Naturalistic quantification of canine olfactory sensitivity

Despite the large and growing dependence on dog-handler teams for solving “real world” problems of odor detection, recognition and localization, no comprehensive methodology for quantifying the capabilities of such teams has been reported. We developed an approach in which each dog is housed with it...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Applied animal behaviour science 2006-05, Vol.97 (2), p.241-254
Hauptverfasser: Walker, Dianne Beidler, Walker, James Cornelius, Cavnar, Peter James, Taylor, Jennifer Leigh, Pickel, Duane Howard, Hall, Sandra Biddle, Suarez, Joseph Carlos
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 254
container_issue 2
container_start_page 241
container_title Applied animal behaviour science
container_volume 97
creator Walker, Dianne Beidler
Walker, James Cornelius
Cavnar, Peter James
Taylor, Jennifer Leigh
Pickel, Duane Howard
Hall, Sandra Biddle
Suarez, Joseph Carlos
description Despite the large and growing dependence on dog-handler teams for solving “real world” problems of odor detection, recognition and localization, no comprehensive methodology for quantifying the capabilities of such teams has been reported. We developed an approach in which each dog is housed with its owner-handler, deprived of neither food nor water and indicates to its handler which of five Teflon boxes contains the target odorant of n-amyl acetate ( nAA). In two dogs (Rottweiler, Standard Schnauzer) trained in this way, precisely controlled concentration ranges of nAA were systematically lowered over the course of several weeks, in blocks of three 9-trial sessions, until chance performance was seen. Data for each concentration were expressed in terms of a logistic regression equation relating concentration to the binomial probability that the observed performance (or better) would be seen by chance alone. That concentration corresponding to a probability of 0.05 was defined as threshold. In this admittedly small sample, the values we obtained (1.9 and 1.14 ppt) are roughly 30- to 20,000-fold lower than the range of thresholds reported by Krestel et al. [Krestel, D., Passe, D., Smith, J.C., Jonsson, L., 1984. Behavioral determination of olfactory thresholds to amyl acetate in dogs. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 8, 169–174] in their conditioned suppression study of beagle sensitivity to nAA. Thus, it appears that there are significant advantages to our approach, though the reasons for differences in results are unclear. The “find the target” aspect of this new method makes it readily applicable to odor processing tasks much more complex than detection of single compounds.
doi_str_mv 10.1016/j.applanim.2005.07.009
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_17157449</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><els_id>S0168159105002194</els_id><sourcerecordid>17157449</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c367t-a27e72a99309146c15ed86205eeeb334fd91c76d1c337243147e601aeb2aaf633</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkD1PwzAQhi0EEqXwFyATW8JdnMSNWEAVX1IFA3S2XOeMXKVxaruV-u9JFZiZbnne9-4exq4RMgSs7taZ6vtWdXaT5QBlBiIDqE_YBGciT2soylM2GcBZimWN5-wihDUMIEeYsPt3FXdetTZEq5PtTnXRGqtVtK5LnEn00NtR4lqjdHT-kATqgo12b-Phkp0Z1Qa6-p1Ttnx--pq_pouPl7f54yLVvBIxVbkgkau65lBjUWksqZlVOZREtOK8ME2NWlQNas5FXnAsBFWAila5UqbifMpux97eu-2OQpQbGzS1w8_kdkGiwFIURT2A1Qhq70LwZGTv7Ub5g0SQR1dyLf9cyaMrCUIOrobgzRg0ykn17W2Qy88ckAOCKLg43vAwEjQ8urfkZdCWOk2N9aSjbJz9b8kPZhp_rA</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>17157449</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Naturalistic quantification of canine olfactory sensitivity</title><source>Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals</source><creator>Walker, Dianne Beidler ; Walker, James Cornelius ; Cavnar, Peter James ; Taylor, Jennifer Leigh ; Pickel, Duane Howard ; Hall, Sandra Biddle ; Suarez, Joseph Carlos</creator><creatorcontrib>Walker, Dianne Beidler ; Walker, James Cornelius ; Cavnar, Peter James ; Taylor, Jennifer Leigh ; Pickel, Duane Howard ; Hall, Sandra Biddle ; Suarez, Joseph Carlos</creatorcontrib><description>Despite the large and growing dependence on dog-handler teams for solving “real world” problems of odor detection, recognition and localization, no comprehensive methodology for quantifying the capabilities of such teams has been reported. We developed an approach in which each dog is housed with its owner-handler, deprived of neither food nor water and indicates to its handler which of five Teflon boxes contains the target odorant of n-amyl acetate ( nAA). In two dogs (Rottweiler, Standard Schnauzer) trained in this way, precisely controlled concentration ranges of nAA were systematically lowered over the course of several weeks, in blocks of three 9-trial sessions, until chance performance was seen. Data for each concentration were expressed in terms of a logistic regression equation relating concentration to the binomial probability that the observed performance (or better) would be seen by chance alone. That concentration corresponding to a probability of 0.05 was defined as threshold. In this admittedly small sample, the values we obtained (1.9 and 1.14 ppt) are roughly 30- to 20,000-fold lower than the range of thresholds reported by Krestel et al. [Krestel, D., Passe, D., Smith, J.C., Jonsson, L., 1984. Behavioral determination of olfactory thresholds to amyl acetate in dogs. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 8, 169–174] in their conditioned suppression study of beagle sensitivity to nAA. Thus, it appears that there are significant advantages to our approach, though the reasons for differences in results are unclear. The “find the target” aspect of this new method makes it readily applicable to odor processing tasks much more complex than detection of single compounds.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0168-1591</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1872-9045</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/j.applanim.2005.07.009</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Elsevier B.V</publisher><subject>accuracy ; Acetate ; chemical concentration ; detection ; Dog ; dogs ; n-Amyl ; Naturalistic ; odor compounds ; Olfactory ; smell ; Threshold ; training (animals)</subject><ispartof>Applied animal behaviour science, 2006-05, Vol.97 (2), p.241-254</ispartof><rights>2005 Elsevier B.V.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c367t-a27e72a99309146c15ed86205eeeb334fd91c76d1c337243147e601aeb2aaf633</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c367t-a27e72a99309146c15ed86205eeeb334fd91c76d1c337243147e601aeb2aaf633</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2005.07.009$$EHTML$$P50$$Gelsevier$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,777,781,3537,27905,27906,45976</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Walker, Dianne Beidler</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Walker, James Cornelius</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Cavnar, Peter James</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Taylor, Jennifer Leigh</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Pickel, Duane Howard</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Hall, Sandra Biddle</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Suarez, Joseph Carlos</creatorcontrib><title>Naturalistic quantification of canine olfactory sensitivity</title><title>Applied animal behaviour science</title><description>Despite the large and growing dependence on dog-handler teams for solving “real world” problems of odor detection, recognition and localization, no comprehensive methodology for quantifying the capabilities of such teams has been reported. We developed an approach in which each dog is housed with its owner-handler, deprived of neither food nor water and indicates to its handler which of five Teflon boxes contains the target odorant of n-amyl acetate ( nAA). In two dogs (Rottweiler, Standard Schnauzer) trained in this way, precisely controlled concentration ranges of nAA were systematically lowered over the course of several weeks, in blocks of three 9-trial sessions, until chance performance was seen. Data for each concentration were expressed in terms of a logistic regression equation relating concentration to the binomial probability that the observed performance (or better) would be seen by chance alone. That concentration corresponding to a probability of 0.05 was defined as threshold. In this admittedly small sample, the values we obtained (1.9 and 1.14 ppt) are roughly 30- to 20,000-fold lower than the range of thresholds reported by Krestel et al. [Krestel, D., Passe, D., Smith, J.C., Jonsson, L., 1984. Behavioral determination of olfactory thresholds to amyl acetate in dogs. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 8, 169–174] in their conditioned suppression study of beagle sensitivity to nAA. Thus, it appears that there are significant advantages to our approach, though the reasons for differences in results are unclear. The “find the target” aspect of this new method makes it readily applicable to odor processing tasks much more complex than detection of single compounds.</description><subject>accuracy</subject><subject>Acetate</subject><subject>chemical concentration</subject><subject>detection</subject><subject>Dog</subject><subject>dogs</subject><subject>n-Amyl</subject><subject>Naturalistic</subject><subject>odor compounds</subject><subject>Olfactory</subject><subject>smell</subject><subject>Threshold</subject><subject>training (animals)</subject><issn>0168-1591</issn><issn>1872-9045</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2006</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><recordid>eNqFkD1PwzAQhi0EEqXwFyATW8JdnMSNWEAVX1IFA3S2XOeMXKVxaruV-u9JFZiZbnne9-4exq4RMgSs7taZ6vtWdXaT5QBlBiIDqE_YBGciT2soylM2GcBZimWN5-wihDUMIEeYsPt3FXdetTZEq5PtTnXRGqtVtK5LnEn00NtR4lqjdHT-kATqgo12b-Phkp0Z1Qa6-p1Ttnx--pq_pouPl7f54yLVvBIxVbkgkau65lBjUWksqZlVOZREtOK8ME2NWlQNas5FXnAsBFWAila5UqbifMpux97eu-2OQpQbGzS1w8_kdkGiwFIURT2A1Qhq70LwZGTv7Ub5g0SQR1dyLf9cyaMrCUIOrobgzRg0ykn17W2Qy88ckAOCKLg43vAwEjQ8urfkZdCWOk2N9aSjbJz9b8kPZhp_rA</recordid><startdate>20060501</startdate><enddate>20060501</enddate><creator>Walker, Dianne Beidler</creator><creator>Walker, James Cornelius</creator><creator>Cavnar, Peter James</creator><creator>Taylor, Jennifer Leigh</creator><creator>Pickel, Duane Howard</creator><creator>Hall, Sandra Biddle</creator><creator>Suarez, Joseph Carlos</creator><general>Elsevier B.V</general><scope>FBQ</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7QG</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20060501</creationdate><title>Naturalistic quantification of canine olfactory sensitivity</title><author>Walker, Dianne Beidler ; Walker, James Cornelius ; Cavnar, Peter James ; Taylor, Jennifer Leigh ; Pickel, Duane Howard ; Hall, Sandra Biddle ; Suarez, Joseph Carlos</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c367t-a27e72a99309146c15ed86205eeeb334fd91c76d1c337243147e601aeb2aaf633</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2006</creationdate><topic>accuracy</topic><topic>Acetate</topic><topic>chemical concentration</topic><topic>detection</topic><topic>Dog</topic><topic>dogs</topic><topic>n-Amyl</topic><topic>Naturalistic</topic><topic>odor compounds</topic><topic>Olfactory</topic><topic>smell</topic><topic>Threshold</topic><topic>training (animals)</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Walker, Dianne Beidler</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Walker, James Cornelius</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Cavnar, Peter James</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Taylor, Jennifer Leigh</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Pickel, Duane Howard</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Hall, Sandra Biddle</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Suarez, Joseph Carlos</creatorcontrib><collection>AGRIS</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><jtitle>Applied animal behaviour science</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Walker, Dianne Beidler</au><au>Walker, James Cornelius</au><au>Cavnar, Peter James</au><au>Taylor, Jennifer Leigh</au><au>Pickel, Duane Howard</au><au>Hall, Sandra Biddle</au><au>Suarez, Joseph Carlos</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Naturalistic quantification of canine olfactory sensitivity</atitle><jtitle>Applied animal behaviour science</jtitle><date>2006-05-01</date><risdate>2006</risdate><volume>97</volume><issue>2</issue><spage>241</spage><epage>254</epage><pages>241-254</pages><issn>0168-1591</issn><eissn>1872-9045</eissn><abstract>Despite the large and growing dependence on dog-handler teams for solving “real world” problems of odor detection, recognition and localization, no comprehensive methodology for quantifying the capabilities of such teams has been reported. We developed an approach in which each dog is housed with its owner-handler, deprived of neither food nor water and indicates to its handler which of five Teflon boxes contains the target odorant of n-amyl acetate ( nAA). In two dogs (Rottweiler, Standard Schnauzer) trained in this way, precisely controlled concentration ranges of nAA were systematically lowered over the course of several weeks, in blocks of three 9-trial sessions, until chance performance was seen. Data for each concentration were expressed in terms of a logistic regression equation relating concentration to the binomial probability that the observed performance (or better) would be seen by chance alone. That concentration corresponding to a probability of 0.05 was defined as threshold. In this admittedly small sample, the values we obtained (1.9 and 1.14 ppt) are roughly 30- to 20,000-fold lower than the range of thresholds reported by Krestel et al. [Krestel, D., Passe, D., Smith, J.C., Jonsson, L., 1984. Behavioral determination of olfactory thresholds to amyl acetate in dogs. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 8, 169–174] in their conditioned suppression study of beagle sensitivity to nAA. Thus, it appears that there are significant advantages to our approach, though the reasons for differences in results are unclear. The “find the target” aspect of this new method makes it readily applicable to odor processing tasks much more complex than detection of single compounds.</abstract><pub>Elsevier B.V</pub><doi>10.1016/j.applanim.2005.07.009</doi><tpages>14</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0168-1591
ispartof Applied animal behaviour science, 2006-05, Vol.97 (2), p.241-254
issn 0168-1591
1872-9045
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_17157449
source Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals
subjects accuracy
Acetate
chemical concentration
detection
Dog
dogs
n-Amyl
Naturalistic
odor compounds
Olfactory
smell
Threshold
training (animals)
title Naturalistic quantification of canine olfactory sensitivity
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-18T14%3A24%3A09IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Naturalistic%20quantification%20of%20canine%20olfactory%20sensitivity&rft.jtitle=Applied%20animal%20behaviour%20science&rft.au=Walker,%20Dianne%20Beidler&rft.date=2006-05-01&rft.volume=97&rft.issue=2&rft.spage=241&rft.epage=254&rft.pages=241-254&rft.issn=0168-1591&rft.eissn=1872-9045&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/j.applanim.2005.07.009&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E17157449%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=17157449&rft_id=info:pmid/&rft_els_id=S0168159105002194&rfr_iscdi=true