Sleep and performance in simulated Navy watch schedules
•We compared sleep, vigilance and sleepiness in simulated Navy watch schedules.•Sleep opportunities were limited; average sleep duration was 5.6h per watch day.•Sleep restriction resulted in build-up of vigilance impairment across watch days.•Vigilance was more variable in the 5/15 than in the 3/9 w...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Accident analysis and prevention 2017-02, Vol.99 (Pt B), p.422-427 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , , , , , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 427 |
---|---|
container_issue | Pt B |
container_start_page | 422 |
container_title | Accident analysis and prevention |
container_volume | 99 |
creator | Skornyakov, Elena Shattuck, Nita L. Winser, Michael A. Matsangas, Panagiotis Sparrow, Amy R. Layton, Matthew E. Gabehart, Rylie J. Van Dongen, Hans P.A. |
description | •We compared sleep, vigilance and sleepiness in simulated Navy watch schedules.•Sleep opportunities were limited; average sleep duration was 5.6h per watch day.•Sleep restriction resulted in build-up of vigilance impairment across watch days.•Vigilance was more variable in the 5/15 than in the 3/9 watch schedule.•Subjective sleepiness did not accurately reflect vigilance impairment.
To operate Navy ships 24h per day, watchstanding is needed around the clock, with watch periods reflecting a variety of rotating or fixed shift schedules. The 5/15 watch schedule cycles through watch periods with 5h on, 15h off watch, such that watches occur 4h earlier on the clock each day – that is, the watches rotate backward. The timing of sleep varies over 4-day cycles, and sleep is split on some days to accommodate nighttime watchstanding. The 3/9 watch schedule cycles through watch periods with 3h on, 9h off watch, allowing for consistent sleep timing over days. In some sections of the 3/9 watch schedule, sleep may need to be split to accommodate nighttime watchstanding. In both the 5/15 and 3/9 watch schedules, four watch sections alternate to cover the 24h of the day. Here we compared sleep duration, psychomotor vigilance and subjective sleepiness in simulated sections of the 5/15 and 3/9 watch schedules. Fifteen healthy male subjects spent 6 consecutive days (5 nights) in the laboratory. Sleep opportunities were restricted to an average of 6.5h daily. Actigraphically estimated sleep duration was 5.6h per watch day on average, with no significant difference between watch sections. Sleep duration was not reduced when sleep opportunities were split. Psychomotor vigilance degraded over watch days, and tended to be more variable in the 5/15 than in the 3/9 watch sections. These laboratory-based findings suggest that Navy watch schedules are associated with cumulative sleep loss and a build-up of fatigue across days. The fixed watch periods of the 3/9 watch schedule appear to yield more stable performance than the backward rotating watch periods of the 5/15 watch schedule. Optimal performance may require longer and more consistent daily opportunities for sleep than are typically obtained in Navy operations. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1016/j.aap.2015.11.021 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_1826646274</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><els_id>S0001457515301366</els_id><sourcerecordid>1826646274</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c396t-b2f25f9cd39bd942eba226c085a414f125a9c0f6fa17c81e646ee0d50327dd7c3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9kEtLxDAUhYMozjj6A9xIl25ac9MmaXEl4gsGXajrkCa3TIe-TNqR-fdmmNGlq8uF7xw4HyGXQBOgIG7WidZDwijwBCChDI7IHHJZxIxyeUzmlFKIMy75jJx5vw6vzCU_JTMmRBEasjmR7w3iEOnORgO6qnet7gxGdRf5up0aPaKNXvVmG33r0awib1Zopwb9OTmpdOPx4nAX5PPx4eP-OV6-Pb3c3y1jkxZijEtWMV4VxqZFaYuMYakZE4bmXGeQVcC4LgytRKVBmhxQZAKRWk5TJq2VJl2Q633v4PqvCf2o2tobbBrdYT95BXnYkgkms4DCHjWu995hpQZXt9ptFVC186XWKvhSO18KQAVfIXN1qJ_KFu1f4ldQAG73AIaRmxqd8qbGoMjWDs2obF__U_8DMsZ6Kw</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1826646274</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Sleep and performance in simulated Navy watch schedules</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals</source><creator>Skornyakov, Elena ; Shattuck, Nita L. ; Winser, Michael A. ; Matsangas, Panagiotis ; Sparrow, Amy R. ; Layton, Matthew E. ; Gabehart, Rylie J. ; Van Dongen, Hans P.A.</creator><creatorcontrib>Skornyakov, Elena ; Shattuck, Nita L. ; Winser, Michael A. ; Matsangas, Panagiotis ; Sparrow, Amy R. ; Layton, Matthew E. ; Gabehart, Rylie J. ; Van Dongen, Hans P.A.</creatorcontrib><description>•We compared sleep, vigilance and sleepiness in simulated Navy watch schedules.•Sleep opportunities were limited; average sleep duration was 5.6h per watch day.•Sleep restriction resulted in build-up of vigilance impairment across watch days.•Vigilance was more variable in the 5/15 than in the 3/9 watch schedule.•Subjective sleepiness did not accurately reflect vigilance impairment.
To operate Navy ships 24h per day, watchstanding is needed around the clock, with watch periods reflecting a variety of rotating or fixed shift schedules. The 5/15 watch schedule cycles through watch periods with 5h on, 15h off watch, such that watches occur 4h earlier on the clock each day – that is, the watches rotate backward. The timing of sleep varies over 4-day cycles, and sleep is split on some days to accommodate nighttime watchstanding. The 3/9 watch schedule cycles through watch periods with 3h on, 9h off watch, allowing for consistent sleep timing over days. In some sections of the 3/9 watch schedule, sleep may need to be split to accommodate nighttime watchstanding. In both the 5/15 and 3/9 watch schedules, four watch sections alternate to cover the 24h of the day. Here we compared sleep duration, psychomotor vigilance and subjective sleepiness in simulated sections of the 5/15 and 3/9 watch schedules. Fifteen healthy male subjects spent 6 consecutive days (5 nights) in the laboratory. Sleep opportunities were restricted to an average of 6.5h daily. Actigraphically estimated sleep duration was 5.6h per watch day on average, with no significant difference between watch sections. Sleep duration was not reduced when sleep opportunities were split. Psychomotor vigilance degraded over watch days, and tended to be more variable in the 5/15 than in the 3/9 watch sections. These laboratory-based findings suggest that Navy watch schedules are associated with cumulative sleep loss and a build-up of fatigue across days. The fixed watch periods of the 3/9 watch schedule appear to yield more stable performance than the backward rotating watch periods of the 5/15 watch schedule. Optimal performance may require longer and more consistent daily opportunities for sleep than are typically obtained in Navy operations.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0001-4575</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1879-2057</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2015.11.021</identifier><identifier>PMID: 26691014</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>England: Elsevier Ltd</publisher><subject>Adult ; Circadian Rhythm ; Fatigue ; Fatigue - physiopathology ; Humans ; Male ; Psychomotor Performance - physiology ; Shift work ; Ships ; Sleep - physiology ; Sleep Disorders, Circadian Rhythm ; Sleep homeostasis ; Sleep Initiation and Maintenance Disorders ; Sleep restriction ; Sleep Stages ; Wake maintenance zone ; Wakefulness ; Work Schedule Tolerance - physiology</subject><ispartof>Accident analysis and prevention, 2017-02, Vol.99 (Pt B), p.422-427</ispartof><rights>2015 Elsevier Ltd</rights><rights>Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c396t-b2f25f9cd39bd942eba226c085a414f125a9c0f6fa17c81e646ee0d50327dd7c3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c396t-b2f25f9cd39bd942eba226c085a414f125a9c0f6fa17c81e646ee0d50327dd7c3</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0001457515301366$$EHTML$$P50$$Gelsevier$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,3536,27903,27904,65309</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26691014$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Skornyakov, Elena</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Shattuck, Nita L.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Winser, Michael A.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Matsangas, Panagiotis</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Sparrow, Amy R.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Layton, Matthew E.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Gabehart, Rylie J.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Van Dongen, Hans P.A.</creatorcontrib><title>Sleep and performance in simulated Navy watch schedules</title><title>Accident analysis and prevention</title><addtitle>Accid Anal Prev</addtitle><description>•We compared sleep, vigilance and sleepiness in simulated Navy watch schedules.•Sleep opportunities were limited; average sleep duration was 5.6h per watch day.•Sleep restriction resulted in build-up of vigilance impairment across watch days.•Vigilance was more variable in the 5/15 than in the 3/9 watch schedule.•Subjective sleepiness did not accurately reflect vigilance impairment.
To operate Navy ships 24h per day, watchstanding is needed around the clock, with watch periods reflecting a variety of rotating or fixed shift schedules. The 5/15 watch schedule cycles through watch periods with 5h on, 15h off watch, such that watches occur 4h earlier on the clock each day – that is, the watches rotate backward. The timing of sleep varies over 4-day cycles, and sleep is split on some days to accommodate nighttime watchstanding. The 3/9 watch schedule cycles through watch periods with 3h on, 9h off watch, allowing for consistent sleep timing over days. In some sections of the 3/9 watch schedule, sleep may need to be split to accommodate nighttime watchstanding. In both the 5/15 and 3/9 watch schedules, four watch sections alternate to cover the 24h of the day. Here we compared sleep duration, psychomotor vigilance and subjective sleepiness in simulated sections of the 5/15 and 3/9 watch schedules. Fifteen healthy male subjects spent 6 consecutive days (5 nights) in the laboratory. Sleep opportunities were restricted to an average of 6.5h daily. Actigraphically estimated sleep duration was 5.6h per watch day on average, with no significant difference between watch sections. Sleep duration was not reduced when sleep opportunities were split. Psychomotor vigilance degraded over watch days, and tended to be more variable in the 5/15 than in the 3/9 watch sections. These laboratory-based findings suggest that Navy watch schedules are associated with cumulative sleep loss and a build-up of fatigue across days. The fixed watch periods of the 3/9 watch schedule appear to yield more stable performance than the backward rotating watch periods of the 5/15 watch schedule. Optimal performance may require longer and more consistent daily opportunities for sleep than are typically obtained in Navy operations.</description><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Circadian Rhythm</subject><subject>Fatigue</subject><subject>Fatigue - physiopathology</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Psychomotor Performance - physiology</subject><subject>Shift work</subject><subject>Ships</subject><subject>Sleep - physiology</subject><subject>Sleep Disorders, Circadian Rhythm</subject><subject>Sleep homeostasis</subject><subject>Sleep Initiation and Maintenance Disorders</subject><subject>Sleep restriction</subject><subject>Sleep Stages</subject><subject>Wake maintenance zone</subject><subject>Wakefulness</subject><subject>Work Schedule Tolerance - physiology</subject><issn>0001-4575</issn><issn>1879-2057</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2017</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><recordid>eNp9kEtLxDAUhYMozjj6A9xIl25ac9MmaXEl4gsGXajrkCa3TIe-TNqR-fdmmNGlq8uF7xw4HyGXQBOgIG7WidZDwijwBCChDI7IHHJZxIxyeUzmlFKIMy75jJx5vw6vzCU_JTMmRBEasjmR7w3iEOnORgO6qnet7gxGdRf5up0aPaKNXvVmG33r0awib1Zopwb9OTmpdOPx4nAX5PPx4eP-OV6-Pb3c3y1jkxZijEtWMV4VxqZFaYuMYakZE4bmXGeQVcC4LgytRKVBmhxQZAKRWk5TJq2VJl2Q633v4PqvCf2o2tobbBrdYT95BXnYkgkms4DCHjWu995hpQZXt9ptFVC186XWKvhSO18KQAVfIXN1qJ_KFu1f4ldQAG73AIaRmxqd8qbGoMjWDs2obF__U_8DMsZ6Kw</recordid><startdate>201702</startdate><enddate>201702</enddate><creator>Skornyakov, Elena</creator><creator>Shattuck, Nita L.</creator><creator>Winser, Michael A.</creator><creator>Matsangas, Panagiotis</creator><creator>Sparrow, Amy R.</creator><creator>Layton, Matthew E.</creator><creator>Gabehart, Rylie J.</creator><creator>Van Dongen, Hans P.A.</creator><general>Elsevier Ltd</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>201702</creationdate><title>Sleep and performance in simulated Navy watch schedules</title><author>Skornyakov, Elena ; Shattuck, Nita L. ; Winser, Michael A. ; Matsangas, Panagiotis ; Sparrow, Amy R. ; Layton, Matthew E. ; Gabehart, Rylie J. ; Van Dongen, Hans P.A.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c396t-b2f25f9cd39bd942eba226c085a414f125a9c0f6fa17c81e646ee0d50327dd7c3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2017</creationdate><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Circadian Rhythm</topic><topic>Fatigue</topic><topic>Fatigue - physiopathology</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Psychomotor Performance - physiology</topic><topic>Shift work</topic><topic>Ships</topic><topic>Sleep - physiology</topic><topic>Sleep Disorders, Circadian Rhythm</topic><topic>Sleep homeostasis</topic><topic>Sleep Initiation and Maintenance Disorders</topic><topic>Sleep restriction</topic><topic>Sleep Stages</topic><topic>Wake maintenance zone</topic><topic>Wakefulness</topic><topic>Work Schedule Tolerance - physiology</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Skornyakov, Elena</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Shattuck, Nita L.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Winser, Michael A.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Matsangas, Panagiotis</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Sparrow, Amy R.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Layton, Matthew E.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Gabehart, Rylie J.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Van Dongen, Hans P.A.</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Accident analysis and prevention</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Skornyakov, Elena</au><au>Shattuck, Nita L.</au><au>Winser, Michael A.</au><au>Matsangas, Panagiotis</au><au>Sparrow, Amy R.</au><au>Layton, Matthew E.</au><au>Gabehart, Rylie J.</au><au>Van Dongen, Hans P.A.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Sleep and performance in simulated Navy watch schedules</atitle><jtitle>Accident analysis and prevention</jtitle><addtitle>Accid Anal Prev</addtitle><date>2017-02</date><risdate>2017</risdate><volume>99</volume><issue>Pt B</issue><spage>422</spage><epage>427</epage><pages>422-427</pages><issn>0001-4575</issn><eissn>1879-2057</eissn><abstract>•We compared sleep, vigilance and sleepiness in simulated Navy watch schedules.•Sleep opportunities were limited; average sleep duration was 5.6h per watch day.•Sleep restriction resulted in build-up of vigilance impairment across watch days.•Vigilance was more variable in the 5/15 than in the 3/9 watch schedule.•Subjective sleepiness did not accurately reflect vigilance impairment.
To operate Navy ships 24h per day, watchstanding is needed around the clock, with watch periods reflecting a variety of rotating or fixed shift schedules. The 5/15 watch schedule cycles through watch periods with 5h on, 15h off watch, such that watches occur 4h earlier on the clock each day – that is, the watches rotate backward. The timing of sleep varies over 4-day cycles, and sleep is split on some days to accommodate nighttime watchstanding. The 3/9 watch schedule cycles through watch periods with 3h on, 9h off watch, allowing for consistent sleep timing over days. In some sections of the 3/9 watch schedule, sleep may need to be split to accommodate nighttime watchstanding. In both the 5/15 and 3/9 watch schedules, four watch sections alternate to cover the 24h of the day. Here we compared sleep duration, psychomotor vigilance and subjective sleepiness in simulated sections of the 5/15 and 3/9 watch schedules. Fifteen healthy male subjects spent 6 consecutive days (5 nights) in the laboratory. Sleep opportunities were restricted to an average of 6.5h daily. Actigraphically estimated sleep duration was 5.6h per watch day on average, with no significant difference between watch sections. Sleep duration was not reduced when sleep opportunities were split. Psychomotor vigilance degraded over watch days, and tended to be more variable in the 5/15 than in the 3/9 watch sections. These laboratory-based findings suggest that Navy watch schedules are associated with cumulative sleep loss and a build-up of fatigue across days. The fixed watch periods of the 3/9 watch schedule appear to yield more stable performance than the backward rotating watch periods of the 5/15 watch schedule. Optimal performance may require longer and more consistent daily opportunities for sleep than are typically obtained in Navy operations.</abstract><cop>England</cop><pub>Elsevier Ltd</pub><pmid>26691014</pmid><doi>10.1016/j.aap.2015.11.021</doi><tpages>6</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0001-4575 |
ispartof | Accident analysis and prevention, 2017-02, Vol.99 (Pt B), p.422-427 |
issn | 0001-4575 1879-2057 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_1826646274 |
source | MEDLINE; Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals |
subjects | Adult Circadian Rhythm Fatigue Fatigue - physiopathology Humans Male Psychomotor Performance - physiology Shift work Ships Sleep - physiology Sleep Disorders, Circadian Rhythm Sleep homeostasis Sleep Initiation and Maintenance Disorders Sleep restriction Sleep Stages Wake maintenance zone Wakefulness Work Schedule Tolerance - physiology |
title | Sleep and performance in simulated Navy watch schedules |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-25T00%3A49%3A54IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Sleep%20and%20performance%20in%20simulated%20Navy%20watch%20schedules&rft.jtitle=Accident%20analysis%20and%20prevention&rft.au=Skornyakov,%20Elena&rft.date=2017-02&rft.volume=99&rft.issue=Pt%20B&rft.spage=422&rft.epage=427&rft.pages=422-427&rft.issn=0001-4575&rft.eissn=1879-2057&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/j.aap.2015.11.021&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E1826646274%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1826646274&rft_id=info:pmid/26691014&rft_els_id=S0001457515301366&rfr_iscdi=true |