Saccade deficits in amnestic mild cognitive impairment resemble mild Alzheimer's disease
Alzheimer's disease (AD) is a disorder of progressive memory loss and executive dysfunction. Little is known about the progression from amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI; isolated memory loss) to AD. Studies have found impairments in mild‐stage AD and aMCI in specific tests of executive...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | The European journal of neuroscience 2014-06, Vol.39 (11), p.2000-2013 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 2013 |
---|---|
container_issue | 11 |
container_start_page | 2000 |
container_title | The European journal of neuroscience |
container_volume | 39 |
creator | Peltsch, Alicia Hemraj, Alisha Garcia, Angeles Munoz, Douglas P. |
description | Alzheimer's disease (AD) is a disorder of progressive memory loss and executive dysfunction. Little is known about the progression from amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI; isolated memory loss) to AD. Studies have found impairments in mild‐stage AD and aMCI in specific tests of executive function. Here, we used objective saccade tasks to determine if they can effectively assess executive function deficits otherwise assessed by neuropsychological testing. To determine which executive function deficits the saccade tasks are most sensitive to, we also investigated the relationship between performance on saccade tasks and neuropsychological test scores. Twenty‐two aMCI patients (63–90 years), 24 mild AD patients (61–87 years) and 76 healthy controls (60–85 years) performed a battery of neuropsychological tests, and two saccade tasks designed to probe sensory, motor and cognitive function. The prosaccade task requires a fast, automatic saccade toward an eccentric visual stimulus. The antisaccade task requires additional executive processing to inhibit the automatic prosaccade toward the stimulus, so that a voluntary saccade can be initiated to a location opposite the stimulus. Antisaccade performance was impaired similarly in aMCI and AD patients relative to controls; both groups were slower to initiate correct antisaccades and they made more direction errors (erroneous prosaccades), suggesting similar brain deficits. Scores on the Stroop task were inversely correlated with the percentage of short‐latency direction errors in the antisaccade task for controls and aMCI patients, whereas other more global measures of executive function were not related to saccade measures in any subject group. Our results show that the antisaccade task is useful for detecting executive dysfunction in aMCI and AD, especially dysfunction in selective attention. Saccade tasks may therefore have potential to assess executive dysfunction when use of neuropsychological tests is not possible.
The prosaccade task (look toward a peripheral stimulus) and antisaccade task (inhibit prosaccade to peripheral stimulus and instead initiate saccade to opposite location) are used to assess cognitive impairment. Disease‐specific patterns begin to emerge. Individual antisaccade traces for a representative 75 year old elderly control, a 76 year old aMCI patient, and a 75 year old AD patient are shown here. Solid lines = correct saccades, dotted lines = direction errors. S = stimulus, FP = fixati |
doi_str_mv | 10.1111/ejn.12617 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_1540224462</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>1540224462</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c4627-412aa08a169087cdfb1c3706cb8a0eb11b17e9b5de137dbea3af156b893ce9df3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqNkE1P3DAQhi1UBFvg0D9Q5VZ6CHhix3aOiK8WrQCprbriYvlj0prG2cXOtqW_vikBbpWYy1ye95nRS8gboAcwziHe9gdQCZAbZAZc0LKphXpFZrSpWalALLbJ65xvKaVK8HqLbFdcNZRLmJHFJ-Oc8Vh4bIMLQy5CX5jYYx6CK2LofOGW3_owhJ9YhLgyIUXshyJhxmg7nJCj7s93DBHTu1z4kNFk3CWbreky7j3uHfLl7PTz8YdyfnX-8fhoXjouKllyqIyhyoBoqJLOtxYck1Q4qwxFC2BBYmNrj8Ckt2iYaaEWVjXMYeNbtkP2J-8qLe_W49s6huyw60yPy3XWUHNaVXw89gKUVVxKqcSIvp9Ql5Y5J2z1KoVo0r0Gqv91rsfO9UPnI_v2Ubu2Ef0z-VTyCBxOwK_Q4f3_Tfr04vJJWU6JkAf8_Zww6YcWkslaf70819XF9fxkcXaj5-wvXqea-A</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1532477786</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Saccade deficits in amnestic mild cognitive impairment resemble mild Alzheimer's disease</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>Wiley Journals</source><creator>Peltsch, Alicia ; Hemraj, Alisha ; Garcia, Angeles ; Munoz, Douglas P.</creator><creatorcontrib>Peltsch, Alicia ; Hemraj, Alisha ; Garcia, Angeles ; Munoz, Douglas P.</creatorcontrib><description>Alzheimer's disease (AD) is a disorder of progressive memory loss and executive dysfunction. Little is known about the progression from amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI; isolated memory loss) to AD. Studies have found impairments in mild‐stage AD and aMCI in specific tests of executive function. Here, we used objective saccade tasks to determine if they can effectively assess executive function deficits otherwise assessed by neuropsychological testing. To determine which executive function deficits the saccade tasks are most sensitive to, we also investigated the relationship between performance on saccade tasks and neuropsychological test scores. Twenty‐two aMCI patients (63–90 years), 24 mild AD patients (61–87 years) and 76 healthy controls (60–85 years) performed a battery of neuropsychological tests, and two saccade tasks designed to probe sensory, motor and cognitive function. The prosaccade task requires a fast, automatic saccade toward an eccentric visual stimulus. The antisaccade task requires additional executive processing to inhibit the automatic prosaccade toward the stimulus, so that a voluntary saccade can be initiated to a location opposite the stimulus. Antisaccade performance was impaired similarly in aMCI and AD patients relative to controls; both groups were slower to initiate correct antisaccades and they made more direction errors (erroneous prosaccades), suggesting similar brain deficits. Scores on the Stroop task were inversely correlated with the percentage of short‐latency direction errors in the antisaccade task for controls and aMCI patients, whereas other more global measures of executive function were not related to saccade measures in any subject group. Our results show that the antisaccade task is useful for detecting executive dysfunction in aMCI and AD, especially dysfunction in selective attention. Saccade tasks may therefore have potential to assess executive dysfunction when use of neuropsychological tests is not possible.
The prosaccade task (look toward a peripheral stimulus) and antisaccade task (inhibit prosaccade to peripheral stimulus and instead initiate saccade to opposite location) are used to assess cognitive impairment. Disease‐specific patterns begin to emerge. Individual antisaccade traces for a representative 75 year old elderly control, a 76 year old aMCI patient, and a 75 year old AD patient are shown here. Solid lines = correct saccades, dotted lines = direction errors. S = stimulus, FP = fixation point.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0953-816X</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1460-9568</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1111/ejn.12617</identifier><identifier>PMID: 24890471</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>France: Blackwell Publishing Ltd</publisher><subject>Aged ; Aged, 80 and over ; aging ; Alzheimer Disease - physiopathology ; Amnesia - physiopathology ; Case-Control Studies ; Cognitive Dysfunction - physiopathology ; dementia ; Executive Function ; eye movements ; Female ; Humans ; Male ; Middle Aged ; Psychomotor Performance ; Reaction Time ; saccade ; Saccades ; Stroop Test</subject><ispartof>The European journal of neuroscience, 2014-06, Vol.39 (11), p.2000-2013</ispartof><rights>2014 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd</rights><rights>2014 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c4627-412aa08a169087cdfb1c3706cb8a0eb11b17e9b5de137dbea3af156b893ce9df3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c4627-412aa08a169087cdfb1c3706cb8a0eb11b17e9b5de137dbea3af156b893ce9df3</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/pdf/10.1111%2Fejn.12617$$EPDF$$P50$$Gwiley$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1111%2Fejn.12617$$EHTML$$P50$$Gwiley$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,1417,27924,27925,45574,45575</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24890471$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Peltsch, Alicia</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Hemraj, Alisha</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Garcia, Angeles</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Munoz, Douglas P.</creatorcontrib><title>Saccade deficits in amnestic mild cognitive impairment resemble mild Alzheimer's disease</title><title>The European journal of neuroscience</title><addtitle>Eur J Neurosci</addtitle><description>Alzheimer's disease (AD) is a disorder of progressive memory loss and executive dysfunction. Little is known about the progression from amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI; isolated memory loss) to AD. Studies have found impairments in mild‐stage AD and aMCI in specific tests of executive function. Here, we used objective saccade tasks to determine if they can effectively assess executive function deficits otherwise assessed by neuropsychological testing. To determine which executive function deficits the saccade tasks are most sensitive to, we also investigated the relationship between performance on saccade tasks and neuropsychological test scores. Twenty‐two aMCI patients (63–90 years), 24 mild AD patients (61–87 years) and 76 healthy controls (60–85 years) performed a battery of neuropsychological tests, and two saccade tasks designed to probe sensory, motor and cognitive function. The prosaccade task requires a fast, automatic saccade toward an eccentric visual stimulus. The antisaccade task requires additional executive processing to inhibit the automatic prosaccade toward the stimulus, so that a voluntary saccade can be initiated to a location opposite the stimulus. Antisaccade performance was impaired similarly in aMCI and AD patients relative to controls; both groups were slower to initiate correct antisaccades and they made more direction errors (erroneous prosaccades), suggesting similar brain deficits. Scores on the Stroop task were inversely correlated with the percentage of short‐latency direction errors in the antisaccade task for controls and aMCI patients, whereas other more global measures of executive function were not related to saccade measures in any subject group. Our results show that the antisaccade task is useful for detecting executive dysfunction in aMCI and AD, especially dysfunction in selective attention. Saccade tasks may therefore have potential to assess executive dysfunction when use of neuropsychological tests is not possible.
The prosaccade task (look toward a peripheral stimulus) and antisaccade task (inhibit prosaccade to peripheral stimulus and instead initiate saccade to opposite location) are used to assess cognitive impairment. Disease‐specific patterns begin to emerge. Individual antisaccade traces for a representative 75 year old elderly control, a 76 year old aMCI patient, and a 75 year old AD patient are shown here. Solid lines = correct saccades, dotted lines = direction errors. S = stimulus, FP = fixation point.</description><subject>Aged</subject><subject>Aged, 80 and over</subject><subject>aging</subject><subject>Alzheimer Disease - physiopathology</subject><subject>Amnesia - physiopathology</subject><subject>Case-Control Studies</subject><subject>Cognitive Dysfunction - physiopathology</subject><subject>dementia</subject><subject>Executive Function</subject><subject>eye movements</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Middle Aged</subject><subject>Psychomotor Performance</subject><subject>Reaction Time</subject><subject>saccade</subject><subject>Saccades</subject><subject>Stroop Test</subject><issn>0953-816X</issn><issn>1460-9568</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2014</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><recordid>eNqNkE1P3DAQhi1UBFvg0D9Q5VZ6CHhix3aOiK8WrQCprbriYvlj0prG2cXOtqW_vikBbpWYy1ye95nRS8gboAcwziHe9gdQCZAbZAZc0LKphXpFZrSpWalALLbJ65xvKaVK8HqLbFdcNZRLmJHFJ-Oc8Vh4bIMLQy5CX5jYYx6CK2LofOGW3_owhJ9YhLgyIUXshyJhxmg7nJCj7s93DBHTu1z4kNFk3CWbreky7j3uHfLl7PTz8YdyfnX-8fhoXjouKllyqIyhyoBoqJLOtxYck1Q4qwxFC2BBYmNrj8Ckt2iYaaEWVjXMYeNbtkP2J-8qLe_W49s6huyw60yPy3XWUHNaVXw89gKUVVxKqcSIvp9Ql5Y5J2z1KoVo0r0Gqv91rsfO9UPnI_v2Ubu2Ef0z-VTyCBxOwK_Q4f3_Tfr04vJJWU6JkAf8_Zww6YcWkslaf70819XF9fxkcXaj5-wvXqea-A</recordid><startdate>201406</startdate><enddate>201406</enddate><creator>Peltsch, Alicia</creator><creator>Hemraj, Alisha</creator><creator>Garcia, Angeles</creator><creator>Munoz, Douglas P.</creator><general>Blackwell Publishing Ltd</general><scope>BSCLL</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>7TK</scope></search><sort><creationdate>201406</creationdate><title>Saccade deficits in amnestic mild cognitive impairment resemble mild Alzheimer's disease</title><author>Peltsch, Alicia ; Hemraj, Alisha ; Garcia, Angeles ; Munoz, Douglas P.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c4627-412aa08a169087cdfb1c3706cb8a0eb11b17e9b5de137dbea3af156b893ce9df3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2014</creationdate><topic>Aged</topic><topic>Aged, 80 and over</topic><topic>aging</topic><topic>Alzheimer Disease - physiopathology</topic><topic>Amnesia - physiopathology</topic><topic>Case-Control Studies</topic><topic>Cognitive Dysfunction - physiopathology</topic><topic>dementia</topic><topic>Executive Function</topic><topic>eye movements</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Middle Aged</topic><topic>Psychomotor Performance</topic><topic>Reaction Time</topic><topic>saccade</topic><topic>Saccades</topic><topic>Stroop Test</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Peltsch, Alicia</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Hemraj, Alisha</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Garcia, Angeles</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Munoz, Douglas P.</creatorcontrib><collection>Istex</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><jtitle>The European journal of neuroscience</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Peltsch, Alicia</au><au>Hemraj, Alisha</au><au>Garcia, Angeles</au><au>Munoz, Douglas P.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Saccade deficits in amnestic mild cognitive impairment resemble mild Alzheimer's disease</atitle><jtitle>The European journal of neuroscience</jtitle><addtitle>Eur J Neurosci</addtitle><date>2014-06</date><risdate>2014</risdate><volume>39</volume><issue>11</issue><spage>2000</spage><epage>2013</epage><pages>2000-2013</pages><issn>0953-816X</issn><eissn>1460-9568</eissn><abstract>Alzheimer's disease (AD) is a disorder of progressive memory loss and executive dysfunction. Little is known about the progression from amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI; isolated memory loss) to AD. Studies have found impairments in mild‐stage AD and aMCI in specific tests of executive function. Here, we used objective saccade tasks to determine if they can effectively assess executive function deficits otherwise assessed by neuropsychological testing. To determine which executive function deficits the saccade tasks are most sensitive to, we also investigated the relationship between performance on saccade tasks and neuropsychological test scores. Twenty‐two aMCI patients (63–90 years), 24 mild AD patients (61–87 years) and 76 healthy controls (60–85 years) performed a battery of neuropsychological tests, and two saccade tasks designed to probe sensory, motor and cognitive function. The prosaccade task requires a fast, automatic saccade toward an eccentric visual stimulus. The antisaccade task requires additional executive processing to inhibit the automatic prosaccade toward the stimulus, so that a voluntary saccade can be initiated to a location opposite the stimulus. Antisaccade performance was impaired similarly in aMCI and AD patients relative to controls; both groups were slower to initiate correct antisaccades and they made more direction errors (erroneous prosaccades), suggesting similar brain deficits. Scores on the Stroop task were inversely correlated with the percentage of short‐latency direction errors in the antisaccade task for controls and aMCI patients, whereas other more global measures of executive function were not related to saccade measures in any subject group. Our results show that the antisaccade task is useful for detecting executive dysfunction in aMCI and AD, especially dysfunction in selective attention. Saccade tasks may therefore have potential to assess executive dysfunction when use of neuropsychological tests is not possible.
The prosaccade task (look toward a peripheral stimulus) and antisaccade task (inhibit prosaccade to peripheral stimulus and instead initiate saccade to opposite location) are used to assess cognitive impairment. Disease‐specific patterns begin to emerge. Individual antisaccade traces for a representative 75 year old elderly control, a 76 year old aMCI patient, and a 75 year old AD patient are shown here. Solid lines = correct saccades, dotted lines = direction errors. S = stimulus, FP = fixation point.</abstract><cop>France</cop><pub>Blackwell Publishing Ltd</pub><pmid>24890471</pmid><doi>10.1111/ejn.12617</doi><tpages>14</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0953-816X |
ispartof | The European journal of neuroscience, 2014-06, Vol.39 (11), p.2000-2013 |
issn | 0953-816X 1460-9568 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_1540224462 |
source | MEDLINE; Wiley Journals |
subjects | Aged Aged, 80 and over aging Alzheimer Disease - physiopathology Amnesia - physiopathology Case-Control Studies Cognitive Dysfunction - physiopathology dementia Executive Function eye movements Female Humans Male Middle Aged Psychomotor Performance Reaction Time saccade Saccades Stroop Test |
title | Saccade deficits in amnestic mild cognitive impairment resemble mild Alzheimer's disease |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-30T23%3A33%3A45IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Saccade%20deficits%20in%20amnestic%20mild%20cognitive%20impairment%20resemble%20mild%20Alzheimer's%20disease&rft.jtitle=The%20European%20journal%20of%20neuroscience&rft.au=Peltsch,%20Alicia&rft.date=2014-06&rft.volume=39&rft.issue=11&rft.spage=2000&rft.epage=2013&rft.pages=2000-2013&rft.issn=0953-816X&rft.eissn=1460-9568&rft_id=info:doi/10.1111/ejn.12617&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E1540224462%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1532477786&rft_id=info:pmid/24890471&rfr_iscdi=true |