Scent marking in free-ranging tigers, Panthera tigris

Data were colleted on the scent-marking patterns of radio-collared and visually identifiable tigers for 4 years in Royal Chitwan National Park, Nepal. Five categories of marking were recorded: urine spraying; scraping with deposits of urine faeces, and anal gland secretions; clawing; cheek rubbing;...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Animal behaviour 1989, Vol.37 (1), p.1-10
Hauptverfasser: David Smith, James L., McDougal, Charles, Miquelle, Dale
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 10
container_issue 1
container_start_page 1
container_title Animal behaviour
container_volume 37
creator David Smith, James L.
McDougal, Charles
Miquelle, Dale
description Data were colleted on the scent-marking patterns of radio-collared and visually identifiable tigers for 4 years in Royal Chitwan National Park, Nepal. Five categories of marking were recorded: urine spraying; scraping with deposits of urine faeces, and anal gland secretions; clawing; cheek rubbing; and vegetation flattening. Urine spraying and scraping were the predominant forms of marking in this population. Tigers marked more heavily at territorial boundaries than in the interior of territories. Furthermore, in border areas marks were highly clumped at contact zones where major routes of travel approached territorial boundaries. This pattern appears to be a result of the density of vegetation which channels travel. The intensity of marking in these zones represented a higher frequency of marking rather than an increase in time spent in these areas. Marking was most intensive when tigers were establishing territories, and animals on adjacent territories appeared to mark in response to each other. Females marked intensively just prior to oestrus; this behaviour was reduced during oestrus. Males marked more frequently when females were in oestrus than during other stages of the females' cycle. A model is proposed where an odour field signals the risk of encountering a conspecific, thus allowing animals to compare the costs of possible encounters with the benefits of use of a given area.
doi_str_mv 10.1016/0003-3472(89)90001-8
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_15843668</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><els_id>0003347289900018</els_id><sourcerecordid>5762575</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c391t-c2525d6fe4ffe19d129c2996f02a9cf07e0a2268ea39d7011e4cc1d21bab4e253</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9kM1KAzEURoMoWKtv4GIQEQVHkzuTTLIRpPgHBQV1HdLMTU1tZ2oyFXx7M7a4cOEqfOHc7yaHkENGLxhl4pJSWuRFWcGpVGcqJZbLLTJgVPFcgoRtMvhFdslejLMUBad8QPizxabLFia8-2aa-SZzATEPppn2ufNTDPE8ezJN94bB9BfBx32y48w84sHmHJLX25uX0X0-frx7GF2Pc1so1uUWOPBaOCydQ6ZqBsqCUsJRMMo6WiE1AEKiKVRdUcawtJbVwCZmUiLwYkhO1r3L0H6sMHZ64aPF-dw02K6iZlyWhRAygUd_wFm7Ck16mwYouVAVQILKNWRDG2NAp5fBp59_aUZ1L1L3lnRvSUulf0Tqvvt4022iNXOX3Fgff2crEElllbCrNYZJyKfHoKP12FisfUDb6br1_-_5BqKWhMA</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>224569722</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Scent marking in free-ranging tigers, Panthera tigris</title><source>Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals</source><creator>David Smith, James L. ; McDougal, Charles ; Miquelle, Dale</creator><creatorcontrib>David Smith, James L. ; McDougal, Charles ; Miquelle, Dale</creatorcontrib><description>Data were colleted on the scent-marking patterns of radio-collared and visually identifiable tigers for 4 years in Royal Chitwan National Park, Nepal. Five categories of marking were recorded: urine spraying; scraping with deposits of urine faeces, and anal gland secretions; clawing; cheek rubbing; and vegetation flattening. Urine spraying and scraping were the predominant forms of marking in this population. Tigers marked more heavily at territorial boundaries than in the interior of territories. Furthermore, in border areas marks were highly clumped at contact zones where major routes of travel approached territorial boundaries. This pattern appears to be a result of the density of vegetation which channels travel. The intensity of marking in these zones represented a higher frequency of marking rather than an increase in time spent in these areas. Marking was most intensive when tigers were establishing territories, and animals on adjacent territories appeared to mark in response to each other. Females marked intensively just prior to oestrus; this behaviour was reduced during oestrus. Males marked more frequently when females were in oestrus than during other stages of the females' cycle. A model is proposed where an odour field signals the risk of encountering a conspecific, thus allowing animals to compare the costs of possible encounters with the benefits of use of a given area.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0003-3472</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1095-8282</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/0003-3472(89)90001-8</identifier><identifier>CODEN: ANBEA8</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Kent: Elsevier Ltd</publisher><subject>Animal behavior ; Animal ethology ; Biological and medical sciences ; Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology ; Mammalia ; Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry ; Vertebrata ; Wildcats</subject><ispartof>Animal behaviour, 1989, Vol.37 (1), p.1-10</ispartof><rights>1989 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour</rights><rights>1989 INIST-CNRS</rights><rights>Copyright Harcourt Brace Jovanovich Ltd. Jan 1989</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c391t-c2525d6fe4ffe19d129c2996f02a9cf07e0a2268ea39d7011e4cc1d21bab4e253</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c391t-c2525d6fe4ffe19d129c2996f02a9cf07e0a2268ea39d7011e4cc1d21bab4e253</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/0003347289900018$$EHTML$$P50$$Gelsevier$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,3537,4010,27900,27901,27902,65306</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&amp;idt=7266507$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>David Smith, James L.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>McDougal, Charles</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Miquelle, Dale</creatorcontrib><title>Scent marking in free-ranging tigers, Panthera tigris</title><title>Animal behaviour</title><description>Data were colleted on the scent-marking patterns of radio-collared and visually identifiable tigers for 4 years in Royal Chitwan National Park, Nepal. Five categories of marking were recorded: urine spraying; scraping with deposits of urine faeces, and anal gland secretions; clawing; cheek rubbing; and vegetation flattening. Urine spraying and scraping were the predominant forms of marking in this population. Tigers marked more heavily at territorial boundaries than in the interior of territories. Furthermore, in border areas marks were highly clumped at contact zones where major routes of travel approached territorial boundaries. This pattern appears to be a result of the density of vegetation which channels travel. The intensity of marking in these zones represented a higher frequency of marking rather than an increase in time spent in these areas. Marking was most intensive when tigers were establishing territories, and animals on adjacent territories appeared to mark in response to each other. Females marked intensively just prior to oestrus; this behaviour was reduced during oestrus. Males marked more frequently when females were in oestrus than during other stages of the females' cycle. A model is proposed where an odour field signals the risk of encountering a conspecific, thus allowing animals to compare the costs of possible encounters with the benefits of use of a given area.</description><subject>Animal behavior</subject><subject>Animal ethology</subject><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</subject><subject>Mammalia</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</subject><subject>Vertebrata</subject><subject>Wildcats</subject><issn>0003-3472</issn><issn>1095-8282</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1989</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><recordid>eNp9kM1KAzEURoMoWKtv4GIQEQVHkzuTTLIRpPgHBQV1HdLMTU1tZ2oyFXx7M7a4cOEqfOHc7yaHkENGLxhl4pJSWuRFWcGpVGcqJZbLLTJgVPFcgoRtMvhFdslejLMUBad8QPizxabLFia8-2aa-SZzATEPppn2ufNTDPE8ezJN94bB9BfBx32y48w84sHmHJLX25uX0X0-frx7GF2Pc1so1uUWOPBaOCydQ6ZqBsqCUsJRMMo6WiE1AEKiKVRdUcawtJbVwCZmUiLwYkhO1r3L0H6sMHZ64aPF-dw02K6iZlyWhRAygUd_wFm7Ck16mwYouVAVQILKNWRDG2NAp5fBp59_aUZ1L1L3lnRvSUulf0Tqvvt4022iNXOX3Fgff2crEElllbCrNYZJyKfHoKP12FisfUDb6br1_-_5BqKWhMA</recordid><startdate>1989</startdate><enddate>1989</enddate><creator>David Smith, James L.</creator><creator>McDougal, Charles</creator><creator>Miquelle, Dale</creator><general>Elsevier Ltd</general><general>Elsevier</general><general>Harcourt Brace Jovanovich Ltd</general><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>7SN</scope><scope>7SS</scope><scope>7U7</scope><scope>C1K</scope><scope>7QR</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>P64</scope></search><sort><creationdate>1989</creationdate><title>Scent marking in free-ranging tigers, Panthera tigris</title><author>David Smith, James L. ; McDougal, Charles ; Miquelle, Dale</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c391t-c2525d6fe4ffe19d129c2996f02a9cf07e0a2268ea39d7011e4cc1d21bab4e253</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1989</creationdate><topic>Animal behavior</topic><topic>Animal ethology</topic><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</topic><topic>Mammalia</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</topic><topic>Vertebrata</topic><topic>Wildcats</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>David Smith, James L.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>McDougal, Charles</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Miquelle, Dale</creatorcontrib><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>Ecology Abstracts</collection><collection>Entomology Abstracts (Full archive)</collection><collection>Toxicology Abstracts</collection><collection>Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management</collection><collection>Chemoreception Abstracts</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><jtitle>Animal behaviour</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>David Smith, James L.</au><au>McDougal, Charles</au><au>Miquelle, Dale</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Scent marking in free-ranging tigers, Panthera tigris</atitle><jtitle>Animal behaviour</jtitle><date>1989</date><risdate>1989</risdate><volume>37</volume><issue>1</issue><spage>1</spage><epage>10</epage><pages>1-10</pages><issn>0003-3472</issn><eissn>1095-8282</eissn><coden>ANBEA8</coden><abstract>Data were colleted on the scent-marking patterns of radio-collared and visually identifiable tigers for 4 years in Royal Chitwan National Park, Nepal. Five categories of marking were recorded: urine spraying; scraping with deposits of urine faeces, and anal gland secretions; clawing; cheek rubbing; and vegetation flattening. Urine spraying and scraping were the predominant forms of marking in this population. Tigers marked more heavily at territorial boundaries than in the interior of territories. Furthermore, in border areas marks were highly clumped at contact zones where major routes of travel approached territorial boundaries. This pattern appears to be a result of the density of vegetation which channels travel. The intensity of marking in these zones represented a higher frequency of marking rather than an increase in time spent in these areas. Marking was most intensive when tigers were establishing territories, and animals on adjacent territories appeared to mark in response to each other. Females marked intensively just prior to oestrus; this behaviour was reduced during oestrus. Males marked more frequently when females were in oestrus than during other stages of the females' cycle. A model is proposed where an odour field signals the risk of encountering a conspecific, thus allowing animals to compare the costs of possible encounters with the benefits of use of a given area.</abstract><cop>Kent</cop><pub>Elsevier Ltd</pub><doi>10.1016/0003-3472(89)90001-8</doi><tpages>10</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0003-3472
ispartof Animal behaviour, 1989, Vol.37 (1), p.1-10
issn 0003-3472
1095-8282
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_15843668
source Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals
subjects Animal behavior
Animal ethology
Biological and medical sciences
Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology
Mammalia
Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry
Vertebrata
Wildcats
title Scent marking in free-ranging tigers, Panthera tigris
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-08T09%3A17%3A43IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Scent%20marking%20in%20free-ranging%20tigers,%20Panthera%20tigris&rft.jtitle=Animal%20behaviour&rft.au=David%20Smith,%20James%20L.&rft.date=1989&rft.volume=37&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=1&rft.epage=10&rft.pages=1-10&rft.issn=0003-3472&rft.eissn=1095-8282&rft.coden=ANBEA8&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/0003-3472(89)90001-8&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E5762575%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=224569722&rft_id=info:pmid/&rft_els_id=0003347289900018&rfr_iscdi=true