Persistence and change of vocal signals in natural populations of chickadees: annual sampling of the gargle call over eight seasons
A study of the gargle call, an agonistic call of the black-capped chickadee Poecile atricapillus, for eight successive fall/winter seasons in three resident populations in Colorado (USA) revealed the following: (1) Individuals had repertoires of calls averaging 8.1 distinct types per bird (range 2-1...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Behaviour 2006, Vol.143 (12), p.1473-1509 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 1509 |
---|---|
container_issue | 12 |
container_start_page | 1473 |
container_title | Behaviour |
container_volume | 143 |
creator | Baker, Myron C. Gammon, David E. |
description | A study of the gargle call, an agonistic call of the black-capped chickadee Poecile atricapillus, for eight successive fall/winter seasons in three resident populations in Colorado (USA) revealed the following: (1) Individuals had repertoires of calls averaging 8.1 distinct types per bird (range 2-18). (2) Among individuals in a local population many calls shared some of the component syllables constituting the whole gargle calls; often the shared syllables occurred in different locations in the sequences of syllables (syntax) making up the calls. (3) Individuals also shared many of the same calls (identical or near identical syllable sequences); thus in a local sample the ensemble of call types in the population (the population repertoire) could be found by recording less than 10 birds. (4) In each population, the ensemble of gargle call types found in a given season consisted of a core group of about 10 kinds of gargle calls that persisted over all or nearly all the eight seasons of the study, but also the ensemble included a number of call types that were present for only one or a few seasons, or that occurred intermittently across time. (5) The great majority of call types that were lost from a population were explained by the loss of individual birds (carriers) by emigration or death. (6) The great majority of call types new in a population in any given season were explained by the occurrence of a new bird in the sample (local recruitment or immigration). (7) Both qualitative classification of gargle syllables and whole gargle calls by the method of visual inspection of sound spectrograms and automated quantitative analysis of acoustic features of syllables and calls were carried out in some examples, and these comparisons gave highly concordant results. Long-term studies of cultural evolution of non-song calls are infrequent but important because the sources and strength of social selection on non-song vocalizations are often likely to differ from those social factors influencing cultural evolution in territorial songs, the more often studied kind of bird acoustic signals. As a primary finding of this study we showed, using fine-grained measurements of acoustic variables, that a population retains a set of core gargle groups, each group representing a cultural tradition (behavioral lineage) exhibiting virtually no drift in detailed structure across eight consecutive seasons. In a small set of samples of core groups going back to 1988, there also was |
doi_str_mv | 10.1163/156853906779366991 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>jstor_proqu</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_19589445</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><jstor_id>4536421</jstor_id><sourcerecordid>4536421</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-b408t-548b875b1b1567fd78810ad0e36b921f123521d7543e3ecb2f3e4bb49e6c6a843</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9UT1v1DAYjhCVOFr-AGLwAluKHcd2zAYnKNBKFKmIbpbjvMn56rODnVR05o_jKFUZKjFZfr7er6J4SfApIZy-JYw3jErMhZCUcynJk2KzgGVGr58WG4wxK4Vk8lnxPKV9_gpG2ab4cwkx2TSBN4C075DZaT8ACj26DUY7lOzgtUvIeuT1NMcMjWGcnZ5s8GnRmZ01N7oDSO9ygp8Xkz6MzvphoacdoEHHwQHKeQ6FW4gI7LCbUAKdcshJcdTnEvDi_j0ufnz6eLX9XF58O_uyfX9RtjVuppLVTdsI1pI2Dyb6TjQNwbrDQHkrK9KTirKKdILVFCiYtuop1G1bS-CG66amx8WbNXeM4dcMaVIHmww4pz2EOSkiWSPrmmVhtQpNDClF6NUY7UHHO0WwWvatHu87m17fp-uUB-2j9samf86GEYo5z7pXq26fphAf-FyW19USU670cpTfD7SON4oLKpj6en6lfl5-uD6n37dq6RWv-jZa59Q-zHG51_86_QtueqgD</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>19589445</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Persistence and change of vocal signals in natural populations of chickadees: annual sampling of the gargle call over eight seasons</title><source>JSTOR Archive Collection A-Z Listing</source><creator>Baker, Myron C. ; Gammon, David E.</creator><creatorcontrib>Baker, Myron C. ; Gammon, David E.</creatorcontrib><description>A study of the gargle call, an agonistic call of the black-capped chickadee Poecile atricapillus, for eight successive fall/winter seasons in three resident populations in Colorado (USA) revealed the following: (1) Individuals had repertoires of calls averaging 8.1 distinct types per bird (range 2-18). (2) Among individuals in a local population many calls shared some of the component syllables constituting the whole gargle calls; often the shared syllables occurred in different locations in the sequences of syllables (syntax) making up the calls. (3) Individuals also shared many of the same calls (identical or near identical syllable sequences); thus in a local sample the ensemble of call types in the population (the population repertoire) could be found by recording less than 10 birds. (4) In each population, the ensemble of gargle call types found in a given season consisted of a core group of about 10 kinds of gargle calls that persisted over all or nearly all the eight seasons of the study, but also the ensemble included a number of call types that were present for only one or a few seasons, or that occurred intermittently across time. (5) The great majority of call types that were lost from a population were explained by the loss of individual birds (carriers) by emigration or death. (6) The great majority of call types new in a population in any given season were explained by the occurrence of a new bird in the sample (local recruitment or immigration). (7) Both qualitative classification of gargle syllables and whole gargle calls by the method of visual inspection of sound spectrograms and automated quantitative analysis of acoustic features of syllables and calls were carried out in some examples, and these comparisons gave highly concordant results. Long-term studies of cultural evolution of non-song calls are infrequent but important because the sources and strength of social selection on non-song vocalizations are often likely to differ from those social factors influencing cultural evolution in territorial songs, the more often studied kind of bird acoustic signals. As a primary finding of this study we showed, using fine-grained measurements of acoustic variables, that a population retains a set of core gargle groups, each group representing a cultural tradition (behavioral lineage) exhibiting virtually no drift in detailed structure across eight consecutive seasons. In a small set of samples of core groups going back to 1988, there also was no evidence of change within a vocal tradition. These results suggest the hypothesis of strong canalizing social selection on the acoustic structure of gargles.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0005-7959</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1568-539X</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1163/156853906779366991</identifier><identifier>CODEN: BEHAA8</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>The Netherlands: Brill</publisher><subject>Animal ethology ; Animal vocalization ; Biological and medical sciences ; Bird calls ; Bird songs ; BIRDS ; CHICKADEES ; CULTURAL EVOLUTION ; Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology ; GARGLE CALL POECILE ATRICAPILLUS ; Poecile ; Poecile atricapilla ; Population dynamics ; Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry ; Sound ; Spectrograms ; Syllables ; VOCALIZATIONS ; Wildlife population studies</subject><ispartof>Behaviour, 2006, Vol.143 (12), p.1473-1509</ispartof><rights>2006 Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, The Netherlands</rights><rights>Copyright 2006 Koninklijke Brill NV</rights><rights>2007 INIST-CNRS</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-b408t-548b875b1b1567fd78810ad0e36b921f123521d7543e3ecb2f3e4bb49e6c6a843</citedby></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.jstor.org/stable/pdf/4536421$$EPDF$$P50$$Gjstor$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.jstor.org/stable/4536421$$EHTML$$P50$$Gjstor$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,803,4022,27922,27923,27924,58016,58249</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=18513066$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Baker, Myron C.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Gammon, David E.</creatorcontrib><title>Persistence and change of vocal signals in natural populations of chickadees: annual sampling of the gargle call over eight seasons</title><title>Behaviour</title><addtitle>BEH</addtitle><description>A study of the gargle call, an agonistic call of the black-capped chickadee Poecile atricapillus, for eight successive fall/winter seasons in three resident populations in Colorado (USA) revealed the following: (1) Individuals had repertoires of calls averaging 8.1 distinct types per bird (range 2-18). (2) Among individuals in a local population many calls shared some of the component syllables constituting the whole gargle calls; often the shared syllables occurred in different locations in the sequences of syllables (syntax) making up the calls. (3) Individuals also shared many of the same calls (identical or near identical syllable sequences); thus in a local sample the ensemble of call types in the population (the population repertoire) could be found by recording less than 10 birds. (4) In each population, the ensemble of gargle call types found in a given season consisted of a core group of about 10 kinds of gargle calls that persisted over all or nearly all the eight seasons of the study, but also the ensemble included a number of call types that were present for only one or a few seasons, or that occurred intermittently across time. (5) The great majority of call types that were lost from a population were explained by the loss of individual birds (carriers) by emigration or death. (6) The great majority of call types new in a population in any given season were explained by the occurrence of a new bird in the sample (local recruitment or immigration). (7) Both qualitative classification of gargle syllables and whole gargle calls by the method of visual inspection of sound spectrograms and automated quantitative analysis of acoustic features of syllables and calls were carried out in some examples, and these comparisons gave highly concordant results. Long-term studies of cultural evolution of non-song calls are infrequent but important because the sources and strength of social selection on non-song vocalizations are often likely to differ from those social factors influencing cultural evolution in territorial songs, the more often studied kind of bird acoustic signals. As a primary finding of this study we showed, using fine-grained measurements of acoustic variables, that a population retains a set of core gargle groups, each group representing a cultural tradition (behavioral lineage) exhibiting virtually no drift in detailed structure across eight consecutive seasons. In a small set of samples of core groups going back to 1988, there also was no evidence of change within a vocal tradition. These results suggest the hypothesis of strong canalizing social selection on the acoustic structure of gargles.</description><subject>Animal ethology</subject><subject>Animal vocalization</subject><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Bird calls</subject><subject>Bird songs</subject><subject>BIRDS</subject><subject>CHICKADEES</subject><subject>CULTURAL EVOLUTION</subject><subject>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</subject><subject>GARGLE CALL POECILE ATRICAPILLUS</subject><subject>Poecile</subject><subject>Poecile atricapilla</subject><subject>Population dynamics</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</subject><subject>Sound</subject><subject>Spectrograms</subject><subject>Syllables</subject><subject>VOCALIZATIONS</subject><subject>Wildlife population studies</subject><issn>0005-7959</issn><issn>1568-539X</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2006</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><recordid>eNp9UT1v1DAYjhCVOFr-AGLwAluKHcd2zAYnKNBKFKmIbpbjvMn56rODnVR05o_jKFUZKjFZfr7er6J4SfApIZy-JYw3jErMhZCUcynJk2KzgGVGr58WG4wxK4Vk8lnxPKV9_gpG2ab4cwkx2TSBN4C075DZaT8ACj26DUY7lOzgtUvIeuT1NMcMjWGcnZ5s8GnRmZ01N7oDSO9ygp8Xkz6MzvphoacdoEHHwQHKeQ6FW4gI7LCbUAKdcshJcdTnEvDi_j0ufnz6eLX9XF58O_uyfX9RtjVuppLVTdsI1pI2Dyb6TjQNwbrDQHkrK9KTirKKdILVFCiYtuop1G1bS-CG66amx8WbNXeM4dcMaVIHmww4pz2EOSkiWSPrmmVhtQpNDClF6NUY7UHHO0WwWvatHu87m17fp-uUB-2j9samf86GEYo5z7pXq26fphAf-FyW19USU670cpTfD7SON4oLKpj6en6lfl5-uD6n37dq6RWv-jZa59Q-zHG51_86_QtueqgD</recordid><startdate>2006</startdate><enddate>2006</enddate><creator>Baker, Myron C.</creator><creator>Gammon, David E.</creator><general>Brill</general><general>BRILL</general><scope>BSCLL</scope><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7QG</scope></search><sort><creationdate>2006</creationdate><title>Persistence and change of vocal signals in natural populations of chickadees: annual sampling of the gargle call over eight seasons</title><author>Baker, Myron C. ; Gammon, David E.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-b408t-548b875b1b1567fd78810ad0e36b921f123521d7543e3ecb2f3e4bb49e6c6a843</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2006</creationdate><topic>Animal ethology</topic><topic>Animal vocalization</topic><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Bird calls</topic><topic>Bird songs</topic><topic>BIRDS</topic><topic>CHICKADEES</topic><topic>CULTURAL EVOLUTION</topic><topic>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</topic><topic>GARGLE CALL POECILE ATRICAPILLUS</topic><topic>Poecile</topic><topic>Poecile atricapilla</topic><topic>Population dynamics</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</topic><topic>Sound</topic><topic>Spectrograms</topic><topic>Syllables</topic><topic>VOCALIZATIONS</topic><topic>Wildlife population studies</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Baker, Myron C.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Gammon, David E.</creatorcontrib><collection>Istex</collection><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><jtitle>Behaviour</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Baker, Myron C.</au><au>Gammon, David E.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Persistence and change of vocal signals in natural populations of chickadees: annual sampling of the gargle call over eight seasons</atitle><jtitle>Behaviour</jtitle><addtitle>BEH</addtitle><date>2006</date><risdate>2006</risdate><volume>143</volume><issue>12</issue><spage>1473</spage><epage>1509</epage><pages>1473-1509</pages><issn>0005-7959</issn><eissn>1568-539X</eissn><coden>BEHAA8</coden><abstract>A study of the gargle call, an agonistic call of the black-capped chickadee Poecile atricapillus, for eight successive fall/winter seasons in three resident populations in Colorado (USA) revealed the following: (1) Individuals had repertoires of calls averaging 8.1 distinct types per bird (range 2-18). (2) Among individuals in a local population many calls shared some of the component syllables constituting the whole gargle calls; often the shared syllables occurred in different locations in the sequences of syllables (syntax) making up the calls. (3) Individuals also shared many of the same calls (identical or near identical syllable sequences); thus in a local sample the ensemble of call types in the population (the population repertoire) could be found by recording less than 10 birds. (4) In each population, the ensemble of gargle call types found in a given season consisted of a core group of about 10 kinds of gargle calls that persisted over all or nearly all the eight seasons of the study, but also the ensemble included a number of call types that were present for only one or a few seasons, or that occurred intermittently across time. (5) The great majority of call types that were lost from a population were explained by the loss of individual birds (carriers) by emigration or death. (6) The great majority of call types new in a population in any given season were explained by the occurrence of a new bird in the sample (local recruitment or immigration). (7) Both qualitative classification of gargle syllables and whole gargle calls by the method of visual inspection of sound spectrograms and automated quantitative analysis of acoustic features of syllables and calls were carried out in some examples, and these comparisons gave highly concordant results. Long-term studies of cultural evolution of non-song calls are infrequent but important because the sources and strength of social selection on non-song vocalizations are often likely to differ from those social factors influencing cultural evolution in territorial songs, the more often studied kind of bird acoustic signals. As a primary finding of this study we showed, using fine-grained measurements of acoustic variables, that a population retains a set of core gargle groups, each group representing a cultural tradition (behavioral lineage) exhibiting virtually no drift in detailed structure across eight consecutive seasons. In a small set of samples of core groups going back to 1988, there also was no evidence of change within a vocal tradition. These results suggest the hypothesis of strong canalizing social selection on the acoustic structure of gargles.</abstract><cop>The Netherlands</cop><pub>Brill</pub><doi>10.1163/156853906779366991</doi><tpages>37</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0005-7959 |
ispartof | Behaviour, 2006, Vol.143 (12), p.1473-1509 |
issn | 0005-7959 1568-539X |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_19589445 |
source | JSTOR Archive Collection A-Z Listing |
subjects | Animal ethology Animal vocalization Biological and medical sciences Bird calls Bird songs BIRDS CHICKADEES CULTURAL EVOLUTION Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology GARGLE CALL POECILE ATRICAPILLUS Poecile Poecile atricapilla Population dynamics Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry Sound Spectrograms Syllables VOCALIZATIONS Wildlife population studies |
title | Persistence and change of vocal signals in natural populations of chickadees: annual sampling of the gargle call over eight seasons |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-12T03%3A40%3A49IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-jstor_proqu&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Persistence%20and%20change%20of%20vocal%20signals%20in%20natural%20populations%20of%20chickadees:%20annual%20sampling%20of%20the%20gargle%20call%20over%20eight%20seasons&rft.jtitle=Behaviour&rft.au=Baker,%20Myron%20C.&rft.date=2006&rft.volume=143&rft.issue=12&rft.spage=1473&rft.epage=1509&rft.pages=1473-1509&rft.issn=0005-7959&rft.eissn=1568-539X&rft.coden=BEHAA8&rft_id=info:doi/10.1163/156853906779366991&rft_dat=%3Cjstor_proqu%3E4536421%3C/jstor_proqu%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=19589445&rft_id=info:pmid/&rft_jstor_id=4536421&rfr_iscdi=true |