Relative errors can cue absolute visuomotor mappings

When repeatedly switching between two visuomotor mappings, e.g. in a reaching or pointing task, adaptation tends to speed up over time. That is, when the error in the feedback corresponds to a mapping switch, fast adaptation occurs. Yet, what is learned, the relative error or the absolute mappings?...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Experimental brain research 2015-12, Vol.233 (12), p.3367-3377
Hauptverfasser: van Dam, Loes C. J., Ernst, Marc O.
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 3377
container_issue 12
container_start_page 3367
container_title Experimental brain research
container_volume 233
creator van Dam, Loes C. J.
Ernst, Marc O.
description When repeatedly switching between two visuomotor mappings, e.g. in a reaching or pointing task, adaptation tends to speed up over time. That is, when the error in the feedback corresponds to a mapping switch, fast adaptation occurs. Yet, what is learned, the relative error or the absolute mappings? When switching between mappings, errors with a size corresponding to the relative difference between the mappings will occur more often than other large errors. Thus, we could learn to correct more for errors with this familiar size (Error Learning). On the other hand, it has been shown that the human visuomotor system can store several absolute visuomotor mappings (Mapping Learning) and can use associated contextual cues to retrieve them. Thus, when contextual information is present, no error feedback is needed to switch between mappings. Using a rapid pointing task, we investigated how these two types of learning may each contribute when repeatedly switching between mappings in the absence of task-irrelevant contextual cues. After training, we examined how participants changed their behaviour when a single error probe indicated either the often-experienced error (Error Learning) or one of the previously experienced absolute mappings (Mapping Learning). Results were consistent with Mapping Learning despite the relative nature of the error information in the feedback. This shows that errors in the feedback can have a double role in visuomotor behaviour: they drive the general adaptation process by making corrections possible on subsequent movements, as well as serve as contextual cues that can signal a learned absolute mapping.
doi_str_mv 10.1007/s00221-015-4403-9
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>gale_proqu</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_1751217255</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><galeid>A434834649</galeid><sourcerecordid>A434834649</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c607t-8a87b620ac8a906f5b53c5cccc4ab48aa668326903d92236811d1e7a2b5ebadf3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqNkt1r1TAYxoMo7jj9A7yRgiB60Zk3X00vx_BjMBCmXoe36duzjrY5Ju3Q_97UM3VHFEwuQpLf80CePIw9BX4CnFevE-dCQMlBl0pxWdb32AaUFCUAN_fZhnNQpbJQH7FHKV2vW1nxh-xIGGG5BL1h6pIGnPsbKijGEFPhcSr8QgU2KQzLTMVNn5YwhjnEYsTdrp-26TF70OGQ6Mntesw-v33z6ex9efHh3fnZ6UXpDa_m0qKtGiM4eos1N51utPTa56GwURbRGCuFqblsayGksQAtUIWi0dRg28lj9nLvu4vhy0JpdmOfPA0DThSW5KDSIKASWv8HKpXISWjI6PM_0OuwxCk_ZKWkklbU8je1xYFcP3VhjuhXU3eqpLJSGVVn6uQvVJ4tjb0PE3V9Pj8QvDoQZGamr_MWl5Tc-cfLQ_bFHfaKcJivfnxKH6Z0CMIe9DGkFKlzu9iPGL854G5tits3xeWmuLUpbtU8u01haUZqfyl-ViMDYg-kfDVtKd6J6Z-u3wEPj8NE</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1733438293</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Relative errors can cue absolute visuomotor mappings</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>SpringerLink Journals - AutoHoldings</source><creator>van Dam, Loes C. J. ; Ernst, Marc O.</creator><creatorcontrib>van Dam, Loes C. J. ; Ernst, Marc O.</creatorcontrib><description>When repeatedly switching between two visuomotor mappings, e.g. in a reaching or pointing task, adaptation tends to speed up over time. That is, when the error in the feedback corresponds to a mapping switch, fast adaptation occurs. Yet, what is learned, the relative error or the absolute mappings? When switching between mappings, errors with a size corresponding to the relative difference between the mappings will occur more often than other large errors. Thus, we could learn to correct more for errors with this familiar size (Error Learning). On the other hand, it has been shown that the human visuomotor system can store several absolute visuomotor mappings (Mapping Learning) and can use associated contextual cues to retrieve them. Thus, when contextual information is present, no error feedback is needed to switch between mappings. Using a rapid pointing task, we investigated how these two types of learning may each contribute when repeatedly switching between mappings in the absence of task-irrelevant contextual cues. After training, we examined how participants changed their behaviour when a single error probe indicated either the often-experienced error (Error Learning) or one of the previously experienced absolute mappings (Mapping Learning). Results were consistent with Mapping Learning despite the relative nature of the error information in the feedback. This shows that errors in the feedback can have a double role in visuomotor behaviour: they drive the general adaptation process by making corrections possible on subsequent movements, as well as serve as contextual cues that can signal a learned absolute mapping.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0014-4819</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1432-1106</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1007/s00221-015-4403-9</identifier><identifier>PMID: 26280315</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg</publisher><subject>Adaptation ; Adaptation, Physiological - physiology ; Adult ; Biomedical and Life Sciences ; Biomedicine ; Brain mapping ; Cognitive learning ; Cues ; Executive Function - physiology ; Feedback, Sensory - physiology ; Humans ; Learning ; Learning - physiology ; Neurology ; Neurosciences ; Physiological aspects ; Psychomotor Performance - physiology ; Research Article ; Young Adult</subject><ispartof>Experimental brain research, 2015-12, Vol.233 (12), p.3367-3377</ispartof><rights>Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2015</rights><rights>COPYRIGHT 2015 Springer</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c607t-8a87b620ac8a906f5b53c5cccc4ab48aa668326903d92236811d1e7a2b5ebadf3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c607t-8a87b620ac8a906f5b53c5cccc4ab48aa668326903d92236811d1e7a2b5ebadf3</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s00221-015-4403-9$$EPDF$$P50$$Gspringer$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://link.springer.com/10.1007/s00221-015-4403-9$$EHTML$$P50$$Gspringer$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,27901,27902,41464,42533,51294</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26280315$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>van Dam, Loes C. J.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Ernst, Marc O.</creatorcontrib><title>Relative errors can cue absolute visuomotor mappings</title><title>Experimental brain research</title><addtitle>Exp Brain Res</addtitle><addtitle>Exp Brain Res</addtitle><description>When repeatedly switching between two visuomotor mappings, e.g. in a reaching or pointing task, adaptation tends to speed up over time. That is, when the error in the feedback corresponds to a mapping switch, fast adaptation occurs. Yet, what is learned, the relative error or the absolute mappings? When switching between mappings, errors with a size corresponding to the relative difference between the mappings will occur more often than other large errors. Thus, we could learn to correct more for errors with this familiar size (Error Learning). On the other hand, it has been shown that the human visuomotor system can store several absolute visuomotor mappings (Mapping Learning) and can use associated contextual cues to retrieve them. Thus, when contextual information is present, no error feedback is needed to switch between mappings. Using a rapid pointing task, we investigated how these two types of learning may each contribute when repeatedly switching between mappings in the absence of task-irrelevant contextual cues. After training, we examined how participants changed their behaviour when a single error probe indicated either the often-experienced error (Error Learning) or one of the previously experienced absolute mappings (Mapping Learning). Results were consistent with Mapping Learning despite the relative nature of the error information in the feedback. This shows that errors in the feedback can have a double role in visuomotor behaviour: they drive the general adaptation process by making corrections possible on subsequent movements, as well as serve as contextual cues that can signal a learned absolute mapping.</description><subject>Adaptation</subject><subject>Adaptation, Physiological - physiology</subject><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Biomedical and Life Sciences</subject><subject>Biomedicine</subject><subject>Brain mapping</subject><subject>Cognitive learning</subject><subject>Cues</subject><subject>Executive Function - physiology</subject><subject>Feedback, Sensory - physiology</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Learning</subject><subject>Learning - physiology</subject><subject>Neurology</subject><subject>Neurosciences</subject><subject>Physiological aspects</subject><subject>Psychomotor Performance - physiology</subject><subject>Research Article</subject><subject>Young Adult</subject><issn>0014-4819</issn><issn>1432-1106</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2015</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><recordid>eNqNkt1r1TAYxoMo7jj9A7yRgiB60Zk3X00vx_BjMBCmXoe36duzjrY5Ju3Q_97UM3VHFEwuQpLf80CePIw9BX4CnFevE-dCQMlBl0pxWdb32AaUFCUAN_fZhnNQpbJQH7FHKV2vW1nxh-xIGGG5BL1h6pIGnPsbKijGEFPhcSr8QgU2KQzLTMVNn5YwhjnEYsTdrp-26TF70OGQ6Mntesw-v33z6ex9efHh3fnZ6UXpDa_m0qKtGiM4eos1N51utPTa56GwURbRGCuFqblsayGksQAtUIWi0dRg28lj9nLvu4vhy0JpdmOfPA0DThSW5KDSIKASWv8HKpXISWjI6PM_0OuwxCk_ZKWkklbU8je1xYFcP3VhjuhXU3eqpLJSGVVn6uQvVJ4tjb0PE3V9Pj8QvDoQZGamr_MWl5Tc-cfLQ_bFHfaKcJivfnxKH6Z0CMIe9DGkFKlzu9iPGL854G5tits3xeWmuLUpbtU8u01haUZqfyl-ViMDYg-kfDVtKd6J6Z-u3wEPj8NE</recordid><startdate>20151201</startdate><enddate>20151201</enddate><creator>van Dam, Loes C. J.</creator><creator>Ernst, Marc O.</creator><general>Springer Berlin Heidelberg</general><general>Springer</general><general>Springer Nature B.V</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>ISR</scope><scope>0-V</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QP</scope><scope>7QR</scope><scope>7RV</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>7TM</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>88J</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ALSLI</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>KB0</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>M2R</scope><scope>NAPCQ</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20151201</creationdate><title>Relative errors can cue absolute visuomotor mappings</title><author>van Dam, Loes C. J. ; Ernst, Marc O.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c607t-8a87b620ac8a906f5b53c5cccc4ab48aa668326903d92236811d1e7a2b5ebadf3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2015</creationdate><topic>Adaptation</topic><topic>Adaptation, Physiological - physiology</topic><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Biomedical and Life Sciences</topic><topic>Biomedicine</topic><topic>Brain mapping</topic><topic>Cognitive learning</topic><topic>Cues</topic><topic>Executive Function - physiology</topic><topic>Feedback, Sensory - physiology</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Learning</topic><topic>Learning - physiology</topic><topic>Neurology</topic><topic>Neurosciences</topic><topic>Physiological aspects</topic><topic>Psychomotor Performance - physiology</topic><topic>Research Article</topic><topic>Young Adult</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>van Dam, Loes C. J.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Ernst, Marc O.</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Gale In Context: Science</collection><collection>ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Calcium &amp; Calcified Tissue Abstracts</collection><collection>Chemoreception Abstracts</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Database</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>Nucleic Acids Abstracts</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>Social Science Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>Social Science Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>ProQuest Health &amp; Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Psychology</collection><collection>Social Science Database</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Premium</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Experimental brain research</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>van Dam, Loes C. J.</au><au>Ernst, Marc O.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Relative errors can cue absolute visuomotor mappings</atitle><jtitle>Experimental brain research</jtitle><stitle>Exp Brain Res</stitle><addtitle>Exp Brain Res</addtitle><date>2015-12-01</date><risdate>2015</risdate><volume>233</volume><issue>12</issue><spage>3367</spage><epage>3377</epage><pages>3367-3377</pages><issn>0014-4819</issn><eissn>1432-1106</eissn><abstract>When repeatedly switching between two visuomotor mappings, e.g. in a reaching or pointing task, adaptation tends to speed up over time. That is, when the error in the feedback corresponds to a mapping switch, fast adaptation occurs. Yet, what is learned, the relative error or the absolute mappings? When switching between mappings, errors with a size corresponding to the relative difference between the mappings will occur more often than other large errors. Thus, we could learn to correct more for errors with this familiar size (Error Learning). On the other hand, it has been shown that the human visuomotor system can store several absolute visuomotor mappings (Mapping Learning) and can use associated contextual cues to retrieve them. Thus, when contextual information is present, no error feedback is needed to switch between mappings. Using a rapid pointing task, we investigated how these two types of learning may each contribute when repeatedly switching between mappings in the absence of task-irrelevant contextual cues. After training, we examined how participants changed their behaviour when a single error probe indicated either the often-experienced error (Error Learning) or one of the previously experienced absolute mappings (Mapping Learning). Results were consistent with Mapping Learning despite the relative nature of the error information in the feedback. This shows that errors in the feedback can have a double role in visuomotor behaviour: they drive the general adaptation process by making corrections possible on subsequent movements, as well as serve as contextual cues that can signal a learned absolute mapping.</abstract><cop>Berlin/Heidelberg</cop><pub>Springer Berlin Heidelberg</pub><pmid>26280315</pmid><doi>10.1007/s00221-015-4403-9</doi><tpages>11</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0014-4819
ispartof Experimental brain research, 2015-12, Vol.233 (12), p.3367-3377
issn 0014-4819
1432-1106
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_1751217255
source MEDLINE; SpringerLink Journals - AutoHoldings
subjects Adaptation
Adaptation, Physiological - physiology
Adult
Biomedical and Life Sciences
Biomedicine
Brain mapping
Cognitive learning
Cues
Executive Function - physiology
Feedback, Sensory - physiology
Humans
Learning
Learning - physiology
Neurology
Neurosciences
Physiological aspects
Psychomotor Performance - physiology
Research Article
Young Adult
title Relative errors can cue absolute visuomotor mappings
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-09T12%3A52%3A41IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-gale_proqu&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Relative%20errors%20can%20cue%20absolute%20visuomotor%20mappings&rft.jtitle=Experimental%20brain%20research&rft.au=van%20Dam,%20Loes%20C.%20J.&rft.date=2015-12-01&rft.volume=233&rft.issue=12&rft.spage=3367&rft.epage=3377&rft.pages=3367-3377&rft.issn=0014-4819&rft.eissn=1432-1106&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007/s00221-015-4403-9&rft_dat=%3Cgale_proqu%3EA434834649%3C/gale_proqu%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1733438293&rft_id=info:pmid/26280315&rft_galeid=A434834649&rfr_iscdi=true