Emotion Regulation and Negative Emotionality Moderate the Effects of Moral (Dis)Engagement on Aggression
The effects of guilt, shame, and externalization of blame on aggressive behavior were investigated among a total of 307 Finnish fifth and sixth graders (M age = 11.9 years). Self-reported proneness to feel guilt and shame was expected to reduce levels of peer-reported aggressive behavior, whereas se...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 2015-01, Vol.61 (1), p.30-50 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 50 |
---|---|
container_issue | 1 |
container_start_page | 30 |
container_title | Merrill-Palmer Quarterly |
container_volume | 61 |
creator | Roos, Sanna Salmivalli, Christina Hodges, Ernest V. E. |
description | The effects of guilt, shame, and externalization of blame on aggressive behavior were investigated among a total of 307 Finnish fifth and sixth graders (M
age = 11.9 years). Self-reported proneness to feel guilt and shame was expected to reduce levels of peer-reported aggressive behavior, whereas self-reported externalization of blame was hypothesized to function as a moral disengagement mechanism with links to greater aggressive behavior. However, these associations were expected to be moderated by children's emotion-regulation capabilities and tendencies to experience negative emotionality (as reported by teachers). Results indicated that guilt and shame were associated with lower levels of aggression for children with poor emotion regulation (or high negative emotionality). For children with effective emotion regulation (or low negative emotionality), shame and externalization of blame were associated with higher levels of aggression. The results suggest that a dark side may be apparent in effective emotion regulation (and low negative emotionality) in that it enables children to disengage from the normally inhibiting functions of guilt and shame and to act aggressively in response to shame and externalization of blame. |
doi_str_mv | 10.13110/merrpalmquar1982.61.1.0030 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>jstor_proqu</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_1697497185</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><ericid>EJ1059479</ericid><jstor_id>10.13110/merrpalmquar1982.61.1.0030</jstor_id><sourcerecordid>10.13110/merrpalmquar1982.61.1.0030</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c442t-6ac9404ce6b75ed603a5f22e06733d0f0b36d44f156502a654cc05b7ed1efc533</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqVkUtr3DAUhUVpodO0P6FgyCZdeKqr55iuQjJ9kbbQB3QnNPKV48G2JpJcyL-vnAkpZNeVHvc7hyMdQk6BroED0Lcjxniww3gz2wjNhq0VrGFNKadPyAoklzVlSj0lK8o0qxtOfz8nL1LaF4I3IFfkejuG3Iep-o7dPNi7rZ3a6it25fAHq_u5Hfp8W30JLUabscrXZeI9upyq4Mt9tEN1dtmnN9upsx2OOOWqWJ13XcSUisFL8szbIeGr-_WE_Hq__Xnxsb769uHTxflV7YRguVbWNYIKh2qnJbaKcis9Y0iV5rylnu64aoXwIJWkzCopnKNyp7EF9E5yfkLOjr6HGG5mTNmMfXI4DHbCMCcDqtGi0bCRBT19hO7DHMtTF0oJKRlTm0K9O1IuhpQienOI_WjjrQFq7lowj1swCgyYpYWifn1UY-zdg3L7GahshG7KXDxk2JfvHOeE_2JIraVS5sfS5FIkSKDFdgm1Ocr2KYf4X4n-AoSzq40</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1664552268</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Emotion Regulation and Negative Emotionality Moderate the Effects of Moral (Dis)Engagement on Aggression</title><source>JSTOR Archive Collection A-Z Listing</source><source>EBSCOhost Education Source</source><creator>Roos, Sanna ; Salmivalli, Christina ; Hodges, Ernest V. E.</creator><creatorcontrib>Roos, Sanna ; Salmivalli, Christina ; Hodges, Ernest V. E.</creatorcontrib><description>The effects of guilt, shame, and externalization of blame on aggressive behavior were investigated among a total of 307 Finnish fifth and sixth graders (M
age = 11.9 years). Self-reported proneness to feel guilt and shame was expected to reduce levels of peer-reported aggressive behavior, whereas self-reported externalization of blame was hypothesized to function as a moral disengagement mechanism with links to greater aggressive behavior. However, these associations were expected to be moderated by children's emotion-regulation capabilities and tendencies to experience negative emotionality (as reported by teachers). Results indicated that guilt and shame were associated with lower levels of aggression for children with poor emotion regulation (or high negative emotionality). For children with effective emotion regulation (or low negative emotionality), shame and externalization of blame were associated with higher levels of aggression. The results suggest that a dark side may be apparent in effective emotion regulation (and low negative emotionality) in that it enables children to disengage from the normally inhibiting functions of guilt and shame and to act aggressively in response to shame and externalization of blame.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0272-930X</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1535-0266</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.13110/merrpalmquar1982.61.1.0030</identifier><identifier>CODEN: MPQUA5</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Detroit: Wayne State University Press</publisher><subject>Aggression ; Aggressiveness ; Anger ; Attribution Theory ; Behavior ; Behavior Problems ; Blame ; Child development ; Children & youth ; Cognitive ability ; Correlation ; Elementary School Students ; Emotion ; Emotional Response ; Emotionality ; Emotions ; Externalization ; Finland ; Finnish language ; Foreign Countries ; Gender Differences ; Grade 5 ; Grade 6 ; Guilt ; Human aggression ; Inhibition ; Measures (Individuals) ; Middle school students ; Moral Values ; Peer Evaluation ; Personality psychology ; Prediction ; Prosocial Behavior ; Psychological Patterns ; Questionnaires ; Regression (Statistics) ; Sanctions ; Secondary school students ; Self Control ; Shame ; Social psychology ; Teachers ; Teaching Methods</subject><ispartof>Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 2015-01, Vol.61 (1), p.30-50</ispartof><rights>2015 by Wayne State University Press</rights><rights>Copyright © 2008 Wayne State University Press.</rights><rights>Copyright Wayne State University Press Jan 2015</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c442t-6ac9404ce6b75ed603a5f22e06733d0f0b36d44f156502a654cc05b7ed1efc533</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c442t-6ac9404ce6b75ed603a5f22e06733d0f0b36d44f156502a654cc05b7ed1efc533</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,803,4024,27923,27924,27925</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ1059479$$DView record in ERIC$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Roos, Sanna</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Salmivalli, Christina</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Hodges, Ernest V. E.</creatorcontrib><title>Emotion Regulation and Negative Emotionality Moderate the Effects of Moral (Dis)Engagement on Aggression</title><title>Merrill-Palmer Quarterly</title><description>The effects of guilt, shame, and externalization of blame on aggressive behavior were investigated among a total of 307 Finnish fifth and sixth graders (M
age = 11.9 years). Self-reported proneness to feel guilt and shame was expected to reduce levels of peer-reported aggressive behavior, whereas self-reported externalization of blame was hypothesized to function as a moral disengagement mechanism with links to greater aggressive behavior. However, these associations were expected to be moderated by children's emotion-regulation capabilities and tendencies to experience negative emotionality (as reported by teachers). Results indicated that guilt and shame were associated with lower levels of aggression for children with poor emotion regulation (or high negative emotionality). For children with effective emotion regulation (or low negative emotionality), shame and externalization of blame were associated with higher levels of aggression. The results suggest that a dark side may be apparent in effective emotion regulation (and low negative emotionality) in that it enables children to disengage from the normally inhibiting functions of guilt and shame and to act aggressively in response to shame and externalization of blame.</description><subject>Aggression</subject><subject>Aggressiveness</subject><subject>Anger</subject><subject>Attribution Theory</subject><subject>Behavior</subject><subject>Behavior Problems</subject><subject>Blame</subject><subject>Child development</subject><subject>Children & youth</subject><subject>Cognitive ability</subject><subject>Correlation</subject><subject>Elementary School Students</subject><subject>Emotion</subject><subject>Emotional Response</subject><subject>Emotionality</subject><subject>Emotions</subject><subject>Externalization</subject><subject>Finland</subject><subject>Finnish language</subject><subject>Foreign Countries</subject><subject>Gender Differences</subject><subject>Grade 5</subject><subject>Grade 6</subject><subject>Guilt</subject><subject>Human aggression</subject><subject>Inhibition</subject><subject>Measures (Individuals)</subject><subject>Middle school students</subject><subject>Moral Values</subject><subject>Peer Evaluation</subject><subject>Personality psychology</subject><subject>Prediction</subject><subject>Prosocial Behavior</subject><subject>Psychological Patterns</subject><subject>Questionnaires</subject><subject>Regression (Statistics)</subject><subject>Sanctions</subject><subject>Secondary school students</subject><subject>Self Control</subject><subject>Shame</subject><subject>Social psychology</subject><subject>Teachers</subject><subject>Teaching Methods</subject><issn>0272-930X</issn><issn>1535-0266</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2015</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>ABUWG</sourceid><sourceid>AFKRA</sourceid><sourceid>AIMQZ</sourceid><sourceid>AZQEC</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>CCPQU</sourceid><sourceid>DWQXO</sourceid><sourceid>GNUQQ</sourceid><recordid>eNqVkUtr3DAUhUVpodO0P6FgyCZdeKqr55iuQjJ9kbbQB3QnNPKV48G2JpJcyL-vnAkpZNeVHvc7hyMdQk6BroED0Lcjxniww3gz2wjNhq0VrGFNKadPyAoklzVlSj0lK8o0qxtOfz8nL1LaF4I3IFfkejuG3Iep-o7dPNi7rZ3a6it25fAHq_u5Hfp8W30JLUabscrXZeI9upyq4Mt9tEN1dtmnN9upsx2OOOWqWJ13XcSUisFL8szbIeGr-_WE_Hq__Xnxsb769uHTxflV7YRguVbWNYIKh2qnJbaKcis9Y0iV5rylnu64aoXwIJWkzCopnKNyp7EF9E5yfkLOjr6HGG5mTNmMfXI4DHbCMCcDqtGi0bCRBT19hO7DHMtTF0oJKRlTm0K9O1IuhpQienOI_WjjrQFq7lowj1swCgyYpYWifn1UY-zdg3L7GahshG7KXDxk2JfvHOeE_2JIraVS5sfS5FIkSKDFdgm1Ocr2KYf4X4n-AoSzq40</recordid><startdate>20150101</startdate><enddate>20150101</enddate><creator>Roos, Sanna</creator><creator>Salmivalli, Christina</creator><creator>Hodges, Ernest V. E.</creator><general>Wayne State University Press</general><scope>7SW</scope><scope>BJH</scope><scope>BNH</scope><scope>BNI</scope><scope>BNJ</scope><scope>BNO</scope><scope>ERI</scope><scope>PET</scope><scope>REK</scope><scope>WWN</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>0-V</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>4T-</scope><scope>4U-</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88B</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>8A4</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>AIMQZ</scope><scope>ALSLI</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>CJNVE</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>LIQON</scope><scope>M0P</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>PQEDU</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>S0X</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20150101</creationdate><title>Emotion Regulation and Negative Emotionality Moderate the Effects of Moral (Dis)Engagement on Aggression</title><author>Roos, Sanna ; Salmivalli, Christina ; Hodges, Ernest V. E.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c442t-6ac9404ce6b75ed603a5f22e06733d0f0b36d44f156502a654cc05b7ed1efc533</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2015</creationdate><topic>Aggression</topic><topic>Aggressiveness</topic><topic>Anger</topic><topic>Attribution Theory</topic><topic>Behavior</topic><topic>Behavior Problems</topic><topic>Blame</topic><topic>Child development</topic><topic>Children & youth</topic><topic>Cognitive ability</topic><topic>Correlation</topic><topic>Elementary School Students</topic><topic>Emotion</topic><topic>Emotional Response</topic><topic>Emotionality</topic><topic>Emotions</topic><topic>Externalization</topic><topic>Finland</topic><topic>Finnish language</topic><topic>Foreign Countries</topic><topic>Gender Differences</topic><topic>Grade 5</topic><topic>Grade 6</topic><topic>Guilt</topic><topic>Human aggression</topic><topic>Inhibition</topic><topic>Measures (Individuals)</topic><topic>Middle school students</topic><topic>Moral Values</topic><topic>Peer Evaluation</topic><topic>Personality psychology</topic><topic>Prediction</topic><topic>Prosocial Behavior</topic><topic>Psychological Patterns</topic><topic>Questionnaires</topic><topic>Regression (Statistics)</topic><topic>Sanctions</topic><topic>Secondary school students</topic><topic>Self Control</topic><topic>Shame</topic><topic>Social psychology</topic><topic>Teachers</topic><topic>Teaching Methods</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Roos, Sanna</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Salmivalli, Christina</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Hodges, Ernest V. E.</creatorcontrib><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Ovid)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>ERIC( SilverPlatter )</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>Education Resources Information Center (ERIC)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Docstoc</collection><collection>University Readers</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Education Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>Education Periodicals</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>ProQuest One Literature</collection><collection>Social Science Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>Education Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>ProQuest One Literature - U.S. Customers Only</collection><collection>Education Database</collection><collection>Psychology Database</collection><collection>ProQuest One Education</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>SIRS Editorial</collection><jtitle>Merrill-Palmer Quarterly</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Roos, Sanna</au><au>Salmivalli, Christina</au><au>Hodges, Ernest V. E.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><ericid>EJ1059479</ericid><atitle>Emotion Regulation and Negative Emotionality Moderate the Effects of Moral (Dis)Engagement on Aggression</atitle><jtitle>Merrill-Palmer Quarterly</jtitle><date>2015-01-01</date><risdate>2015</risdate><volume>61</volume><issue>1</issue><spage>30</spage><epage>50</epage><pages>30-50</pages><issn>0272-930X</issn><eissn>1535-0266</eissn><coden>MPQUA5</coden><abstract>The effects of guilt, shame, and externalization of blame on aggressive behavior were investigated among a total of 307 Finnish fifth and sixth graders (M
age = 11.9 years). Self-reported proneness to feel guilt and shame was expected to reduce levels of peer-reported aggressive behavior, whereas self-reported externalization of blame was hypothesized to function as a moral disengagement mechanism with links to greater aggressive behavior. However, these associations were expected to be moderated by children's emotion-regulation capabilities and tendencies to experience negative emotionality (as reported by teachers). Results indicated that guilt and shame were associated with lower levels of aggression for children with poor emotion regulation (or high negative emotionality). For children with effective emotion regulation (or low negative emotionality), shame and externalization of blame were associated with higher levels of aggression. The results suggest that a dark side may be apparent in effective emotion regulation (and low negative emotionality) in that it enables children to disengage from the normally inhibiting functions of guilt and shame and to act aggressively in response to shame and externalization of blame.</abstract><cop>Detroit</cop><pub>Wayne State University Press</pub><doi>10.13110/merrpalmquar1982.61.1.0030</doi><tpages>21</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0272-930X |
ispartof | Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 2015-01, Vol.61 (1), p.30-50 |
issn | 0272-930X 1535-0266 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_1697497185 |
source | JSTOR Archive Collection A-Z Listing; EBSCOhost Education Source |
subjects | Aggression Aggressiveness Anger Attribution Theory Behavior Behavior Problems Blame Child development Children & youth Cognitive ability Correlation Elementary School Students Emotion Emotional Response Emotionality Emotions Externalization Finland Finnish language Foreign Countries Gender Differences Grade 5 Grade 6 Guilt Human aggression Inhibition Measures (Individuals) Middle school students Moral Values Peer Evaluation Personality psychology Prediction Prosocial Behavior Psychological Patterns Questionnaires Regression (Statistics) Sanctions Secondary school students Self Control Shame Social psychology Teachers Teaching Methods |
title | Emotion Regulation and Negative Emotionality Moderate the Effects of Moral (Dis)Engagement on Aggression |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-02T23%3A31%3A09IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-jstor_proqu&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Emotion%20Regulation%20and%20Negative%20Emotionality%20Moderate%20the%20Effects%20of%20Moral%20(Dis)Engagement%20on%20Aggression&rft.jtitle=Merrill-Palmer%20Quarterly&rft.au=Roos,%20Sanna&rft.date=2015-01-01&rft.volume=61&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=30&rft.epage=50&rft.pages=30-50&rft.issn=0272-930X&rft.eissn=1535-0266&rft.coden=MPQUA5&rft_id=info:doi/10.13110/merrpalmquar1982.61.1.0030&rft_dat=%3Cjstor_proqu%3E10.13110/merrpalmquar1982.61.1.0030%3C/jstor_proqu%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1664552268&rft_id=info:pmid/&rft_ericid=EJ1059479&rft_jstor_id=10.13110/merrpalmquar1982.61.1.0030&rfr_iscdi=true |