Startle and Habituation Responses of Blue Jays (Cyanocitta Cristata) in a Laboratory Simulation of Anti-Predator Defenses of Catocala Moths (Lepidoptera: Noctuidae)
AbstractBlue jays were used in laboratory experiments to determine what visual characteristics of Catocala hindwings are important in eliciting and maintaining startle reactions in avian predators. Hand-reared blue jays, inexperienced with warningly coloured, inedible prey, took significantly longer...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Behaviour 1993, Vol.126 (1-2), p.77-95 |
---|---|
1. Verfasser: | |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 95 |
---|---|
container_issue | 1-2 |
container_start_page | 77 |
container_title | Behaviour |
container_volume | 126 |
creator | Ingalls, Victoria |
description | AbstractBlue jays were used in laboratory experiments to determine what visual characteristics of Catocala hindwings are important in eliciting and maintaining startle reactions in avian predators. Hand-reared blue jays, inexperienced with warningly coloured, inedible prey, took significantly longer to touch novel colours that possessed bold, black bands than to touch novel, unbanded colours when these discs covered a food reward. Thus, being conspicuous (as opposed to simply being novel) appears to enhance startle reactions. Bold patterns are commonly found on aposematic prey and Catocala hindwings. These same hand-reared birds also tended to take longer to touch novel coloured discs of yellow and red hues as opposed to discs of blue, green, or purple hues. The colours that produced the greatest hesitation are similar to the colours found on Catocala hindwings. Red-yellow colours are also characteristic of aposematic prey. Wild-caught birds took longer to habituate to startling stimuli when presented with several different startling disc types during the habituation process. Thus, predator pressure may explain why several different Catocala species with different hindwing colours occur sympatrically. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1163/156853993X00353 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>jstor_proqu</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_17090565</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><jstor_id>4535125</jstor_id><sourcerecordid>4535125</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-b339t-7d53169d3d605804a3b5e68563dd99a24ed6a9b2d6eb01188edceafa902ceb8a3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp1kUFv1DAQhSMEEkvhzIWDJRCih1A7jpOYW0lbSgkF0QJ7sybxRHjJxsF2JPb_8EPxasuCKnGyPO-bp5l5SfKY0ZeMFfyIiaISXEq-pJQLfidZbCtpLC3vJgtKqUhLKeT95IH3q_gtBReL5NdVABcGJDBqcg6tCTMEY0fyCf1kR4-e2J68HmYkF7Dx5EW9gdF2JgQgtTM-QIBDYkYCpIHWOgjWbciVWc_Dzid2H4_BpB8d6q1ITrDHP751rHQwAHlvw7do3uBktJ0COnhFLm0XZqMBDx8m93oYPD66eQ-Sz2en1_V52nx487Y-btKWcxnSUgvOCqm5LqioaA68FRiPUnCtpYQsR12AbDNdYEsZqyrUHUIPkmYdthXwg-T5zndy9seMPqi18R0OA4xoZ69YSSUVhYjg01vgys5ujLMpximvMl7keaSOdlTnrPcOezU5swa3UYyqbWbqVmax49mNL_h4l97B2Bm_b-NVnomcRuzJDlv5eNG9nMdEWbadLt3JMR78uZfBfVdFyUuhLt5dq-asWV5-_UJV_ZdvnRmGf1b5z5S_Aez3vZI</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1303823644</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Startle and Habituation Responses of Blue Jays (Cyanocitta Cristata) in a Laboratory Simulation of Anti-Predator Defenses of Catocala Moths (Lepidoptera: Noctuidae)</title><source>Jstor Complete Legacy</source><source>Alma/SFX Local Collection</source><source>Periodicals Index Online</source><creator>Ingalls, Victoria</creator><creatorcontrib>Ingalls, Victoria</creatorcontrib><description>AbstractBlue jays were used in laboratory experiments to determine what visual characteristics of Catocala hindwings are important in eliciting and maintaining startle reactions in avian predators. Hand-reared blue jays, inexperienced with warningly coloured, inedible prey, took significantly longer to touch novel colours that possessed bold, black bands than to touch novel, unbanded colours when these discs covered a food reward. Thus, being conspicuous (as opposed to simply being novel) appears to enhance startle reactions. Bold patterns are commonly found on aposematic prey and Catocala hindwings. These same hand-reared birds also tended to take longer to touch novel coloured discs of yellow and red hues as opposed to discs of blue, green, or purple hues. The colours that produced the greatest hesitation are similar to the colours found on Catocala hindwings. Red-yellow colours are also characteristic of aposematic prey. Wild-caught birds took longer to habituate to startling stimuli when presented with several different startling disc types during the habituation process. Thus, predator pressure may explain why several different Catocala species with different hindwing colours occur sympatrically.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0005-7959</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1568-539X</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1163/156853993X00353</identifier><identifier>CODEN: BEHAA8</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>The Netherlands: Brill</publisher><subject>Animal ethology ; Biological and medical sciences ; Bird banding ; Birds ; Catocala ; Colors ; Cyanocitta cristata ; Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology ; Habituation ; Insect behavior ; Invertebrates ; Lepidoptera ; Mental stimulation ; Mimicry ; Moths ; Noctuidae ; Predators ; Protozoa. Invertebrata ; Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry ; Quadrants</subject><ispartof>Behaviour, 1993, Vol.126 (1-2), p.77-95</ispartof><rights>1993 Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, The Netherlands</rights><rights>Copyright 1993 E. J. Brill</rights><rights>1994 INIST-CNRS</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-b339t-7d53169d3d605804a3b5e68563dd99a24ed6a9b2d6eb01188edceafa902ceb8a3</citedby></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.jstor.org/stable/pdf/4535125$$EPDF$$P50$$Gjstor$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.jstor.org/stable/4535125$$EHTML$$P50$$Gjstor$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,799,4010,27846,27900,27901,27902,57992,58225</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=3842540$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Ingalls, Victoria</creatorcontrib><title>Startle and Habituation Responses of Blue Jays (Cyanocitta Cristata) in a Laboratory Simulation of Anti-Predator Defenses of Catocala Moths (Lepidoptera: Noctuidae)</title><title>Behaviour</title><addtitle>BEH</addtitle><description>AbstractBlue jays were used in laboratory experiments to determine what visual characteristics of Catocala hindwings are important in eliciting and maintaining startle reactions in avian predators. Hand-reared blue jays, inexperienced with warningly coloured, inedible prey, took significantly longer to touch novel colours that possessed bold, black bands than to touch novel, unbanded colours when these discs covered a food reward. Thus, being conspicuous (as opposed to simply being novel) appears to enhance startle reactions. Bold patterns are commonly found on aposematic prey and Catocala hindwings. These same hand-reared birds also tended to take longer to touch novel coloured discs of yellow and red hues as opposed to discs of blue, green, or purple hues. The colours that produced the greatest hesitation are similar to the colours found on Catocala hindwings. Red-yellow colours are also characteristic of aposematic prey. Wild-caught birds took longer to habituate to startling stimuli when presented with several different startling disc types during the habituation process. Thus, predator pressure may explain why several different Catocala species with different hindwing colours occur sympatrically.</description><subject>Animal ethology</subject><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Bird banding</subject><subject>Birds</subject><subject>Catocala</subject><subject>Colors</subject><subject>Cyanocitta cristata</subject><subject>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</subject><subject>Habituation</subject><subject>Insect behavior</subject><subject>Invertebrates</subject><subject>Lepidoptera</subject><subject>Mental stimulation</subject><subject>Mimicry</subject><subject>Moths</subject><subject>Noctuidae</subject><subject>Predators</subject><subject>Protozoa. Invertebrata</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</subject><subject>Quadrants</subject><issn>0005-7959</issn><issn>1568-539X</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1993</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>K30</sourceid><recordid>eNp1kUFv1DAQhSMEEkvhzIWDJRCih1A7jpOYW0lbSgkF0QJ7sybxRHjJxsF2JPb_8EPxasuCKnGyPO-bp5l5SfKY0ZeMFfyIiaISXEq-pJQLfidZbCtpLC3vJgtKqUhLKeT95IH3q_gtBReL5NdVABcGJDBqcg6tCTMEY0fyCf1kR4-e2J68HmYkF7Dx5EW9gdF2JgQgtTM-QIBDYkYCpIHWOgjWbciVWc_Dzid2H4_BpB8d6q1ITrDHP751rHQwAHlvw7do3uBktJ0COnhFLm0XZqMBDx8m93oYPD66eQ-Sz2en1_V52nx487Y-btKWcxnSUgvOCqm5LqioaA68FRiPUnCtpYQsR12AbDNdYEsZqyrUHUIPkmYdthXwg-T5zndy9seMPqi18R0OA4xoZ69YSSUVhYjg01vgys5ujLMpximvMl7keaSOdlTnrPcOezU5swa3UYyqbWbqVmax49mNL_h4l97B2Bm_b-NVnomcRuzJDlv5eNG9nMdEWbadLt3JMR78uZfBfVdFyUuhLt5dq-asWV5-_UJV_ZdvnRmGf1b5z5S_Aez3vZI</recordid><startdate>1993</startdate><enddate>1993</enddate><creator>Ingalls, Victoria</creator><general>Brill</general><general>BRILL</general><general>E. J. Brill</general><scope>BSCLL</scope><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>HQAFP</scope><scope>IBDFT</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>7SS</scope></search><sort><creationdate>1993</creationdate><title>Startle and Habituation Responses of Blue Jays (Cyanocitta Cristata) in a Laboratory Simulation of Anti-Predator Defenses of Catocala Moths (Lepidoptera: Noctuidae)</title><author>Ingalls, Victoria</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-b339t-7d53169d3d605804a3b5e68563dd99a24ed6a9b2d6eb01188edceafa902ceb8a3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1993</creationdate><topic>Animal ethology</topic><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Bird banding</topic><topic>Birds</topic><topic>Catocala</topic><topic>Colors</topic><topic>Cyanocitta cristata</topic><topic>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</topic><topic>Habituation</topic><topic>Insect behavior</topic><topic>Invertebrates</topic><topic>Lepidoptera</topic><topic>Mental stimulation</topic><topic>Mimicry</topic><topic>Moths</topic><topic>Noctuidae</topic><topic>Predators</topic><topic>Protozoa. Invertebrata</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</topic><topic>Quadrants</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Ingalls, Victoria</creatorcontrib><collection>Istex</collection><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 23</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 27</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>Entomology Abstracts (Full archive)</collection><jtitle>Behaviour</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Ingalls, Victoria</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Startle and Habituation Responses of Blue Jays (Cyanocitta Cristata) in a Laboratory Simulation of Anti-Predator Defenses of Catocala Moths (Lepidoptera: Noctuidae)</atitle><jtitle>Behaviour</jtitle><addtitle>BEH</addtitle><date>1993</date><risdate>1993</risdate><volume>126</volume><issue>1-2</issue><spage>77</spage><epage>95</epage><pages>77-95</pages><issn>0005-7959</issn><eissn>1568-539X</eissn><coden>BEHAA8</coden><abstract>AbstractBlue jays were used in laboratory experiments to determine what visual characteristics of Catocala hindwings are important in eliciting and maintaining startle reactions in avian predators. Hand-reared blue jays, inexperienced with warningly coloured, inedible prey, took significantly longer to touch novel colours that possessed bold, black bands than to touch novel, unbanded colours when these discs covered a food reward. Thus, being conspicuous (as opposed to simply being novel) appears to enhance startle reactions. Bold patterns are commonly found on aposematic prey and Catocala hindwings. These same hand-reared birds also tended to take longer to touch novel coloured discs of yellow and red hues as opposed to discs of blue, green, or purple hues. The colours that produced the greatest hesitation are similar to the colours found on Catocala hindwings. Red-yellow colours are also characteristic of aposematic prey. Wild-caught birds took longer to habituate to startling stimuli when presented with several different startling disc types during the habituation process. Thus, predator pressure may explain why several different Catocala species with different hindwing colours occur sympatrically.</abstract><cop>The Netherlands</cop><pub>Brill</pub><doi>10.1163/156853993X00353</doi><tpages>19</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0005-7959 |
ispartof | Behaviour, 1993, Vol.126 (1-2), p.77-95 |
issn | 0005-7959 1568-539X |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_17090565 |
source | Jstor Complete Legacy; Alma/SFX Local Collection; Periodicals Index Online |
subjects | Animal ethology Biological and medical sciences Bird banding Birds Catocala Colors Cyanocitta cristata Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology Habituation Insect behavior Invertebrates Lepidoptera Mental stimulation Mimicry Moths Noctuidae Predators Protozoa. Invertebrata Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry Quadrants |
title | Startle and Habituation Responses of Blue Jays (Cyanocitta Cristata) in a Laboratory Simulation of Anti-Predator Defenses of Catocala Moths (Lepidoptera: Noctuidae) |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-08T06%3A59%3A26IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-jstor_proqu&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Startle%20and%20Habituation%20Responses%20of%20Blue%20Jays%20(Cyanocitta%20Cristata)%20in%20a%20Laboratory%20Simulation%20of%20Anti-Predator%20Defenses%20of%20Catocala%20Moths%20(Lepidoptera:%20Noctuidae)&rft.jtitle=Behaviour&rft.au=Ingalls,%20Victoria&rft.date=1993&rft.volume=126&rft.issue=1-2&rft.spage=77&rft.epage=95&rft.pages=77-95&rft.issn=0005-7959&rft.eissn=1568-539X&rft.coden=BEHAA8&rft_id=info:doi/10.1163/156853993X00353&rft_dat=%3Cjstor_proqu%3E4535125%3C/jstor_proqu%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1303823644&rft_id=info:pmid/&rft_jstor_id=4535125&rfr_iscdi=true |