Optimal isn’t good enough
The notion that biological systems come to embody optimal solutions seems consistent with the competitive drive of evolution. It has been used to interpret many examples of sensorimotor behavior. It is attractive from the viewpoint of control engineers because it solves the redundancy problem by ide...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Biological cybernetics 2012-12, Vol.106 (11-12), p.757-765 |
---|---|
1. Verfasser: | |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 765 |
---|---|
container_issue | 11-12 |
container_start_page | 757 |
container_title | Biological cybernetics |
container_volume | 106 |
creator | Loeb, Gerald E. |
description | The notion that biological systems come to embody optimal solutions seems consistent with the competitive drive of evolution. It has been used to interpret many examples of sensorimotor behavior. It is attractive from the viewpoint of control engineers because it solves the redundancy problem by identifying the one optimal motor strategy out of many similarly acceptable possibilities. This perspective examines whether there is sufficient basis to apply the formal engineering tools of optimal control to a reductionist understanding of biological systems. For an experimental biologist, this translates into whether the theory of optimal control generates nontrivial and testable hypotheses that accurately predict novel phenomena, ideally at deeper levels of structure than the observable behavior. The methodology of optimal control is applicable when there is (i) a single, known cost function to be optimized, (ii) an invertible model of the plant, and (iii) simple noise interfering with optimal performance. None of these is likely to be true for biological organisms. Furthermore, their motivation is usually good-enough rather than globally optimal behavior. Even then, the performance of a biological organism is often much farther from optimal than the physical limits of its hardware because the brain is continuously testing the acceptable limits of performance as well as just performing the task. This perspective considers an alternative strategy called “good-enough” control, in which the organism uses trial-and-error learning to acquire a repertoire of sensorimotor behaviors that are known to be useful, but not necessarily optimal. This leads to a diversity of solutions that tends to confer robustness on the individual organism and its evolution. It is also more consistent with the capabilities of higher sensorimotor structures, such as cerebral cortex, which seems to be designed to classify and recall complex sets of information, thereby allowing the organism to learn from experience, rather than to compute new strategies online. Optimal control has been a useful metaphor for understanding some superficial aspects of motor psychophysics. Reductionists who want to understand the underlying neural mechanisms need to move on. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1007/s00422-012-0514-6 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_1685808337</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>1439739711</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c471t-6458be53213bb651b166cda1fb7ecd805d751ea15e137fc77362428e4a815d813</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqNkctKAzEUhoMotlYfQAQpuHEzek7us5TiDQrd6DrMJVNbppOazCzc-Rq-nk9iylQRQRQSQsh3_pPkI-QY4QIB1GUA4JQmgHEK5IncIUPkLO6Ugl0yBMYhQQowIAchLAEgpSLdJwNKdSo0gyE5ma3bxSqrx4vQvL--teO5c-XYNq6bPx2SvSqrgz3ariPyeHP9MLlLprPb-8nVNCm4wjaRXOjcCkaR5bkUmKOURZlhlStblBpEqQTaDIVFpqpCKSYpp9ryTKMoNbIROe9z1949dza0ZrUIha3rrLGuCwalFho0Y-pvlLNUxYH_SKUUkcbriYie_UCXrvNNfHOkpORAgelIYU8V3oXgbWXWPn6dfzEIZuPD9D5M9GE2PoyMNafb5C5f2fKr4lNABGgPhHjUzK3_1vrX1A962JFA</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1266402038</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Optimal isn’t good enough</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>SpringerLink Journals</source><creator>Loeb, Gerald E.</creator><creatorcontrib>Loeb, Gerald E.</creatorcontrib><description>The notion that biological systems come to embody optimal solutions seems consistent with the competitive drive of evolution. It has been used to interpret many examples of sensorimotor behavior. It is attractive from the viewpoint of control engineers because it solves the redundancy problem by identifying the one optimal motor strategy out of many similarly acceptable possibilities. This perspective examines whether there is sufficient basis to apply the formal engineering tools of optimal control to a reductionist understanding of biological systems. For an experimental biologist, this translates into whether the theory of optimal control generates nontrivial and testable hypotheses that accurately predict novel phenomena, ideally at deeper levels of structure than the observable behavior. The methodology of optimal control is applicable when there is (i) a single, known cost function to be optimized, (ii) an invertible model of the plant, and (iii) simple noise interfering with optimal performance. None of these is likely to be true for biological organisms. Furthermore, their motivation is usually good-enough rather than globally optimal behavior. Even then, the performance of a biological organism is often much farther from optimal than the physical limits of its hardware because the brain is continuously testing the acceptable limits of performance as well as just performing the task. This perspective considers an alternative strategy called “good-enough” control, in which the organism uses trial-and-error learning to acquire a repertoire of sensorimotor behaviors that are known to be useful, but not necessarily optimal. This leads to a diversity of solutions that tends to confer robustness on the individual organism and its evolution. It is also more consistent with the capabilities of higher sensorimotor structures, such as cerebral cortex, which seems to be designed to classify and recall complex sets of information, thereby allowing the organism to learn from experience, rather than to compute new strategies online. Optimal control has been a useful metaphor for understanding some superficial aspects of motor psychophysics. Reductionists who want to understand the underlying neural mechanisms need to move on.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0340-1200</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1432-0770</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1007/s00422-012-0514-6</identifier><identifier>PMID: 22895830</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag</publisher><subject>Algorithms ; Animals ; Bioinformatics ; Biological ; Biomedical and Life Sciences ; Biomedicine ; Brain - physiology ; Complex Systems ; Computer Appl. in Life Sciences ; Cybernetics ; Evolution ; Feedback, Sensory - physiology ; Humans ; Learning - physiology ; Mathematical models ; Motivation ; Motor ability ; Motors ; Neurobiology ; Neurosciences ; Optimal control ; Optimization ; Organisms ; Prospects ; Sensation - physiology ; Spinal cord ; Strategy</subject><ispartof>Biological cybernetics, 2012-12, Vol.106 (11-12), p.757-765</ispartof><rights>Springer-Verlag 2012</rights><rights>Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2012</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c471t-6458be53213bb651b166cda1fb7ecd805d751ea15e137fc77362428e4a815d813</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c471t-6458be53213bb651b166cda1fb7ecd805d751ea15e137fc77362428e4a815d813</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s00422-012-0514-6$$EPDF$$P50$$Gspringer$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://link.springer.com/10.1007/s00422-012-0514-6$$EHTML$$P50$$Gspringer$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,27901,27902,41464,42533,51294</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22895830$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Loeb, Gerald E.</creatorcontrib><title>Optimal isn’t good enough</title><title>Biological cybernetics</title><addtitle>Biol Cybern</addtitle><addtitle>Biol Cybern</addtitle><description>The notion that biological systems come to embody optimal solutions seems consistent with the competitive drive of evolution. It has been used to interpret many examples of sensorimotor behavior. It is attractive from the viewpoint of control engineers because it solves the redundancy problem by identifying the one optimal motor strategy out of many similarly acceptable possibilities. This perspective examines whether there is sufficient basis to apply the formal engineering tools of optimal control to a reductionist understanding of biological systems. For an experimental biologist, this translates into whether the theory of optimal control generates nontrivial and testable hypotheses that accurately predict novel phenomena, ideally at deeper levels of structure than the observable behavior. The methodology of optimal control is applicable when there is (i) a single, known cost function to be optimized, (ii) an invertible model of the plant, and (iii) simple noise interfering with optimal performance. None of these is likely to be true for biological organisms. Furthermore, their motivation is usually good-enough rather than globally optimal behavior. Even then, the performance of a biological organism is often much farther from optimal than the physical limits of its hardware because the brain is continuously testing the acceptable limits of performance as well as just performing the task. This perspective considers an alternative strategy called “good-enough” control, in which the organism uses trial-and-error learning to acquire a repertoire of sensorimotor behaviors that are known to be useful, but not necessarily optimal. This leads to a diversity of solutions that tends to confer robustness on the individual organism and its evolution. It is also more consistent with the capabilities of higher sensorimotor structures, such as cerebral cortex, which seems to be designed to classify and recall complex sets of information, thereby allowing the organism to learn from experience, rather than to compute new strategies online. Optimal control has been a useful metaphor for understanding some superficial aspects of motor psychophysics. Reductionists who want to understand the underlying neural mechanisms need to move on.</description><subject>Algorithms</subject><subject>Animals</subject><subject>Bioinformatics</subject><subject>Biological</subject><subject>Biomedical and Life Sciences</subject><subject>Biomedicine</subject><subject>Brain - physiology</subject><subject>Complex Systems</subject><subject>Computer Appl. in Life Sciences</subject><subject>Cybernetics</subject><subject>Evolution</subject><subject>Feedback, Sensory - physiology</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Learning - physiology</subject><subject>Mathematical models</subject><subject>Motivation</subject><subject>Motor ability</subject><subject>Motors</subject><subject>Neurobiology</subject><subject>Neurosciences</subject><subject>Optimal control</subject><subject>Optimization</subject><subject>Organisms</subject><subject>Prospects</subject><subject>Sensation - physiology</subject><subject>Spinal cord</subject><subject>Strategy</subject><issn>0340-1200</issn><issn>1432-0770</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2012</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><recordid>eNqNkctKAzEUhoMotlYfQAQpuHEzek7us5TiDQrd6DrMJVNbppOazCzc-Rq-nk9iylQRQRQSQsh3_pPkI-QY4QIB1GUA4JQmgHEK5IncIUPkLO6Ugl0yBMYhQQowIAchLAEgpSLdJwNKdSo0gyE5ma3bxSqrx4vQvL--teO5c-XYNq6bPx2SvSqrgz3ariPyeHP9MLlLprPb-8nVNCm4wjaRXOjcCkaR5bkUmKOURZlhlStblBpEqQTaDIVFpqpCKSYpp9ryTKMoNbIROe9z1949dza0ZrUIha3rrLGuCwalFho0Y-pvlLNUxYH_SKUUkcbriYie_UCXrvNNfHOkpORAgelIYU8V3oXgbWXWPn6dfzEIZuPD9D5M9GE2PoyMNafb5C5f2fKr4lNABGgPhHjUzK3_1vrX1A962JFA</recordid><startdate>20121201</startdate><enddate>20121201</enddate><creator>Loeb, Gerald E.</creator><general>Springer-Verlag</general><general>Springer Nature B.V</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QO</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88A</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>88I</scope><scope>8AL</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FE</scope><scope>8FG</scope><scope>8FH</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AEUYN</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ARAPS</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BBNVY</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BGLVJ</scope><scope>BHPHI</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>H8D</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>JQ2</scope><scope>K7-</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>L7M</scope><scope>LK8</scope><scope>M0N</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M2P</scope><scope>M7P</scope><scope>P5Z</scope><scope>P62</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PHGZM</scope><scope>PHGZT</scope><scope>PJZUB</scope><scope>PKEHL</scope><scope>PPXIY</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQGLB</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20121201</creationdate><title>Optimal isn’t good enough</title><author>Loeb, Gerald E.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c471t-6458be53213bb651b166cda1fb7ecd805d751ea15e137fc77362428e4a815d813</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2012</creationdate><topic>Algorithms</topic><topic>Animals</topic><topic>Bioinformatics</topic><topic>Biological</topic><topic>Biomedical and Life Sciences</topic><topic>Biomedicine</topic><topic>Brain - physiology</topic><topic>Complex Systems</topic><topic>Computer Appl. in Life Sciences</topic><topic>Cybernetics</topic><topic>Evolution</topic><topic>Feedback, Sensory - physiology</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Learning - physiology</topic><topic>Mathematical models</topic><topic>Motivation</topic><topic>Motor ability</topic><topic>Motors</topic><topic>Neurobiology</topic><topic>Neurosciences</topic><topic>Optimal control</topic><topic>Optimization</topic><topic>Organisms</topic><topic>Prospects</topic><topic>Sensation - physiology</topic><topic>Spinal cord</topic><topic>Strategy</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Loeb, Gerald E.</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Biotechnology Research Abstracts</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Biology Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Science Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Computing Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>ProQuest SciTech Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Technology Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Sustainability</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Technology Collection</collection><collection>Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>Aerospace Database</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Computer Science Collection</collection><collection>Computer Science Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies Database with Aerospace</collection><collection>ProQuest Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>Computing Database</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>Science Database</collection><collection>Biological Science Database</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (New)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic (New)</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Research Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Health & Nursing</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Applied & Life Sciences</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Biological cybernetics</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Loeb, Gerald E.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Optimal isn’t good enough</atitle><jtitle>Biological cybernetics</jtitle><stitle>Biol Cybern</stitle><addtitle>Biol Cybern</addtitle><date>2012-12-01</date><risdate>2012</risdate><volume>106</volume><issue>11-12</issue><spage>757</spage><epage>765</epage><pages>757-765</pages><issn>0340-1200</issn><eissn>1432-0770</eissn><abstract>The notion that biological systems come to embody optimal solutions seems consistent with the competitive drive of evolution. It has been used to interpret many examples of sensorimotor behavior. It is attractive from the viewpoint of control engineers because it solves the redundancy problem by identifying the one optimal motor strategy out of many similarly acceptable possibilities. This perspective examines whether there is sufficient basis to apply the formal engineering tools of optimal control to a reductionist understanding of biological systems. For an experimental biologist, this translates into whether the theory of optimal control generates nontrivial and testable hypotheses that accurately predict novel phenomena, ideally at deeper levels of structure than the observable behavior. The methodology of optimal control is applicable when there is (i) a single, known cost function to be optimized, (ii) an invertible model of the plant, and (iii) simple noise interfering with optimal performance. None of these is likely to be true for biological organisms. Furthermore, their motivation is usually good-enough rather than globally optimal behavior. Even then, the performance of a biological organism is often much farther from optimal than the physical limits of its hardware because the brain is continuously testing the acceptable limits of performance as well as just performing the task. This perspective considers an alternative strategy called “good-enough” control, in which the organism uses trial-and-error learning to acquire a repertoire of sensorimotor behaviors that are known to be useful, but not necessarily optimal. This leads to a diversity of solutions that tends to confer robustness on the individual organism and its evolution. It is also more consistent with the capabilities of higher sensorimotor structures, such as cerebral cortex, which seems to be designed to classify and recall complex sets of information, thereby allowing the organism to learn from experience, rather than to compute new strategies online. Optimal control has been a useful metaphor for understanding some superficial aspects of motor psychophysics. Reductionists who want to understand the underlying neural mechanisms need to move on.</abstract><cop>Berlin/Heidelberg</cop><pub>Springer-Verlag</pub><pmid>22895830</pmid><doi>10.1007/s00422-012-0514-6</doi><tpages>9</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0340-1200 |
ispartof | Biological cybernetics, 2012-12, Vol.106 (11-12), p.757-765 |
issn | 0340-1200 1432-0770 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_1685808337 |
source | MEDLINE; SpringerLink Journals |
subjects | Algorithms Animals Bioinformatics Biological Biomedical and Life Sciences Biomedicine Brain - physiology Complex Systems Computer Appl. in Life Sciences Cybernetics Evolution Feedback, Sensory - physiology Humans Learning - physiology Mathematical models Motivation Motor ability Motors Neurobiology Neurosciences Optimal control Optimization Organisms Prospects Sensation - physiology Spinal cord Strategy |
title | Optimal isn’t good enough |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-20T02%3A55%3A50IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Optimal%20isn%E2%80%99t%20good%20enough&rft.jtitle=Biological%20cybernetics&rft.au=Loeb,%20Gerald%20E.&rft.date=2012-12-01&rft.volume=106&rft.issue=11-12&rft.spage=757&rft.epage=765&rft.pages=757-765&rft.issn=0340-1200&rft.eissn=1432-0770&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007/s00422-012-0514-6&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E1439739711%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1266402038&rft_id=info:pmid/22895830&rfr_iscdi=true |