic strategies were equally afforded. Furthermore, we found systematicity to be promoted by the nature of the referent environment. When the referent environment was open and unstable, analytic systematic strategies were more likely to emerge compared to stimulus environments with a closed set of referents. Lastly, we found that displacement of communication promoted systematicity. That is, when interlocutors had to communicate about items not immediately present in the moment of communication, they were more likely to evolve systematic solutions, supposedly due to working memory advantages. Together, our findings provide experimental evidence for the idea that linguistic structure evolves adaptively from contextually situated language use.
doi_str_mv 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.08.014
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_2099041407</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><els_id>S0010027718302208</els_id><sourcerecordid>2133833417</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c496t-c60394a69c7a4b84fe88407d66a15249f116541b7f0634367a48664af0ef101c3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkcGKFDEQhoMo7rj6Chrw4qXHSiebpL0ti7rCgpf1HDLpym6G7mRM0iPz9maYcREvQkFdvvqrqI-QdwzWDJj8uF279BBDDSmue2B6Da2YeEZWTCveKc31c7ICYNBBr9QFeVXKFgBEr_RLcsGBKc5Arki8f0SKM-YHjA5p8rQcSsXZ1uBCPXyit-kXxbgPOcUZY7UTtXGkLs3zEoNr2B6pt66mXGh5tDuklsa0x-lvJsVz7Gvywtup4JtzvyQ_vny-v7nt7r5__XZzfdc5McjaOQl8EFYOTlmx0cKj1gLUKKVlV70YPGPySrCN8iC54LJRWkphPaBv_3H8knw45e5y-rlgqWYOxeE02YhpKaaHYQDBWmZD3_-DbtOSY7vO9IxzzblgR0qdKJdTKRm92eUw23wwDMxRidmaJyXmqMRAKyba5Ntz_rKZcXya--OgAdcnANtD9gGzKS4cbYwho6tmTOG_S34DvQOh1g</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2133833417</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>The emergence of systematicity: How environmental and communicative factors shape a novel communication system</title><source>Sociological Abstracts</source><source>Access via ScienceDirect (Elsevier)</source><creator>Nölle, Jonas ; Staib, Marlene ; Fusaroli, Riccardo ; Tylén, Kristian</creator><creatorcontrib>Nölle, Jonas ; Staib, Marlene ; Fusaroli, Riccardo ; Tylén, Kristian</creatorcontrib><description>Where does linguistic structure come from? We suggest that systematicity in language evolves adaptively in response to environmental and contextual affordances associated with the practice of communication itself. In two experiments, we used a silent gesture referential game paradigm to investigate environmental and social factors promoting the propagation of systematicity in a novel communication system. We found that structure in the emerging communication systems evolve contingent on structural properties of the environment. More specifically, interlocutors spontaneously relied on structural features of the referent stimuli they communicated about to motivate systematic aspects of the evolving communication system even when idiosyncratic iconic strategies were equally afforded. Furthermore, we found systematicity to be promoted by the nature of the referent environment. When the referent environment was open and unstable, analytic systematic strategies were more likely to emerge compared to stimulus environments with a closed set of referents. Lastly, we found that displacement of communication promoted systematicity. That is, when interlocutors had to communicate about items not immediately present in the moment of communication, they were more likely to evolve systematic solutions, supposedly due to working memory advantages. Together, our findings provide experimental evidence for the idea that linguistic structure evolves adaptively from contextually situated language use.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0010-0277</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1873-7838</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.08.014</identifier><identifier>PMID: 30173106</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Netherlands: Elsevier B.V</publisher><subject>Communication ; Communications systems ; Environment ; Experimental semiotics ; Iconicity ; Interaction ; Language ; Language evolution ; Language usage ; Nonverbal communication ; Referents ; Semiotics ; Short term memory ; Silent gesture ; Social factors ; Social interaction ; Stimulus ; Systematicity</subject><ispartof>Cognition, 2018-12, Vol.181, p.93-104</ispartof><rights>2018 Elsevier B.V.</rights><rights>Copyright © 2018 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.</rights><rights>Copyright Elsevier Science Ltd. Dec 2018</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c496t-c60394a69c7a4b84fe88407d66a15249f116541b7f0634367a48664af0ef101c3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c496t-c60394a69c7a4b84fe88407d66a15249f116541b7f0634367a48664af0ef101c3</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.08.014$$EHTML$$P50$$Gelsevier$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,3550,27924,27925,33774,45995</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30173106$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Nölle, Jonas</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Staib, Marlene</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Fusaroli, Riccardo</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Tylén, Kristian</creatorcontrib><title>The emergence of systematicity: How environmental and communicative factors shape a novel communication system</title><title>Cognition</title><addtitle>Cognition</addtitle><description>Where does linguistic structure come from? We suggest that systematicity in language evolves adaptively in response to environmental and contextual affordances associated with the practice of communication itself. In two experiments, we used a silent gesture referential game paradigm to investigate environmental and social factors promoting the propagation of systematicity in a novel communication system. We found that structure in the emerging communication systems evolve contingent on structural properties of the environment. More specifically, interlocutors spontaneously relied on structural features of the referent stimuli they communicated about to motivate systematic aspects of the evolving communication system even when idiosyncratic iconic strategies were equally afforded. Furthermore, we found systematicity to be promoted by the nature of the referent environment. When the referent environment was open and unstable, analytic systematic strategies were more likely to emerge compared to stimulus environments with a closed set of referents. Lastly, we found that displacement of communication promoted systematicity. That is, when interlocutors had to communicate about items not immediately present in the moment of communication, they were more likely to evolve systematic solutions, supposedly due to working memory advantages. Together, our findings provide experimental evidence for the idea that linguistic structure evolves adaptively from contextually situated language use.</description><subject>Communication</subject><subject>Communications systems</subject><subject>Environment</subject><subject>Experimental semiotics</subject><subject>Iconicity</subject><subject>Interaction</subject><subject>Language</subject><subject>Language evolution</subject><subject>Language usage</subject><subject>Nonverbal communication</subject><subject>Referents</subject><subject>Semiotics</subject><subject>Short term memory</subject><subject>Silent gesture</subject><subject>Social factors</subject><subject>Social interaction</subject><subject>Stimulus</subject><subject>Systematicity</subject><issn>0010-0277</issn><issn>1873-7838</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2018</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>BHHNA</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkcGKFDEQhoMo7rj6Chrw4qXHSiebpL0ti7rCgpf1HDLpym6G7mRM0iPz9maYcREvQkFdvvqrqI-QdwzWDJj8uF279BBDDSmue2B6Da2YeEZWTCveKc31c7ICYNBBr9QFeVXKFgBEr_RLcsGBKc5Arki8f0SKM-YHjA5p8rQcSsXZ1uBCPXyit-kXxbgPOcUZY7UTtXGkLs3zEoNr2B6pt66mXGh5tDuklsa0x-lvJsVz7Gvywtup4JtzvyQ_vny-v7nt7r5__XZzfdc5McjaOQl8EFYOTlmx0cKj1gLUKKVlV70YPGPySrCN8iC54LJRWkphPaBv_3H8knw45e5y-rlgqWYOxeE02YhpKaaHYQDBWmZD3_-DbtOSY7vO9IxzzblgR0qdKJdTKRm92eUw23wwDMxRidmaJyXmqMRAKyba5Ntz_rKZcXya--OgAdcnANtD9gGzKS4cbYwho6tmTOG_S34DvQOh1g</recordid><startdate>20181201</startdate><enddate>20181201</enddate><creator>Nölle, Jonas</creator><creator>Staib, Marlene</creator><creator>Fusaroli, Riccardo</creator><creator>Tylén, Kristian</creator><general>Elsevier B.V</general><general>Elsevier Science Ltd</general><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>7U4</scope><scope>8BJ</scope><scope>BHHNA</scope><scope>DWI</scope><scope>FQK</scope><scope>JBE</scope><scope>WZK</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20181201</creationdate><title>The emergence of systematicity: How environmental and communicative factors shape a novel communication system</title><author>Nölle, Jonas ; Staib, Marlene ; Fusaroli, Riccardo ; Tylén, Kristian</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c496t-c60394a69c7a4b84fe88407d66a15249f116541b7f0634367a48664af0ef101c3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2018</creationdate><topic>Communication</topic><topic>Communications systems</topic><topic>Environment</topic><topic>Experimental semiotics</topic><topic>Iconicity</topic><topic>Interaction</topic><topic>Language</topic><topic>Language evolution</topic><topic>Language usage</topic><topic>Nonverbal communication</topic><topic>Referents</topic><topic>Semiotics</topic><topic>Short term memory</topic><topic>Silent gesture</topic><topic>Social factors</topic><topic>Social interaction</topic><topic>Stimulus</topic><topic>Systematicity</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Nölle, Jonas</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Staib, Marlene</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Fusaroli, Riccardo</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Tylén, Kristian</creatorcontrib><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>Sociological Abstracts (pre-2017)</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS)</collection><collection>Sociological Abstracts</collection><collection>Sociological Abstracts</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences</collection><collection>Sociological Abstracts (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Cognition</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Nölle, Jonas</au><au>Staib, Marlene</au><au>Fusaroli, Riccardo</au><au>Tylén, Kristian</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>The emergence of systematicity: How environmental and communicative factors shape a novel communication system</atitle><jtitle>Cognition</jtitle><addtitle>Cognition</addtitle><date>2018-12-01</date><risdate>2018</risdate><volume>181</volume><spage>93</spage><epage>104</epage><pages>93-104</pages><issn>0010-0277</issn><eissn>1873-7838</eissn><abstract>Where does linguistic structure come from? We suggest that systematicity in language evolves adaptively in response to environmental and contextual affordances associated with the practice of communication itself. In two experiments, we used a silent gesture referential game paradigm to investigate environmental and social factors promoting the propagation of systematicity in a novel communication system. We found that structure in the emerging communication systems evolve contingent on structural properties of the environment. More specifically, interlocutors spontaneously relied on structural features of the referent stimuli they communicated about to motivate systematic aspects of the evolving communication system even when idiosyncratic iconic strategies were equally afforded. Furthermore, we found systematicity to be promoted by the nature of the referent environment. When the referent environment was open and unstable, analytic systematic strategies were more likely to emerge compared to stimulus environments with a closed set of referents. Lastly, we found that displacement of communication promoted systematicity. That is, when interlocutors had to communicate about items not immediately present in the moment of communication, they were more likely to evolve systematic solutions, supposedly due to working memory advantages. Together, our findings provide experimental evidence for the idea that linguistic structure evolves adaptively from contextually situated language use.</abstract><cop>Netherlands</cop><pub>Elsevier B.V</pub><pmid>30173106</pmid><doi>10.1016/j.cognition.2018.08.014</doi><tpages>12</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0010-0277
ispartof Cognition, 2018-12, Vol.181, p.93-104
issn 0010-0277
1873-7838
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_2099041407
source Sociological Abstracts; Access via ScienceDirect (Elsevier)
subjects Communication
Communications systems
Environment
Experimental semiotics
Iconicity
Interaction
Language
Language evolution
Language usage
Nonverbal communication
Referents
Semiotics
Short term memory
Silent gesture
Social factors
Social interaction
Stimulus
Systematicity
title The emergence of systematicity: How environmental and communicative factors shape a novel communication system
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-25T18%3A56%3A27IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=The%20emergence%20of%20systematicity:%20How%20environmental%20and%20communicative%20factors%20shape%20a%20novel%20communication%20system&rft.jtitle=Cognition&rft.au=N%C3%B6lle,%20Jonas&rft.date=2018-12-01&rft.volume=181&rft.spage=93&rft.epage=104&rft.pages=93-104&rft.issn=0010-0277&rft.eissn=1873-7838&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.08.014&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E2133833417%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2133833417&rft_id=info:pmid/30173106&rft_els_id=S0010027718302208&rfr_iscdi=true