Individual variation in intentionality in the mind-wandering state is reflected in the integration of the default-mode, fronto-parietal, and limbic networks
Mind-wandering has a controversial relationship with cognitive control. Existing psychological evidence supports the hypothesis that episodes of mind-wandering reflect a failure to constrain thinking to task-relevant material, as well the apparently alternative view that control can facilitate the e...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | NeuroImage (Orlando, Fla.) Fla.), 2017-02, Vol.146, p.226-235 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , , , , , , , , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 235 |
---|---|
container_issue | |
container_start_page | 226 |
container_title | NeuroImage (Orlando, Fla.) |
container_volume | 146 |
creator | Golchert, Johannes Smallwood, Jonathan Jefferies, Elizabeth Seli, Paul Huntenburg, Julia M. Liem, Franziskus Lauckner, Mark E. Oligschläger, Sabine Bernhardt, Boris C. Villringer, Arno Margulies, Daniel S. |
description | Mind-wandering has a controversial relationship with cognitive control. Existing psychological evidence supports the hypothesis that episodes of mind-wandering reflect a failure to constrain thinking to task-relevant material, as well the apparently alternative view that control can facilitate the expression of self-generated mental content. We assessed whether this apparent contradiction arises because of a failure to consider differences in the types of thoughts that occur during mind-wandering, and in particular, the associated level of intentionality. Using multi-modal magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) analysis, we examined the cortical organisation that underlies inter-individual differences in descriptions of the spontaneous or deliberate nature of mind-wandering. Cortical thickness, as well as functional connectivity analyses, implicated regions relevant to cognitive control and regions of the default-mode network for individuals who reported high rates of deliberate mind-wandering. In contrast, higher reports of spontaneous mind-wandering were associated with cortical thinning in parietal and posterior temporal regions in the left hemisphere (which are important in the control of cognition and attention) as well as heightened connectivity between the intraparietal sulcus and a region that spanned limbic and default-mode regions in the ventral inferior frontal gyrus. Finally, we observed a dissociation in the thickness of the retrosplenial cortex/lingual gyrus, with higher reports of spontaneous mind-wandering being associated with thickening in the left hemisphere, and higher repots of deliberate mind-wandering with thinning in the right hemisphere. These results suggest that the intentionality of the mind-wandering state depends on integration between the control and default-mode networks, with more deliberation being associated with greater integration between these systems. We conclude that one reason why mind-wandering has a controversial relationship with control is because it depends on whether the thoughts emerge in a deliberate or spontaneous fashion.
•Deliberate and spontaneous mind-wandering have unique structural and functional correlates.•Reports of deliberate mind-wandering correlated with regions in both default-mode and fronto-parietal networks.•Spontaneous mind-wandering was linked to less integrity in parietal and temporal regions.•Intentionality during the mind-wandering state may depend upon integration between the default-mode an |
doi_str_mv | 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.025 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_1877844191</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><els_id>S1053811916306425</els_id><sourcerecordid>1841793815</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c551t-af158f33109b8d39d587486e0368e66f0194be8cf26289f22997e0cf769b39653</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqNkc-O1SAUhxujcf7oKxgSNy6mV6CFwlIn6kwyiRtdE1oOV64tXIHeybzLPKzgndHEjSYkhZPv_E56vqZBBG8IJvztbuNhjcEtegsbWiobQjaYsifNKcGStZIN9Gm9s64VhMiT5iylHcZYkl48b07oIHiPBT1t7q-9cQdnVj2jg45OZxc8cvVk8PWhZ5fvaiV_A7Q4b9pb7Q1E57coZZ0BuYQi2BmmDOYRrO3beEwL9lfJgNXrnNslGLhANgafQ7svMyHr-QKVUDS7ZXQT8pBvQ_yeXjTPrJ4TvHz4njdfP374cnnV3nz-dH357qadGCO51ZYwYbuu_PooTCcNE0MvOOCOC-DcYiL7EcRkKadCWkqlHABPduBy7CRn3Xnz5pi7j-HHCimrxaUJ5ll7CGtSRAyD6HsiyX-gPRlkJ0hNff0XugtrLPusFB8kY5LWQHGkphhSKntU-1i8xjtFsKqy1U79ka2qbEWIKrJL66uHAeu4gPnd-Gi3AO-PAJTlHRxElSYHfgLjYrGlTHD_nvITYB7Bsg</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1867955921</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Individual variation in intentionality in the mind-wandering state is reflected in the integration of the default-mode, fronto-parietal, and limbic networks</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>Access via ScienceDirect (Elsevier)</source><source>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</source><creator>Golchert, Johannes ; Smallwood, Jonathan ; Jefferies, Elizabeth ; Seli, Paul ; Huntenburg, Julia M. ; Liem, Franziskus ; Lauckner, Mark E. ; Oligschläger, Sabine ; Bernhardt, Boris C. ; Villringer, Arno ; Margulies, Daniel S.</creator><creatorcontrib>Golchert, Johannes ; Smallwood, Jonathan ; Jefferies, Elizabeth ; Seli, Paul ; Huntenburg, Julia M. ; Liem, Franziskus ; Lauckner, Mark E. ; Oligschläger, Sabine ; Bernhardt, Boris C. ; Villringer, Arno ; Margulies, Daniel S.</creatorcontrib><description>Mind-wandering has a controversial relationship with cognitive control. Existing psychological evidence supports the hypothesis that episodes of mind-wandering reflect a failure to constrain thinking to task-relevant material, as well the apparently alternative view that control can facilitate the expression of self-generated mental content. We assessed whether this apparent contradiction arises because of a failure to consider differences in the types of thoughts that occur during mind-wandering, and in particular, the associated level of intentionality. Using multi-modal magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) analysis, we examined the cortical organisation that underlies inter-individual differences in descriptions of the spontaneous or deliberate nature of mind-wandering. Cortical thickness, as well as functional connectivity analyses, implicated regions relevant to cognitive control and regions of the default-mode network for individuals who reported high rates of deliberate mind-wandering. In contrast, higher reports of spontaneous mind-wandering were associated with cortical thinning in parietal and posterior temporal regions in the left hemisphere (which are important in the control of cognition and attention) as well as heightened connectivity between the intraparietal sulcus and a region that spanned limbic and default-mode regions in the ventral inferior frontal gyrus. Finally, we observed a dissociation in the thickness of the retrosplenial cortex/lingual gyrus, with higher reports of spontaneous mind-wandering being associated with thickening in the left hemisphere, and higher repots of deliberate mind-wandering with thinning in the right hemisphere. These results suggest that the intentionality of the mind-wandering state depends on integration between the control and default-mode networks, with more deliberation being associated with greater integration between these systems. We conclude that one reason why mind-wandering has a controversial relationship with control is because it depends on whether the thoughts emerge in a deliberate or spontaneous fashion.
•Deliberate and spontaneous mind-wandering have unique structural and functional correlates.•Reports of deliberate mind-wandering correlated with regions in both default-mode and fronto-parietal networks.•Spontaneous mind-wandering was linked to less integrity in parietal and temporal regions.•Intentionality during the mind-wandering state may depend upon integration between the default-mode and fronto-parietal networks.•These neurocognitive differences explain why mind-wandering has a complex relationship with cognitive control.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1053-8119</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1095-9572</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.025</identifier><identifier>PMID: 27864082</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: Elsevier Inc</publisher><subject><![CDATA[Adult ; Brain - anatomy & histology ; Brain - physiology ; Brain Mapping ; Cognition & reasoning ; Cognitive ability ; Cognitive control ; Cortical thickness ; Female ; Frontal gyrus ; Frontal Lobe - anatomy & histology ; Frontal Lobe - physiology ; functional connectivity ; Hemispheric laterality ; Humans ; Individuality ; Integration ; Intention ; Intentionality ; Intraparietal sulcus ; Limbic Lobe - anatomy & histology ; Limbic Lobe - physiology ; Magnetic Resonance Imaging ; Male ; Mind-wandering ; Neural Pathways - anatomy & histology ; Neural Pathways - physiology ; Neuroimaging ; NMR ; Nuclear magnetic resonance ; Parietal Lobe - anatomy & histology ; Parietal Lobe - physiology ; Studies ; Temporal cortex ; Temporal Lobe - anatomy & histology ; Temporal Lobe - physiology ; Thinking - physiology ; Thinning ; Young Adult]]></subject><ispartof>NeuroImage (Orlando, Fla.), 2017-02, Vol.146, p.226-235</ispartof><rights>2016 Elsevier Inc.</rights><rights>Copyright © 2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.</rights><rights>2016. Elsevier Inc.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c551t-af158f33109b8d39d587486e0368e66f0194be8cf26289f22997e0cf769b39653</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c551t-af158f33109b8d39d587486e0368e66f0194be8cf26289f22997e0cf769b39653</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/1867955921?pq-origsite=primo$$EHTML$$P50$$Gproquest$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,3550,27924,27925,45995,64385,64387,64389,72469</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27864082$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Golchert, Johannes</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Smallwood, Jonathan</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Jefferies, Elizabeth</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Seli, Paul</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Huntenburg, Julia M.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Liem, Franziskus</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Lauckner, Mark E.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Oligschläger, Sabine</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Bernhardt, Boris C.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Villringer, Arno</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Margulies, Daniel S.</creatorcontrib><title>Individual variation in intentionality in the mind-wandering state is reflected in the integration of the default-mode, fronto-parietal, and limbic networks</title><title>NeuroImage (Orlando, Fla.)</title><addtitle>Neuroimage</addtitle><description>Mind-wandering has a controversial relationship with cognitive control. Existing psychological evidence supports the hypothesis that episodes of mind-wandering reflect a failure to constrain thinking to task-relevant material, as well the apparently alternative view that control can facilitate the expression of self-generated mental content. We assessed whether this apparent contradiction arises because of a failure to consider differences in the types of thoughts that occur during mind-wandering, and in particular, the associated level of intentionality. Using multi-modal magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) analysis, we examined the cortical organisation that underlies inter-individual differences in descriptions of the spontaneous or deliberate nature of mind-wandering. Cortical thickness, as well as functional connectivity analyses, implicated regions relevant to cognitive control and regions of the default-mode network for individuals who reported high rates of deliberate mind-wandering. In contrast, higher reports of spontaneous mind-wandering were associated with cortical thinning in parietal and posterior temporal regions in the left hemisphere (which are important in the control of cognition and attention) as well as heightened connectivity between the intraparietal sulcus and a region that spanned limbic and default-mode regions in the ventral inferior frontal gyrus. Finally, we observed a dissociation in the thickness of the retrosplenial cortex/lingual gyrus, with higher reports of spontaneous mind-wandering being associated with thickening in the left hemisphere, and higher repots of deliberate mind-wandering with thinning in the right hemisphere. These results suggest that the intentionality of the mind-wandering state depends on integration between the control and default-mode networks, with more deliberation being associated with greater integration between these systems. We conclude that one reason why mind-wandering has a controversial relationship with control is because it depends on whether the thoughts emerge in a deliberate or spontaneous fashion.
•Deliberate and spontaneous mind-wandering have unique structural and functional correlates.•Reports of deliberate mind-wandering correlated with regions in both default-mode and fronto-parietal networks.•Spontaneous mind-wandering was linked to less integrity in parietal and temporal regions.•Intentionality during the mind-wandering state may depend upon integration between the default-mode and fronto-parietal networks.•These neurocognitive differences explain why mind-wandering has a complex relationship with cognitive control.</description><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Brain - anatomy & histology</subject><subject>Brain - physiology</subject><subject>Brain Mapping</subject><subject>Cognition & reasoning</subject><subject>Cognitive ability</subject><subject>Cognitive control</subject><subject>Cortical thickness</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Frontal gyrus</subject><subject>Frontal Lobe - anatomy & histology</subject><subject>Frontal Lobe - physiology</subject><subject>functional connectivity</subject><subject>Hemispheric laterality</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Individuality</subject><subject>Integration</subject><subject>Intention</subject><subject>Intentionality</subject><subject>Intraparietal sulcus</subject><subject>Limbic Lobe - anatomy & histology</subject><subject>Limbic Lobe - physiology</subject><subject>Magnetic Resonance Imaging</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Mind-wandering</subject><subject>Neural Pathways - anatomy & histology</subject><subject>Neural Pathways - physiology</subject><subject>Neuroimaging</subject><subject>NMR</subject><subject>Nuclear magnetic resonance</subject><subject>Parietal Lobe - anatomy & histology</subject><subject>Parietal Lobe - physiology</subject><subject>Studies</subject><subject>Temporal cortex</subject><subject>Temporal Lobe - anatomy & histology</subject><subject>Temporal Lobe - physiology</subject><subject>Thinking - physiology</subject><subject>Thinning</subject><subject>Young Adult</subject><issn>1053-8119</issn><issn>1095-9572</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2017</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>ABUWG</sourceid><sourceid>AFKRA</sourceid><sourceid>AZQEC</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>CCPQU</sourceid><sourceid>DWQXO</sourceid><sourceid>GNUQQ</sourceid><recordid>eNqNkc-O1SAUhxujcf7oKxgSNy6mV6CFwlIn6kwyiRtdE1oOV64tXIHeybzLPKzgndHEjSYkhZPv_E56vqZBBG8IJvztbuNhjcEtegsbWiobQjaYsifNKcGStZIN9Gm9s64VhMiT5iylHcZYkl48b07oIHiPBT1t7q-9cQdnVj2jg45OZxc8cvVk8PWhZ5fvaiV_A7Q4b9pb7Q1E57coZZ0BuYQi2BmmDOYRrO3beEwL9lfJgNXrnNslGLhANgafQ7svMyHr-QKVUDS7ZXQT8pBvQ_yeXjTPrJ4TvHz4njdfP374cnnV3nz-dH357qadGCO51ZYwYbuu_PooTCcNE0MvOOCOC-DcYiL7EcRkKadCWkqlHABPduBy7CRn3Xnz5pi7j-HHCimrxaUJ5ll7CGtSRAyD6HsiyX-gPRlkJ0hNff0XugtrLPusFB8kY5LWQHGkphhSKntU-1i8xjtFsKqy1U79ka2qbEWIKrJL66uHAeu4gPnd-Gi3AO-PAJTlHRxElSYHfgLjYrGlTHD_nvITYB7Bsg</recordid><startdate>20170201</startdate><enddate>20170201</enddate><creator>Golchert, Johannes</creator><creator>Smallwood, Jonathan</creator><creator>Jefferies, Elizabeth</creator><creator>Seli, Paul</creator><creator>Huntenburg, Julia M.</creator><creator>Liem, Franziskus</creator><creator>Lauckner, Mark E.</creator><creator>Oligschläger, Sabine</creator><creator>Bernhardt, Boris C.</creator><creator>Villringer, Arno</creator><creator>Margulies, Daniel S.</creator><general>Elsevier Inc</general><general>Elsevier Limited</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FE</scope><scope>8FH</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BBNVY</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BHPHI</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>LK8</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>M7P</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>7QO</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20170201</creationdate><title>Individual variation in intentionality in the mind-wandering state is reflected in the integration of the default-mode, fronto-parietal, and limbic networks</title><author>Golchert, Johannes ; Smallwood, Jonathan ; Jefferies, Elizabeth ; Seli, Paul ; Huntenburg, Julia M. ; Liem, Franziskus ; Lauckner, Mark E. ; Oligschläger, Sabine ; Bernhardt, Boris C. ; Villringer, Arno ; Margulies, Daniel S.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c551t-af158f33109b8d39d587486e0368e66f0194be8cf26289f22997e0cf769b39653</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2017</creationdate><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Brain - anatomy & histology</topic><topic>Brain - physiology</topic><topic>Brain Mapping</topic><topic>Cognition & reasoning</topic><topic>Cognitive ability</topic><topic>Cognitive control</topic><topic>Cortical thickness</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Frontal gyrus</topic><topic>Frontal Lobe - anatomy & histology</topic><topic>Frontal Lobe - physiology</topic><topic>functional connectivity</topic><topic>Hemispheric laterality</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Individuality</topic><topic>Integration</topic><topic>Intention</topic><topic>Intentionality</topic><topic>Intraparietal sulcus</topic><topic>Limbic Lobe - anatomy & histology</topic><topic>Limbic Lobe - physiology</topic><topic>Magnetic Resonance Imaging</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Mind-wandering</topic><topic>Neural Pathways - anatomy & histology</topic><topic>Neural Pathways - physiology</topic><topic>Neuroimaging</topic><topic>NMR</topic><topic>Nuclear magnetic resonance</topic><topic>Parietal Lobe - anatomy & histology</topic><topic>Parietal Lobe - physiology</topic><topic>Studies</topic><topic>Temporal cortex</topic><topic>Temporal Lobe - anatomy & histology</topic><topic>Temporal Lobe - physiology</topic><topic>Thinking - physiology</topic><topic>Thinning</topic><topic>Young Adult</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Golchert, Johannes</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Smallwood, Jonathan</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Jefferies, Elizabeth</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Seli, Paul</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Huntenburg, Julia M.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Liem, Franziskus</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Lauckner, Mark E.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Oligschläger, Sabine</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Bernhardt, Boris C.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Villringer, Arno</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Margulies, Daniel S.</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>ProQuest SciTech Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>Psychology Database</collection><collection>Biological Science Database</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>Biotechnology Research Abstracts</collection><jtitle>NeuroImage (Orlando, Fla.)</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Golchert, Johannes</au><au>Smallwood, Jonathan</au><au>Jefferies, Elizabeth</au><au>Seli, Paul</au><au>Huntenburg, Julia M.</au><au>Liem, Franziskus</au><au>Lauckner, Mark E.</au><au>Oligschläger, Sabine</au><au>Bernhardt, Boris C.</au><au>Villringer, Arno</au><au>Margulies, Daniel S.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Individual variation in intentionality in the mind-wandering state is reflected in the integration of the default-mode, fronto-parietal, and limbic networks</atitle><jtitle>NeuroImage (Orlando, Fla.)</jtitle><addtitle>Neuroimage</addtitle><date>2017-02-01</date><risdate>2017</risdate><volume>146</volume><spage>226</spage><epage>235</epage><pages>226-235</pages><issn>1053-8119</issn><eissn>1095-9572</eissn><abstract>Mind-wandering has a controversial relationship with cognitive control. Existing psychological evidence supports the hypothesis that episodes of mind-wandering reflect a failure to constrain thinking to task-relevant material, as well the apparently alternative view that control can facilitate the expression of self-generated mental content. We assessed whether this apparent contradiction arises because of a failure to consider differences in the types of thoughts that occur during mind-wandering, and in particular, the associated level of intentionality. Using multi-modal magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) analysis, we examined the cortical organisation that underlies inter-individual differences in descriptions of the spontaneous or deliberate nature of mind-wandering. Cortical thickness, as well as functional connectivity analyses, implicated regions relevant to cognitive control and regions of the default-mode network for individuals who reported high rates of deliberate mind-wandering. In contrast, higher reports of spontaneous mind-wandering were associated with cortical thinning in parietal and posterior temporal regions in the left hemisphere (which are important in the control of cognition and attention) as well as heightened connectivity between the intraparietal sulcus and a region that spanned limbic and default-mode regions in the ventral inferior frontal gyrus. Finally, we observed a dissociation in the thickness of the retrosplenial cortex/lingual gyrus, with higher reports of spontaneous mind-wandering being associated with thickening in the left hemisphere, and higher repots of deliberate mind-wandering with thinning in the right hemisphere. These results suggest that the intentionality of the mind-wandering state depends on integration between the control and default-mode networks, with more deliberation being associated with greater integration between these systems. We conclude that one reason why mind-wandering has a controversial relationship with control is because it depends on whether the thoughts emerge in a deliberate or spontaneous fashion.
•Deliberate and spontaneous mind-wandering have unique structural and functional correlates.•Reports of deliberate mind-wandering correlated with regions in both default-mode and fronto-parietal networks.•Spontaneous mind-wandering was linked to less integrity in parietal and temporal regions.•Intentionality during the mind-wandering state may depend upon integration between the default-mode and fronto-parietal networks.•These neurocognitive differences explain why mind-wandering has a complex relationship with cognitive control.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>Elsevier Inc</pub><pmid>27864082</pmid><doi>10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.025</doi><tpages>10</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 1053-8119 |
ispartof | NeuroImage (Orlando, Fla.), 2017-02, Vol.146, p.226-235 |
issn | 1053-8119 1095-9572 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_1877844191 |
source | MEDLINE; Access via ScienceDirect (Elsevier); ProQuest Central UK/Ireland |
subjects | Adult Brain - anatomy & histology Brain - physiology Brain Mapping Cognition & reasoning Cognitive ability Cognitive control Cortical thickness Female Frontal gyrus Frontal Lobe - anatomy & histology Frontal Lobe - physiology functional connectivity Hemispheric laterality Humans Individuality Integration Intention Intentionality Intraparietal sulcus Limbic Lobe - anatomy & histology Limbic Lobe - physiology Magnetic Resonance Imaging Male Mind-wandering Neural Pathways - anatomy & histology Neural Pathways - physiology Neuroimaging NMR Nuclear magnetic resonance Parietal Lobe - anatomy & histology Parietal Lobe - physiology Studies Temporal cortex Temporal Lobe - anatomy & histology Temporal Lobe - physiology Thinking - physiology Thinning Young Adult |
title | Individual variation in intentionality in the mind-wandering state is reflected in the integration of the default-mode, fronto-parietal, and limbic networks |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-23T11%3A21%3A02IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Individual%20variation%20in%20intentionality%20in%20the%20mind-wandering%20state%20is%20reflected%20in%20the%20integration%20of%20the%20default-mode,%20fronto-parietal,%20and%20limbic%20networks&rft.jtitle=NeuroImage%20(Orlando,%20Fla.)&rft.au=Golchert,%20Johannes&rft.date=2017-02-01&rft.volume=146&rft.spage=226&rft.epage=235&rft.pages=226-235&rft.issn=1053-8119&rft.eissn=1095-9572&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.025&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E1841793815%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1867955921&rft_id=info:pmid/27864082&rft_els_id=S1053811916306425&rfr_iscdi=true |