Individual variation in intentionality in the mind-wandering state is reflected in the integration of the default-mode, fronto-parietal, and limbic networks

Mind-wandering has a controversial relationship with cognitive control. Existing psychological evidence supports the hypothesis that episodes of mind-wandering reflect a failure to constrain thinking to task-relevant material, as well the apparently alternative view that control can facilitate the e...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:NeuroImage (Orlando, Fla.) Fla.), 2017-02, Vol.146, p.226-235
Hauptverfasser: Golchert, Johannes, Smallwood, Jonathan, Jefferies, Elizabeth, Seli, Paul, Huntenburg, Julia M., Liem, Franziskus, Lauckner, Mark E., Oligschläger, Sabine, Bernhardt, Boris C., Villringer, Arno, Margulies, Daniel S.
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 235
container_issue
container_start_page 226
container_title NeuroImage (Orlando, Fla.)
container_volume 146
creator Golchert, Johannes
Smallwood, Jonathan
Jefferies, Elizabeth
Seli, Paul
Huntenburg, Julia M.
Liem, Franziskus
Lauckner, Mark E.
Oligschläger, Sabine
Bernhardt, Boris C.
Villringer, Arno
Margulies, Daniel S.
description Mind-wandering has a controversial relationship with cognitive control. Existing psychological evidence supports the hypothesis that episodes of mind-wandering reflect a failure to constrain thinking to task-relevant material, as well the apparently alternative view that control can facilitate the expression of self-generated mental content. We assessed whether this apparent contradiction arises because of a failure to consider differences in the types of thoughts that occur during mind-wandering, and in particular, the associated level of intentionality. Using multi-modal magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) analysis, we examined the cortical organisation that underlies inter-individual differences in descriptions of the spontaneous or deliberate nature of mind-wandering. Cortical thickness, as well as functional connectivity analyses, implicated regions relevant to cognitive control and regions of the default-mode network for individuals who reported high rates of deliberate mind-wandering. In contrast, higher reports of spontaneous mind-wandering were associated with cortical thinning in parietal and posterior temporal regions in the left hemisphere (which are important in the control of cognition and attention) as well as heightened connectivity between the intraparietal sulcus and a region that spanned limbic and default-mode regions in the ventral inferior frontal gyrus. Finally, we observed a dissociation in the thickness of the retrosplenial cortex/lingual gyrus, with higher reports of spontaneous mind-wandering being associated with thickening in the left hemisphere, and higher repots of deliberate mind-wandering with thinning in the right hemisphere. These results suggest that the intentionality of the mind-wandering state depends on integration between the control and default-mode networks, with more deliberation being associated with greater integration between these systems. We conclude that one reason why mind-wandering has a controversial relationship with control is because it depends on whether the thoughts emerge in a deliberate or spontaneous fashion. •Deliberate and spontaneous mind-wandering have unique structural and functional correlates.•Reports of deliberate mind-wandering correlated with regions in both default-mode and fronto-parietal networks.•Spontaneous mind-wandering was linked to less integrity in parietal and temporal regions.•Intentionality during the mind-wandering state may depend upon integration between the default-mode an
doi_str_mv 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.025
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_1877844191</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><els_id>S1053811916306425</els_id><sourcerecordid>1841793815</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c551t-af158f33109b8d39d587486e0368e66f0194be8cf26289f22997e0cf769b39653</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqNkc-O1SAUhxujcf7oKxgSNy6mV6CFwlIn6kwyiRtdE1oOV64tXIHeybzLPKzgndHEjSYkhZPv_E56vqZBBG8IJvztbuNhjcEtegsbWiobQjaYsifNKcGStZIN9Gm9s64VhMiT5iylHcZYkl48b07oIHiPBT1t7q-9cQdnVj2jg45OZxc8cvVk8PWhZ5fvaiV_A7Q4b9pb7Q1E57coZZ0BuYQi2BmmDOYRrO3beEwL9lfJgNXrnNslGLhANgafQ7svMyHr-QKVUDS7ZXQT8pBvQ_yeXjTPrJ4TvHz4njdfP374cnnV3nz-dH357qadGCO51ZYwYbuu_PooTCcNE0MvOOCOC-DcYiL7EcRkKadCWkqlHABPduBy7CRn3Xnz5pi7j-HHCimrxaUJ5ll7CGtSRAyD6HsiyX-gPRlkJ0hNff0XugtrLPusFB8kY5LWQHGkphhSKntU-1i8xjtFsKqy1U79ka2qbEWIKrJL66uHAeu4gPnd-Gi3AO-PAJTlHRxElSYHfgLjYrGlTHD_nvITYB7Bsg</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1867955921</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Individual variation in intentionality in the mind-wandering state is reflected in the integration of the default-mode, fronto-parietal, and limbic networks</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>Access via ScienceDirect (Elsevier)</source><source>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</source><creator>Golchert, Johannes ; Smallwood, Jonathan ; Jefferies, Elizabeth ; Seli, Paul ; Huntenburg, Julia M. ; Liem, Franziskus ; Lauckner, Mark E. ; Oligschläger, Sabine ; Bernhardt, Boris C. ; Villringer, Arno ; Margulies, Daniel S.</creator><creatorcontrib>Golchert, Johannes ; Smallwood, Jonathan ; Jefferies, Elizabeth ; Seli, Paul ; Huntenburg, Julia M. ; Liem, Franziskus ; Lauckner, Mark E. ; Oligschläger, Sabine ; Bernhardt, Boris C. ; Villringer, Arno ; Margulies, Daniel S.</creatorcontrib><description>Mind-wandering has a controversial relationship with cognitive control. Existing psychological evidence supports the hypothesis that episodes of mind-wandering reflect a failure to constrain thinking to task-relevant material, as well the apparently alternative view that control can facilitate the expression of self-generated mental content. We assessed whether this apparent contradiction arises because of a failure to consider differences in the types of thoughts that occur during mind-wandering, and in particular, the associated level of intentionality. Using multi-modal magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) analysis, we examined the cortical organisation that underlies inter-individual differences in descriptions of the spontaneous or deliberate nature of mind-wandering. Cortical thickness, as well as functional connectivity analyses, implicated regions relevant to cognitive control and regions of the default-mode network for individuals who reported high rates of deliberate mind-wandering. In contrast, higher reports of spontaneous mind-wandering were associated with cortical thinning in parietal and posterior temporal regions in the left hemisphere (which are important in the control of cognition and attention) as well as heightened connectivity between the intraparietal sulcus and a region that spanned limbic and default-mode regions in the ventral inferior frontal gyrus. Finally, we observed a dissociation in the thickness of the retrosplenial cortex/lingual gyrus, with higher reports of spontaneous mind-wandering being associated with thickening in the left hemisphere, and higher repots of deliberate mind-wandering with thinning in the right hemisphere. These results suggest that the intentionality of the mind-wandering state depends on integration between the control and default-mode networks, with more deliberation being associated with greater integration between these systems. We conclude that one reason why mind-wandering has a controversial relationship with control is because it depends on whether the thoughts emerge in a deliberate or spontaneous fashion. •Deliberate and spontaneous mind-wandering have unique structural and functional correlates.•Reports of deliberate mind-wandering correlated with regions in both default-mode and fronto-parietal networks.•Spontaneous mind-wandering was linked to less integrity in parietal and temporal regions.•Intentionality during the mind-wandering state may depend upon integration between the default-mode and fronto-parietal networks.•These neurocognitive differences explain why mind-wandering has a complex relationship with cognitive control.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1053-8119</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1095-9572</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.025</identifier><identifier>PMID: 27864082</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: Elsevier Inc</publisher><subject><![CDATA[Adult ; Brain - anatomy & histology ; Brain - physiology ; Brain Mapping ; Cognition & reasoning ; Cognitive ability ; Cognitive control ; Cortical thickness ; Female ; Frontal gyrus ; Frontal Lobe - anatomy & histology ; Frontal Lobe - physiology ; functional connectivity ; Hemispheric laterality ; Humans ; Individuality ; Integration ; Intention ; Intentionality ; Intraparietal sulcus ; Limbic Lobe - anatomy & histology ; Limbic Lobe - physiology ; Magnetic Resonance Imaging ; Male ; Mind-wandering ; Neural Pathways - anatomy & histology ; Neural Pathways - physiology ; Neuroimaging ; NMR ; Nuclear magnetic resonance ; Parietal Lobe - anatomy & histology ; Parietal Lobe - physiology ; Studies ; Temporal cortex ; Temporal Lobe - anatomy & histology ; Temporal Lobe - physiology ; Thinking - physiology ; Thinning ; Young Adult]]></subject><ispartof>NeuroImage (Orlando, Fla.), 2017-02, Vol.146, p.226-235</ispartof><rights>2016 Elsevier Inc.</rights><rights>Copyright © 2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.</rights><rights>2016. Elsevier Inc.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c551t-af158f33109b8d39d587486e0368e66f0194be8cf26289f22997e0cf769b39653</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c551t-af158f33109b8d39d587486e0368e66f0194be8cf26289f22997e0cf769b39653</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/1867955921?pq-origsite=primo$$EHTML$$P50$$Gproquest$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,3550,27924,27925,45995,64385,64387,64389,72469</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27864082$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Golchert, Johannes</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Smallwood, Jonathan</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Jefferies, Elizabeth</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Seli, Paul</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Huntenburg, Julia M.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Liem, Franziskus</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Lauckner, Mark E.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Oligschläger, Sabine</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Bernhardt, Boris C.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Villringer, Arno</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Margulies, Daniel S.</creatorcontrib><title>Individual variation in intentionality in the mind-wandering state is reflected in the integration of the default-mode, fronto-parietal, and limbic networks</title><title>NeuroImage (Orlando, Fla.)</title><addtitle>Neuroimage</addtitle><description>Mind-wandering has a controversial relationship with cognitive control. Existing psychological evidence supports the hypothesis that episodes of mind-wandering reflect a failure to constrain thinking to task-relevant material, as well the apparently alternative view that control can facilitate the expression of self-generated mental content. We assessed whether this apparent contradiction arises because of a failure to consider differences in the types of thoughts that occur during mind-wandering, and in particular, the associated level of intentionality. Using multi-modal magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) analysis, we examined the cortical organisation that underlies inter-individual differences in descriptions of the spontaneous or deliberate nature of mind-wandering. Cortical thickness, as well as functional connectivity analyses, implicated regions relevant to cognitive control and regions of the default-mode network for individuals who reported high rates of deliberate mind-wandering. In contrast, higher reports of spontaneous mind-wandering were associated with cortical thinning in parietal and posterior temporal regions in the left hemisphere (which are important in the control of cognition and attention) as well as heightened connectivity between the intraparietal sulcus and a region that spanned limbic and default-mode regions in the ventral inferior frontal gyrus. Finally, we observed a dissociation in the thickness of the retrosplenial cortex/lingual gyrus, with higher reports of spontaneous mind-wandering being associated with thickening in the left hemisphere, and higher repots of deliberate mind-wandering with thinning in the right hemisphere. These results suggest that the intentionality of the mind-wandering state depends on integration between the control and default-mode networks, with more deliberation being associated with greater integration between these systems. We conclude that one reason why mind-wandering has a controversial relationship with control is because it depends on whether the thoughts emerge in a deliberate or spontaneous fashion. •Deliberate and spontaneous mind-wandering have unique structural and functional correlates.•Reports of deliberate mind-wandering correlated with regions in both default-mode and fronto-parietal networks.•Spontaneous mind-wandering was linked to less integrity in parietal and temporal regions.•Intentionality during the mind-wandering state may depend upon integration between the default-mode and fronto-parietal networks.•These neurocognitive differences explain why mind-wandering has a complex relationship with cognitive control.</description><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Brain - anatomy &amp; histology</subject><subject>Brain - physiology</subject><subject>Brain Mapping</subject><subject>Cognition &amp; reasoning</subject><subject>Cognitive ability</subject><subject>Cognitive control</subject><subject>Cortical thickness</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Frontal gyrus</subject><subject>Frontal Lobe - anatomy &amp; histology</subject><subject>Frontal Lobe - physiology</subject><subject>functional connectivity</subject><subject>Hemispheric laterality</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Individuality</subject><subject>Integration</subject><subject>Intention</subject><subject>Intentionality</subject><subject>Intraparietal sulcus</subject><subject>Limbic Lobe - anatomy &amp; histology</subject><subject>Limbic Lobe - physiology</subject><subject>Magnetic Resonance Imaging</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Mind-wandering</subject><subject>Neural Pathways - anatomy &amp; histology</subject><subject>Neural Pathways - physiology</subject><subject>Neuroimaging</subject><subject>NMR</subject><subject>Nuclear magnetic resonance</subject><subject>Parietal Lobe - anatomy &amp; histology</subject><subject>Parietal Lobe - physiology</subject><subject>Studies</subject><subject>Temporal cortex</subject><subject>Temporal Lobe - anatomy &amp; histology</subject><subject>Temporal Lobe - physiology</subject><subject>Thinking - physiology</subject><subject>Thinning</subject><subject>Young Adult</subject><issn>1053-8119</issn><issn>1095-9572</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2017</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>ABUWG</sourceid><sourceid>AFKRA</sourceid><sourceid>AZQEC</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>CCPQU</sourceid><sourceid>DWQXO</sourceid><sourceid>GNUQQ</sourceid><recordid>eNqNkc-O1SAUhxujcf7oKxgSNy6mV6CFwlIn6kwyiRtdE1oOV64tXIHeybzLPKzgndHEjSYkhZPv_E56vqZBBG8IJvztbuNhjcEtegsbWiobQjaYsifNKcGStZIN9Gm9s64VhMiT5iylHcZYkl48b07oIHiPBT1t7q-9cQdnVj2jg45OZxc8cvVk8PWhZ5fvaiV_A7Q4b9pb7Q1E57coZZ0BuYQi2BmmDOYRrO3beEwL9lfJgNXrnNslGLhANgafQ7svMyHr-QKVUDS7ZXQT8pBvQ_yeXjTPrJ4TvHz4njdfP374cnnV3nz-dH357qadGCO51ZYwYbuu_PooTCcNE0MvOOCOC-DcYiL7EcRkKadCWkqlHABPduBy7CRn3Xnz5pi7j-HHCimrxaUJ5ll7CGtSRAyD6HsiyX-gPRlkJ0hNff0XugtrLPusFB8kY5LWQHGkphhSKntU-1i8xjtFsKqy1U79ka2qbEWIKrJL66uHAeu4gPnd-Gi3AO-PAJTlHRxElSYHfgLjYrGlTHD_nvITYB7Bsg</recordid><startdate>20170201</startdate><enddate>20170201</enddate><creator>Golchert, Johannes</creator><creator>Smallwood, Jonathan</creator><creator>Jefferies, Elizabeth</creator><creator>Seli, Paul</creator><creator>Huntenburg, Julia M.</creator><creator>Liem, Franziskus</creator><creator>Lauckner, Mark E.</creator><creator>Oligschläger, Sabine</creator><creator>Bernhardt, Boris C.</creator><creator>Villringer, Arno</creator><creator>Margulies, Daniel S.</creator><general>Elsevier Inc</general><general>Elsevier Limited</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FE</scope><scope>8FH</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BBNVY</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BHPHI</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>LK8</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>M7P</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>7QO</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20170201</creationdate><title>Individual variation in intentionality in the mind-wandering state is reflected in the integration of the default-mode, fronto-parietal, and limbic networks</title><author>Golchert, Johannes ; Smallwood, Jonathan ; Jefferies, Elizabeth ; Seli, Paul ; Huntenburg, Julia M. ; Liem, Franziskus ; Lauckner, Mark E. ; Oligschläger, Sabine ; Bernhardt, Boris C. ; Villringer, Arno ; Margulies, Daniel S.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c551t-af158f33109b8d39d587486e0368e66f0194be8cf26289f22997e0cf769b39653</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2017</creationdate><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Brain - anatomy &amp; histology</topic><topic>Brain - physiology</topic><topic>Brain Mapping</topic><topic>Cognition &amp; reasoning</topic><topic>Cognitive ability</topic><topic>Cognitive control</topic><topic>Cortical thickness</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Frontal gyrus</topic><topic>Frontal Lobe - anatomy &amp; histology</topic><topic>Frontal Lobe - physiology</topic><topic>functional connectivity</topic><topic>Hemispheric laterality</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Individuality</topic><topic>Integration</topic><topic>Intention</topic><topic>Intentionality</topic><topic>Intraparietal sulcus</topic><topic>Limbic Lobe - anatomy &amp; histology</topic><topic>Limbic Lobe - physiology</topic><topic>Magnetic Resonance Imaging</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Mind-wandering</topic><topic>Neural Pathways - anatomy &amp; histology</topic><topic>Neural Pathways - physiology</topic><topic>Neuroimaging</topic><topic>NMR</topic><topic>Nuclear magnetic resonance</topic><topic>Parietal Lobe - anatomy &amp; histology</topic><topic>Parietal Lobe - physiology</topic><topic>Studies</topic><topic>Temporal cortex</topic><topic>Temporal Lobe - anatomy &amp; histology</topic><topic>Temporal Lobe - physiology</topic><topic>Thinking - physiology</topic><topic>Thinning</topic><topic>Young Adult</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Golchert, Johannes</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Smallwood, Jonathan</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Jefferies, Elizabeth</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Seli, Paul</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Huntenburg, Julia M.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Liem, Franziskus</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Lauckner, Mark E.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Oligschläger, Sabine</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Bernhardt, Boris C.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Villringer, Arno</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Margulies, Daniel S.</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>ProQuest SciTech Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Health &amp; Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>Psychology Database</collection><collection>Biological Science Database</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>Biotechnology Research Abstracts</collection><jtitle>NeuroImage (Orlando, Fla.)</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Golchert, Johannes</au><au>Smallwood, Jonathan</au><au>Jefferies, Elizabeth</au><au>Seli, Paul</au><au>Huntenburg, Julia M.</au><au>Liem, Franziskus</au><au>Lauckner, Mark E.</au><au>Oligschläger, Sabine</au><au>Bernhardt, Boris C.</au><au>Villringer, Arno</au><au>Margulies, Daniel S.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Individual variation in intentionality in the mind-wandering state is reflected in the integration of the default-mode, fronto-parietal, and limbic networks</atitle><jtitle>NeuroImage (Orlando, Fla.)</jtitle><addtitle>Neuroimage</addtitle><date>2017-02-01</date><risdate>2017</risdate><volume>146</volume><spage>226</spage><epage>235</epage><pages>226-235</pages><issn>1053-8119</issn><eissn>1095-9572</eissn><abstract>Mind-wandering has a controversial relationship with cognitive control. Existing psychological evidence supports the hypothesis that episodes of mind-wandering reflect a failure to constrain thinking to task-relevant material, as well the apparently alternative view that control can facilitate the expression of self-generated mental content. We assessed whether this apparent contradiction arises because of a failure to consider differences in the types of thoughts that occur during mind-wandering, and in particular, the associated level of intentionality. Using multi-modal magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) analysis, we examined the cortical organisation that underlies inter-individual differences in descriptions of the spontaneous or deliberate nature of mind-wandering. Cortical thickness, as well as functional connectivity analyses, implicated regions relevant to cognitive control and regions of the default-mode network for individuals who reported high rates of deliberate mind-wandering. In contrast, higher reports of spontaneous mind-wandering were associated with cortical thinning in parietal and posterior temporal regions in the left hemisphere (which are important in the control of cognition and attention) as well as heightened connectivity between the intraparietal sulcus and a region that spanned limbic and default-mode regions in the ventral inferior frontal gyrus. Finally, we observed a dissociation in the thickness of the retrosplenial cortex/lingual gyrus, with higher reports of spontaneous mind-wandering being associated with thickening in the left hemisphere, and higher repots of deliberate mind-wandering with thinning in the right hemisphere. These results suggest that the intentionality of the mind-wandering state depends on integration between the control and default-mode networks, with more deliberation being associated with greater integration between these systems. We conclude that one reason why mind-wandering has a controversial relationship with control is because it depends on whether the thoughts emerge in a deliberate or spontaneous fashion. •Deliberate and spontaneous mind-wandering have unique structural and functional correlates.•Reports of deliberate mind-wandering correlated with regions in both default-mode and fronto-parietal networks.•Spontaneous mind-wandering was linked to less integrity in parietal and temporal regions.•Intentionality during the mind-wandering state may depend upon integration between the default-mode and fronto-parietal networks.•These neurocognitive differences explain why mind-wandering has a complex relationship with cognitive control.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>Elsevier Inc</pub><pmid>27864082</pmid><doi>10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.025</doi><tpages>10</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 1053-8119
ispartof NeuroImage (Orlando, Fla.), 2017-02, Vol.146, p.226-235
issn 1053-8119
1095-9572
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_1877844191
source MEDLINE; Access via ScienceDirect (Elsevier); ProQuest Central UK/Ireland
subjects Adult
Brain - anatomy & histology
Brain - physiology
Brain Mapping
Cognition & reasoning
Cognitive ability
Cognitive control
Cortical thickness
Female
Frontal gyrus
Frontal Lobe - anatomy & histology
Frontal Lobe - physiology
functional connectivity
Hemispheric laterality
Humans
Individuality
Integration
Intention
Intentionality
Intraparietal sulcus
Limbic Lobe - anatomy & histology
Limbic Lobe - physiology
Magnetic Resonance Imaging
Male
Mind-wandering
Neural Pathways - anatomy & histology
Neural Pathways - physiology
Neuroimaging
NMR
Nuclear magnetic resonance
Parietal Lobe - anatomy & histology
Parietal Lobe - physiology
Studies
Temporal cortex
Temporal Lobe - anatomy & histology
Temporal Lobe - physiology
Thinking - physiology
Thinning
Young Adult
title Individual variation in intentionality in the mind-wandering state is reflected in the integration of the default-mode, fronto-parietal, and limbic networks
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-23T11%3A21%3A02IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Individual%20variation%20in%20intentionality%20in%20the%20mind-wandering%20state%20is%20reflected%20in%20the%20integration%20of%20the%20default-mode,%20fronto-parietal,%20and%20limbic%20networks&rft.jtitle=NeuroImage%20(Orlando,%20Fla.)&rft.au=Golchert,%20Johannes&rft.date=2017-02-01&rft.volume=146&rft.spage=226&rft.epage=235&rft.pages=226-235&rft.issn=1053-8119&rft.eissn=1095-9572&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.025&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E1841793815%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1867955921&rft_id=info:pmid/27864082&rft_els_id=S1053811916306425&rfr_iscdi=true