Naturalistic quantification of canine olfactory sensitivity
Despite the large and growing dependence on dog-handler teams for solving “real world” problems of odor detection, recognition and localization, no comprehensive methodology for quantifying the capabilities of such teams has been reported. We developed an approach in which each dog is housed with it...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Applied animal behaviour science 2006-05, Vol.97 (2), p.241-254 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , , , , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 254 |
---|---|
container_issue | 2 |
container_start_page | 241 |
container_title | Applied animal behaviour science |
container_volume | 97 |
creator | Walker, Dianne Beidler Walker, James Cornelius Cavnar, Peter James Taylor, Jennifer Leigh Pickel, Duane Howard Hall, Sandra Biddle Suarez, Joseph Carlos |
description | Despite the large and growing dependence on dog-handler teams for solving “real world” problems of odor detection, recognition and localization, no comprehensive methodology for quantifying the capabilities of such teams has been reported. We developed an approach in which each dog is housed with its owner-handler, deprived of neither food nor water and indicates to its handler which of five Teflon boxes contains the target odorant of
n-amyl acetate (
nAA). In two dogs (Rottweiler, Standard Schnauzer) trained in this way, precisely controlled concentration ranges of
nAA were systematically lowered over the course of several weeks, in blocks of three 9-trial sessions, until chance performance was seen. Data for each concentration were expressed in terms of a logistic regression equation relating concentration to the binomial probability that the observed performance (or better) would be seen by chance alone. That concentration corresponding to a probability of 0.05 was defined as threshold. In this admittedly small sample, the values we obtained (1.9 and 1.14
ppt) are roughly 30- to 20,000-fold lower than the range of thresholds reported by Krestel et al. [Krestel, D., Passe, D., Smith, J.C., Jonsson, L., 1984. Behavioral determination of olfactory thresholds to amyl acetate in dogs. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 8, 169–174] in their conditioned suppression study of beagle sensitivity to
nAA. Thus, it appears that there are significant advantages to our approach, though the reasons for differences in results are unclear. The “find the target” aspect of this new method makes it readily applicable to odor processing tasks much more complex than detection of single compounds. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1016/j.applanim.2005.07.009 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_17157449</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><els_id>S0168159105002194</els_id><sourcerecordid>17157449</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c367t-a27e72a99309146c15ed86205eeeb334fd91c76d1c337243147e601aeb2aaf633</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkD1PwzAQhi0EEqXwFyATW8JdnMSNWEAVX1IFA3S2XOeMXKVxaruV-u9JFZiZbnne9-4exq4RMgSs7taZ6vtWdXaT5QBlBiIDqE_YBGciT2soylM2GcBZimWN5-wihDUMIEeYsPt3FXdetTZEq5PtTnXRGqtVtK5LnEn00NtR4lqjdHT-kATqgo12b-Phkp0Z1Qa6-p1Ttnx--pq_pouPl7f54yLVvBIxVbkgkau65lBjUWksqZlVOZREtOK8ME2NWlQNas5FXnAsBFWAila5UqbifMpux97eu-2OQpQbGzS1w8_kdkGiwFIURT2A1Qhq70LwZGTv7Ub5g0SQR1dyLf9cyaMrCUIOrobgzRg0ykn17W2Qy88ckAOCKLg43vAwEjQ8urfkZdCWOk2N9aSjbJz9b8kPZhp_rA</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>17157449</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Naturalistic quantification of canine olfactory sensitivity</title><source>Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals</source><creator>Walker, Dianne Beidler ; Walker, James Cornelius ; Cavnar, Peter James ; Taylor, Jennifer Leigh ; Pickel, Duane Howard ; Hall, Sandra Biddle ; Suarez, Joseph Carlos</creator><creatorcontrib>Walker, Dianne Beidler ; Walker, James Cornelius ; Cavnar, Peter James ; Taylor, Jennifer Leigh ; Pickel, Duane Howard ; Hall, Sandra Biddle ; Suarez, Joseph Carlos</creatorcontrib><description>Despite the large and growing dependence on dog-handler teams for solving “real world” problems of odor detection, recognition and localization, no comprehensive methodology for quantifying the capabilities of such teams has been reported. We developed an approach in which each dog is housed with its owner-handler, deprived of neither food nor water and indicates to its handler which of five Teflon boxes contains the target odorant of
n-amyl acetate (
nAA). In two dogs (Rottweiler, Standard Schnauzer) trained in this way, precisely controlled concentration ranges of
nAA were systematically lowered over the course of several weeks, in blocks of three 9-trial sessions, until chance performance was seen. Data for each concentration were expressed in terms of a logistic regression equation relating concentration to the binomial probability that the observed performance (or better) would be seen by chance alone. That concentration corresponding to a probability of 0.05 was defined as threshold. In this admittedly small sample, the values we obtained (1.9 and 1.14
ppt) are roughly 30- to 20,000-fold lower than the range of thresholds reported by Krestel et al. [Krestel, D., Passe, D., Smith, J.C., Jonsson, L., 1984. Behavioral determination of olfactory thresholds to amyl acetate in dogs. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 8, 169–174] in their conditioned suppression study of beagle sensitivity to
nAA. Thus, it appears that there are significant advantages to our approach, though the reasons for differences in results are unclear. The “find the target” aspect of this new method makes it readily applicable to odor processing tasks much more complex than detection of single compounds.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0168-1591</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1872-9045</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/j.applanim.2005.07.009</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Elsevier B.V</publisher><subject>accuracy ; Acetate ; chemical concentration ; detection ; Dog ; dogs ; n-Amyl ; Naturalistic ; odor compounds ; Olfactory ; smell ; Threshold ; training (animals)</subject><ispartof>Applied animal behaviour science, 2006-05, Vol.97 (2), p.241-254</ispartof><rights>2005 Elsevier B.V.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c367t-a27e72a99309146c15ed86205eeeb334fd91c76d1c337243147e601aeb2aaf633</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c367t-a27e72a99309146c15ed86205eeeb334fd91c76d1c337243147e601aeb2aaf633</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2005.07.009$$EHTML$$P50$$Gelsevier$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,777,781,3537,27905,27906,45976</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Walker, Dianne Beidler</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Walker, James Cornelius</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Cavnar, Peter James</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Taylor, Jennifer Leigh</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Pickel, Duane Howard</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Hall, Sandra Biddle</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Suarez, Joseph Carlos</creatorcontrib><title>Naturalistic quantification of canine olfactory sensitivity</title><title>Applied animal behaviour science</title><description>Despite the large and growing dependence on dog-handler teams for solving “real world” problems of odor detection, recognition and localization, no comprehensive methodology for quantifying the capabilities of such teams has been reported. We developed an approach in which each dog is housed with its owner-handler, deprived of neither food nor water and indicates to its handler which of five Teflon boxes contains the target odorant of
n-amyl acetate (
nAA). In two dogs (Rottweiler, Standard Schnauzer) trained in this way, precisely controlled concentration ranges of
nAA were systematically lowered over the course of several weeks, in blocks of three 9-trial sessions, until chance performance was seen. Data for each concentration were expressed in terms of a logistic regression equation relating concentration to the binomial probability that the observed performance (or better) would be seen by chance alone. That concentration corresponding to a probability of 0.05 was defined as threshold. In this admittedly small sample, the values we obtained (1.9 and 1.14
ppt) are roughly 30- to 20,000-fold lower than the range of thresholds reported by Krestel et al. [Krestel, D., Passe, D., Smith, J.C., Jonsson, L., 1984. Behavioral determination of olfactory thresholds to amyl acetate in dogs. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 8, 169–174] in their conditioned suppression study of beagle sensitivity to
nAA. Thus, it appears that there are significant advantages to our approach, though the reasons for differences in results are unclear. The “find the target” aspect of this new method makes it readily applicable to odor processing tasks much more complex than detection of single compounds.</description><subject>accuracy</subject><subject>Acetate</subject><subject>chemical concentration</subject><subject>detection</subject><subject>Dog</subject><subject>dogs</subject><subject>n-Amyl</subject><subject>Naturalistic</subject><subject>odor compounds</subject><subject>Olfactory</subject><subject>smell</subject><subject>Threshold</subject><subject>training (animals)</subject><issn>0168-1591</issn><issn>1872-9045</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2006</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><recordid>eNqFkD1PwzAQhi0EEqXwFyATW8JdnMSNWEAVX1IFA3S2XOeMXKVxaruV-u9JFZiZbnne9-4exq4RMgSs7taZ6vtWdXaT5QBlBiIDqE_YBGciT2soylM2GcBZimWN5-wihDUMIEeYsPt3FXdetTZEq5PtTnXRGqtVtK5LnEn00NtR4lqjdHT-kATqgo12b-Phkp0Z1Qa6-p1Ttnx--pq_pouPl7f54yLVvBIxVbkgkau65lBjUWksqZlVOZREtOK8ME2NWlQNas5FXnAsBFWAila5UqbifMpux97eu-2OQpQbGzS1w8_kdkGiwFIURT2A1Qhq70LwZGTv7Ub5g0SQR1dyLf9cyaMrCUIOrobgzRg0ykn17W2Qy88ckAOCKLg43vAwEjQ8urfkZdCWOk2N9aSjbJz9b8kPZhp_rA</recordid><startdate>20060501</startdate><enddate>20060501</enddate><creator>Walker, Dianne Beidler</creator><creator>Walker, James Cornelius</creator><creator>Cavnar, Peter James</creator><creator>Taylor, Jennifer Leigh</creator><creator>Pickel, Duane Howard</creator><creator>Hall, Sandra Biddle</creator><creator>Suarez, Joseph Carlos</creator><general>Elsevier B.V</general><scope>FBQ</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7QG</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20060501</creationdate><title>Naturalistic quantification of canine olfactory sensitivity</title><author>Walker, Dianne Beidler ; Walker, James Cornelius ; Cavnar, Peter James ; Taylor, Jennifer Leigh ; Pickel, Duane Howard ; Hall, Sandra Biddle ; Suarez, Joseph Carlos</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c367t-a27e72a99309146c15ed86205eeeb334fd91c76d1c337243147e601aeb2aaf633</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2006</creationdate><topic>accuracy</topic><topic>Acetate</topic><topic>chemical concentration</topic><topic>detection</topic><topic>Dog</topic><topic>dogs</topic><topic>n-Amyl</topic><topic>Naturalistic</topic><topic>odor compounds</topic><topic>Olfactory</topic><topic>smell</topic><topic>Threshold</topic><topic>training (animals)</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Walker, Dianne Beidler</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Walker, James Cornelius</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Cavnar, Peter James</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Taylor, Jennifer Leigh</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Pickel, Duane Howard</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Hall, Sandra Biddle</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Suarez, Joseph Carlos</creatorcontrib><collection>AGRIS</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><jtitle>Applied animal behaviour science</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Walker, Dianne Beidler</au><au>Walker, James Cornelius</au><au>Cavnar, Peter James</au><au>Taylor, Jennifer Leigh</au><au>Pickel, Duane Howard</au><au>Hall, Sandra Biddle</au><au>Suarez, Joseph Carlos</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Naturalistic quantification of canine olfactory sensitivity</atitle><jtitle>Applied animal behaviour science</jtitle><date>2006-05-01</date><risdate>2006</risdate><volume>97</volume><issue>2</issue><spage>241</spage><epage>254</epage><pages>241-254</pages><issn>0168-1591</issn><eissn>1872-9045</eissn><abstract>Despite the large and growing dependence on dog-handler teams for solving “real world” problems of odor detection, recognition and localization, no comprehensive methodology for quantifying the capabilities of such teams has been reported. We developed an approach in which each dog is housed with its owner-handler, deprived of neither food nor water and indicates to its handler which of five Teflon boxes contains the target odorant of
n-amyl acetate (
nAA). In two dogs (Rottweiler, Standard Schnauzer) trained in this way, precisely controlled concentration ranges of
nAA were systematically lowered over the course of several weeks, in blocks of three 9-trial sessions, until chance performance was seen. Data for each concentration were expressed in terms of a logistic regression equation relating concentration to the binomial probability that the observed performance (or better) would be seen by chance alone. That concentration corresponding to a probability of 0.05 was defined as threshold. In this admittedly small sample, the values we obtained (1.9 and 1.14
ppt) are roughly 30- to 20,000-fold lower than the range of thresholds reported by Krestel et al. [Krestel, D., Passe, D., Smith, J.C., Jonsson, L., 1984. Behavioral determination of olfactory thresholds to amyl acetate in dogs. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 8, 169–174] in their conditioned suppression study of beagle sensitivity to
nAA. Thus, it appears that there are significant advantages to our approach, though the reasons for differences in results are unclear. The “find the target” aspect of this new method makes it readily applicable to odor processing tasks much more complex than detection of single compounds.</abstract><pub>Elsevier B.V</pub><doi>10.1016/j.applanim.2005.07.009</doi><tpages>14</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0168-1591 |
ispartof | Applied animal behaviour science, 2006-05, Vol.97 (2), p.241-254 |
issn | 0168-1591 1872-9045 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_17157449 |
source | Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals |
subjects | accuracy Acetate chemical concentration detection Dog dogs n-Amyl Naturalistic odor compounds Olfactory smell Threshold training (animals) |
title | Naturalistic quantification of canine olfactory sensitivity |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-18T14%3A24%3A09IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Naturalistic%20quantification%20of%20canine%20olfactory%20sensitivity&rft.jtitle=Applied%20animal%20behaviour%20science&rft.au=Walker,%20Dianne%20Beidler&rft.date=2006-05-01&rft.volume=97&rft.issue=2&rft.spage=241&rft.epage=254&rft.pages=241-254&rft.issn=0168-1591&rft.eissn=1872-9045&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/j.applanim.2005.07.009&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E17157449%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=17157449&rft_id=info:pmid/&rft_els_id=S0168159105002194&rfr_iscdi=true |