The evolutionary origin of human hyper-cooperation
Proactive, that is, unsolicited, prosociality is a key component of our hyper-cooperation, which in turn has enabled the emergence of various uniquely human traits, including complex cognition, morality and cumulative culture and technology. However, the evolutionary foundation of the human prosocia...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Nature communications 2014-08, Vol.5 (1), p.4747-4747, Article 4747 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext bestellen |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 4747 |
---|---|
container_issue | 1 |
container_start_page | 4747 |
container_title | Nature communications |
container_volume | 5 |
creator | Burkart, J. M. Allon, O. Amici, F. Fichtel, C. Finkenwirth, C. Heschl, A. Huber, J. Isler, K. Kosonen, Z. K. Martins, E. Meulman, E.J. Richiger, R. Rueth, K. Spillmann, B. Wiesendanger, S. van Schaik, C. P. |
description | Proactive, that is, unsolicited, prosociality is a key component of our hyper-cooperation, which in turn has enabled the emergence of various uniquely human traits, including complex cognition, morality and cumulative culture and technology. However, the evolutionary foundation of the human prosocial sentiment remains poorly understood, largely because primate data from numerous, often incommensurable testing paradigms do not provide an adequate basis for formal tests of the various functional hypotheses. We therefore present the results of standardized prosociality experiments in 24 groups of 15 primate species, including humans. Extensive allomaternal care is by far the best predictor of interspecific variation in proactive prosociality. Proactive prosocial motivations therefore systematically arise whenever selection favours the evolution of cooperative breeding. Because the human data fit this general primate pattern, the adoption of cooperative breeding by our hominin ancestors also provides the most parsimonious explanation for the origin of human hyper-cooperation.
The evolutionary foundation of human prosociality remains poorly understood. Here, the authors show that extensive allomaternal care is the best predictor of prosocial behaviour among 15 primate species, including humans, which suggests that prosocial motivations arise along with cooperative breeding. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1038/ncomms5747 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_C6C</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_1558525522</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>3414110081</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c453t-e09ba55a2909c2297586728839794239d9de7297930c3399ccd2a83090a42a903</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNpl0E1PAyEQBmBiNLapvfgDzCZejGYVBljgaBq_kiZe6nlDWdpuswsVuib996Vp1Ua5DIEn78AgdEnwPcFUPjjj2zZywcQJ6gNmJCcC6OnRvoeGMS5xWlQRydg56gEnXIoC9xFMFjazX77p1rV3OmwyH-p57TI_yxZdq1222KxsyI33qegdukBnM91EOzzUAfp4fpqMXvPx-8vb6HGcG8bpOrdYTTXnGhRWBkAJLgsBUlIlFAOqKlVZkY4VxYZSpYypQEuKFdYMtMJ0gG72uavgPzsb12VbR2ObRjvru1gSziUHzgESvf5Dl74LLr1upwqQIAqZ1O1emeBjDHZWrkLdpj-XBJe7YZa_w0z46hDZTVtb_dDv0SVwtwcxXbm5DUc9_8dtATwnfBA</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1556282768</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>The evolutionary origin of human hyper-cooperation</title><source>SpringerOpen</source><creator>Burkart, J. M. ; Allon, O. ; Amici, F. ; Fichtel, C. ; Finkenwirth, C. ; Heschl, A. ; Huber, J. ; Isler, K. ; Kosonen, Z. K. ; Martins, E. ; Meulman, E.J. ; Richiger, R. ; Rueth, K. ; Spillmann, B. ; Wiesendanger, S. ; van Schaik, C. P.</creator><creatorcontrib>Burkart, J. M. ; Allon, O. ; Amici, F. ; Fichtel, C. ; Finkenwirth, C. ; Heschl, A. ; Huber, J. ; Isler, K. ; Kosonen, Z. K. ; Martins, E. ; Meulman, E.J. ; Richiger, R. ; Rueth, K. ; Spillmann, B. ; Wiesendanger, S. ; van Schaik, C. P.</creatorcontrib><description>Proactive, that is, unsolicited, prosociality is a key component of our hyper-cooperation, which in turn has enabled the emergence of various uniquely human traits, including complex cognition, morality and cumulative culture and technology. However, the evolutionary foundation of the human prosocial sentiment remains poorly understood, largely because primate data from numerous, often incommensurable testing paradigms do not provide an adequate basis for formal tests of the various functional hypotheses. We therefore present the results of standardized prosociality experiments in 24 groups of 15 primate species, including humans. Extensive allomaternal care is by far the best predictor of interspecific variation in proactive prosociality. Proactive prosocial motivations therefore systematically arise whenever selection favours the evolution of cooperative breeding. Because the human data fit this general primate pattern, the adoption of cooperative breeding by our hominin ancestors also provides the most parsimonious explanation for the origin of human hyper-cooperation.
The evolutionary foundation of human prosociality remains poorly understood. Here, the authors show that extensive allomaternal care is the best predictor of prosocial behaviour among 15 primate species, including humans, which suggests that prosocial motivations arise along with cooperative breeding.</description><identifier>ISSN: 2041-1723</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 2041-1723</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1038/ncomms5747</identifier><identifier>PMID: 25158760</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>London: Nature Publishing Group UK</publisher><subject>631/181/2469 ; 631/378/2645/2646 ; Animals ; Behavior, Animal ; Biological Evolution ; Child ; Child, Preschool ; Cooperative Behavior ; Female ; Humanities and Social Sciences ; Humans ; Male ; Motivation ; multidisciplinary ; Nontherapeutic Human Experimentation ; Primates - psychology ; Science ; Science (multidisciplinary)</subject><ispartof>Nature communications, 2014-08, Vol.5 (1), p.4747-4747, Article 4747</ispartof><rights>Springer Nature Limited 2014</rights><rights>Copyright Nature Publishing Group Aug 2014</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c453t-e09ba55a2909c2297586728839794239d9de7297930c3399ccd2a83090a42a903</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c453t-e09ba55a2909c2297586728839794239d9de7297930c3399ccd2a83090a42a903</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1038/ncomms5747$$EPDF$$P50$$Gspringer$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms5747$$EHTML$$P50$$Gspringer$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,27901,27902,41096,42165,51551</link.rule.ids><linktorsrc>$$Uhttps://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms5747$$EView_record_in_Springer_Nature$$FView_record_in_$$GSpringer_Nature</linktorsrc><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25158760$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Burkart, J. M.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Allon, O.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Amici, F.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Fichtel, C.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Finkenwirth, C.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Heschl, A.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Huber, J.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Isler, K.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kosonen, Z. K.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Martins, E.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Meulman, E.J.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Richiger, R.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Rueth, K.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Spillmann, B.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Wiesendanger, S.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>van Schaik, C. P.</creatorcontrib><title>The evolutionary origin of human hyper-cooperation</title><title>Nature communications</title><addtitle>Nat Commun</addtitle><addtitle>Nat Commun</addtitle><description>Proactive, that is, unsolicited, prosociality is a key component of our hyper-cooperation, which in turn has enabled the emergence of various uniquely human traits, including complex cognition, morality and cumulative culture and technology. However, the evolutionary foundation of the human prosocial sentiment remains poorly understood, largely because primate data from numerous, often incommensurable testing paradigms do not provide an adequate basis for formal tests of the various functional hypotheses. We therefore present the results of standardized prosociality experiments in 24 groups of 15 primate species, including humans. Extensive allomaternal care is by far the best predictor of interspecific variation in proactive prosociality. Proactive prosocial motivations therefore systematically arise whenever selection favours the evolution of cooperative breeding. Because the human data fit this general primate pattern, the adoption of cooperative breeding by our hominin ancestors also provides the most parsimonious explanation for the origin of human hyper-cooperation.
The evolutionary foundation of human prosociality remains poorly understood. Here, the authors show that extensive allomaternal care is the best predictor of prosocial behaviour among 15 primate species, including humans, which suggests that prosocial motivations arise along with cooperative breeding.</description><subject>631/181/2469</subject><subject>631/378/2645/2646</subject><subject>Animals</subject><subject>Behavior, Animal</subject><subject>Biological Evolution</subject><subject>Child</subject><subject>Child, Preschool</subject><subject>Cooperative Behavior</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Humanities and Social Sciences</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Motivation</subject><subject>multidisciplinary</subject><subject>Nontherapeutic Human Experimentation</subject><subject>Primates - psychology</subject><subject>Science</subject><subject>Science (multidisciplinary)</subject><issn>2041-1723</issn><issn>2041-1723</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2014</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><recordid>eNpl0E1PAyEQBmBiNLapvfgDzCZejGYVBljgaBq_kiZe6nlDWdpuswsVuib996Vp1Ua5DIEn78AgdEnwPcFUPjjj2zZywcQJ6gNmJCcC6OnRvoeGMS5xWlQRydg56gEnXIoC9xFMFjazX77p1rV3OmwyH-p57TI_yxZdq1222KxsyI33qegdukBnM91EOzzUAfp4fpqMXvPx-8vb6HGcG8bpOrdYTTXnGhRWBkAJLgsBUlIlFAOqKlVZkY4VxYZSpYypQEuKFdYMtMJ0gG72uavgPzsb12VbR2ObRjvru1gSziUHzgESvf5Dl74LLr1upwqQIAqZ1O1emeBjDHZWrkLdpj-XBJe7YZa_w0z46hDZTVtb_dDv0SVwtwcxXbm5DUc9_8dtATwnfBA</recordid><startdate>20140827</startdate><enddate>20140827</enddate><creator>Burkart, J. M.</creator><creator>Allon, O.</creator><creator>Amici, F.</creator><creator>Fichtel, C.</creator><creator>Finkenwirth, C.</creator><creator>Heschl, A.</creator><creator>Huber, J.</creator><creator>Isler, K.</creator><creator>Kosonen, Z. K.</creator><creator>Martins, E.</creator><creator>Meulman, E.J.</creator><creator>Richiger, R.</creator><creator>Rueth, K.</creator><creator>Spillmann, B.</creator><creator>Wiesendanger, S.</creator><creator>van Schaik, C. P.</creator><general>Nature Publishing Group UK</general><general>Nature Publishing Group</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QL</scope><scope>7QP</scope><scope>7QR</scope><scope>7SN</scope><scope>7SS</scope><scope>7ST</scope><scope>7T5</scope><scope>7T7</scope><scope>7TM</scope><scope>7TO</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FE</scope><scope>8FG</scope><scope>8FH</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AEUYN</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ARAPS</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BBNVY</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BGLVJ</scope><scope>BHPHI</scope><scope>C1K</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>H94</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>LK8</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M7P</scope><scope>P5Z</scope><scope>P62</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PIMPY</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>SOI</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20140827</creationdate><title>The evolutionary origin of human hyper-cooperation</title><author>Burkart, J. M. ; Allon, O. ; Amici, F. ; Fichtel, C. ; Finkenwirth, C. ; Heschl, A. ; Huber, J. ; Isler, K. ; Kosonen, Z. K. ; Martins, E. ; Meulman, E.J. ; Richiger, R. ; Rueth, K. ; Spillmann, B. ; Wiesendanger, S. ; van Schaik, C. P.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c453t-e09ba55a2909c2297586728839794239d9de7297930c3399ccd2a83090a42a903</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2014</creationdate><topic>631/181/2469</topic><topic>631/378/2645/2646</topic><topic>Animals</topic><topic>Behavior, Animal</topic><topic>Biological Evolution</topic><topic>Child</topic><topic>Child, Preschool</topic><topic>Cooperative Behavior</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Humanities and Social Sciences</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Motivation</topic><topic>multidisciplinary</topic><topic>Nontherapeutic Human Experimentation</topic><topic>Primates - psychology</topic><topic>Science</topic><topic>Science (multidisciplinary)</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Burkart, J. M.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Allon, O.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Amici, F.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Fichtel, C.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Finkenwirth, C.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Heschl, A.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Huber, J.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Isler, K.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kosonen, Z. K.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Martins, E.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Meulman, E.J.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Richiger, R.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Rueth, K.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Spillmann, B.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Wiesendanger, S.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>van Schaik, C. P.</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B)</collection><collection>Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts</collection><collection>Chemoreception Abstracts</collection><collection>Ecology Abstracts</collection><collection>Entomology Abstracts (Full archive)</collection><collection>Environment Abstracts</collection><collection>Immunology Abstracts</collection><collection>Industrial and Applied Microbiology Abstracts (Microbiology A)</collection><collection>Nucleic Acids Abstracts</collection><collection>Oncogenes and Growth Factors Abstracts</collection><collection>ProQuest_Health & Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>ProQuest SciTech Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Technology Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Sustainability</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Database (1962 - current)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Technology Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Biological Sciences</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>PML(ProQuest Medical Library)</collection><collection>ProQuest Biological Science Journals</collection><collection>ProQuest advanced technologies & aerospace journals</collection><collection>ProQuest Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>Publicly Available Content (ProQuest)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>Environment Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Nature communications</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext_linktorsrc</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Burkart, J. M.</au><au>Allon, O.</au><au>Amici, F.</au><au>Fichtel, C.</au><au>Finkenwirth, C.</au><au>Heschl, A.</au><au>Huber, J.</au><au>Isler, K.</au><au>Kosonen, Z. K.</au><au>Martins, E.</au><au>Meulman, E.J.</au><au>Richiger, R.</au><au>Rueth, K.</au><au>Spillmann, B.</au><au>Wiesendanger, S.</au><au>van Schaik, C. P.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>The evolutionary origin of human hyper-cooperation</atitle><jtitle>Nature communications</jtitle><stitle>Nat Commun</stitle><addtitle>Nat Commun</addtitle><date>2014-08-27</date><risdate>2014</risdate><volume>5</volume><issue>1</issue><spage>4747</spage><epage>4747</epage><pages>4747-4747</pages><artnum>4747</artnum><issn>2041-1723</issn><eissn>2041-1723</eissn><abstract>Proactive, that is, unsolicited, prosociality is a key component of our hyper-cooperation, which in turn has enabled the emergence of various uniquely human traits, including complex cognition, morality and cumulative culture and technology. However, the evolutionary foundation of the human prosocial sentiment remains poorly understood, largely because primate data from numerous, often incommensurable testing paradigms do not provide an adequate basis for formal tests of the various functional hypotheses. We therefore present the results of standardized prosociality experiments in 24 groups of 15 primate species, including humans. Extensive allomaternal care is by far the best predictor of interspecific variation in proactive prosociality. Proactive prosocial motivations therefore systematically arise whenever selection favours the evolution of cooperative breeding. Because the human data fit this general primate pattern, the adoption of cooperative breeding by our hominin ancestors also provides the most parsimonious explanation for the origin of human hyper-cooperation.
The evolutionary foundation of human prosociality remains poorly understood. Here, the authors show that extensive allomaternal care is the best predictor of prosocial behaviour among 15 primate species, including humans, which suggests that prosocial motivations arise along with cooperative breeding.</abstract><cop>London</cop><pub>Nature Publishing Group UK</pub><pmid>25158760</pmid><doi>10.1038/ncomms5747</doi><tpages>1</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext_linktorsrc |
identifier | ISSN: 2041-1723 |
ispartof | Nature communications, 2014-08, Vol.5 (1), p.4747-4747, Article 4747 |
issn | 2041-1723 2041-1723 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_1558525522 |
source | SpringerOpen |
subjects | 631/181/2469 631/378/2645/2646 Animals Behavior, Animal Biological Evolution Child Child, Preschool Cooperative Behavior Female Humanities and Social Sciences Humans Male Motivation multidisciplinary Nontherapeutic Human Experimentation Primates - psychology Science Science (multidisciplinary) |
title | The evolutionary origin of human hyper-cooperation |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-11T20%3A53%3A06IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_C6C&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=The%20evolutionary%20origin%20of%20human%20hyper-cooperation&rft.jtitle=Nature%20communications&rft.au=Burkart,%20J.%20M.&rft.date=2014-08-27&rft.volume=5&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=4747&rft.epage=4747&rft.pages=4747-4747&rft.artnum=4747&rft.issn=2041-1723&rft.eissn=2041-1723&rft_id=info:doi/10.1038/ncomms5747&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_C6C%3E3414110081%3C/proquest_C6C%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1556282768&rft_id=info:pmid/25158760&rfr_iscdi=true |