Principles of spatial problem solving
Evaluated the hypotheses that (a) spatial learning produces a cognitive map and (b) this map is picturelike. It was also hypothesized that special properties of pictures would be demonstrated by the behavior of the Ss. The special properties were (a) simultaneous representation of sequentially place...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Journal of experimental psychology. General 1982-06, Vol.111 (2), p.157-175 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 175 |
---|---|
container_issue | 2 |
container_start_page | 157 |
container_title | Journal of experimental psychology. General |
container_volume | 111 |
creator | Levine, Marvin Jankovic, Irwin N Palij, Michael |
description | Evaluated the hypotheses that (a) spatial learning produces a cognitive map and (b) this map is picturelike. It was also hypothesized that special properties of pictures would be demonstrated by the behavior of the Ss. The special properties were (a) simultaneous representation of sequentially placed points and (b) orientation. 106 blindfolded college students learned simple paths either by moving their fingers over the successive points of a map of the path, walking through the path laid out on the floor, or (with blindfold temporarily removed) viewing a map of the path. They were tested for knowledge of the path by having to locate a target; still blindfolded, they were placed at a point on that path and required to move to another point on the path. This required either moving toward the next point in the sequence or taking a shortcut. It is concluded that Ss had an internal pictorial map since they took shortcuts with the same ease as they took the originally learned path segments. The manifestation of orientation was particularly dramatic, with Ss moving in the wrong direction (angle error greater than 90°) on more than 25% of the specified trials. (39 ref) |
doi_str_mv | 10.1037/0096-3445.111.2.157 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_journals_614302932</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>1297422476</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-a332t-817977b79acd4f0363609b16959a051a95bcf89fa9f376a2d3d5d9949224c6c03</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp1kE1LAzEURYMoWKu_wM2gdDljkpdJ-pZS_IKCLnQd3mQmZcq0MyZT0X9vSkXceDdvc-59cBi7FLwQHMwN56hzUKoshBCFLERpjthEIGAuU47Z5Jc4ZWcxrnkKzPWEzV5Cu3Xt0DUx630WBxpb6rIh9FXXbLLYdx_tdnXOTjx1sbn4uVP2dn_3unjMl88PT4vbZU4AcsznwqAxlUFytfIcNGiOldBYIvFSEJaV83P0hB6MJllDXdaICqVUTjsOU3Z12E3_33dNHO2634Vtemm1UMAlgkzQ9X-QkGhUGjM6UXCgXOhjDI23Q2g3FL6s4HYvze6V2L0Sm6RZaZO01JodWjSQHeKXozC2Lsmxn6vmD_cN80xo2w</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>614302932</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Principles of spatial problem solving</title><source>EBSCOhost APA PsycARTICLES</source><source>Periodicals Index Online</source><creator>Levine, Marvin ; Jankovic, Irwin N ; Palij, Michael</creator><creatorcontrib>Levine, Marvin ; Jankovic, Irwin N ; Palij, Michael</creatorcontrib><description>Evaluated the hypotheses that (a) spatial learning produces a cognitive map and (b) this map is picturelike. It was also hypothesized that special properties of pictures would be demonstrated by the behavior of the Ss. The special properties were (a) simultaneous representation of sequentially placed points and (b) orientation. 106 blindfolded college students learned simple paths either by moving their fingers over the successive points of a map of the path, walking through the path laid out on the floor, or (with blindfold temporarily removed) viewing a map of the path. They were tested for knowledge of the path by having to locate a target; still blindfolded, they were placed at a point on that path and required to move to another point on the path. This required either moving toward the next point in the sequence or taking a shortcut. It is concluded that Ss had an internal pictorial map since they took shortcuts with the same ease as they took the originally learned path segments. The manifestation of orientation was particularly dramatic, with Ss moving in the wrong direction (angle error greater than 90°) on more than 25% of the specified trials. (39 ref)</description><identifier>ISSN: 0096-3445</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1939-2222</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.111.2.157</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Washington, etc: American Psychological Association</publisher><subject>Cognitive Maps ; Human ; Problem Solving ; Spatial Imagery</subject><ispartof>Journal of experimental psychology. General, 1982-06, Vol.111 (2), p.157-175</ispartof><rights>1982 American Psychological Association</rights><rights>1982, American Psychological Association</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-a332t-817977b79acd4f0363609b16959a051a95bcf89fa9f376a2d3d5d9949224c6c03</citedby></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,778,782,27852,27907,27908</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Levine, Marvin</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Jankovic, Irwin N</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Palij, Michael</creatorcontrib><title>Principles of spatial problem solving</title><title>Journal of experimental psychology. General</title><description>Evaluated the hypotheses that (a) spatial learning produces a cognitive map and (b) this map is picturelike. It was also hypothesized that special properties of pictures would be demonstrated by the behavior of the Ss. The special properties were (a) simultaneous representation of sequentially placed points and (b) orientation. 106 blindfolded college students learned simple paths either by moving their fingers over the successive points of a map of the path, walking through the path laid out on the floor, or (with blindfold temporarily removed) viewing a map of the path. They were tested for knowledge of the path by having to locate a target; still blindfolded, they were placed at a point on that path and required to move to another point on the path. This required either moving toward the next point in the sequence or taking a shortcut. It is concluded that Ss had an internal pictorial map since they took shortcuts with the same ease as they took the originally learned path segments. The manifestation of orientation was particularly dramatic, with Ss moving in the wrong direction (angle error greater than 90°) on more than 25% of the specified trials. (39 ref)</description><subject>Cognitive Maps</subject><subject>Human</subject><subject>Problem Solving</subject><subject>Spatial Imagery</subject><issn>0096-3445</issn><issn>1939-2222</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1982</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>K30</sourceid><recordid>eNp1kE1LAzEURYMoWKu_wM2gdDljkpdJ-pZS_IKCLnQd3mQmZcq0MyZT0X9vSkXceDdvc-59cBi7FLwQHMwN56hzUKoshBCFLERpjthEIGAuU47Z5Jc4ZWcxrnkKzPWEzV5Cu3Xt0DUx630WBxpb6rIh9FXXbLLYdx_tdnXOTjx1sbn4uVP2dn_3unjMl88PT4vbZU4AcsznwqAxlUFytfIcNGiOldBYIvFSEJaV83P0hB6MJllDXdaICqVUTjsOU3Z12E3_33dNHO2634Vtemm1UMAlgkzQ9X-QkGhUGjM6UXCgXOhjDI23Q2g3FL6s4HYvze6V2L0Sm6RZaZO01JodWjSQHeKXozC2Lsmxn6vmD_cN80xo2w</recordid><startdate>198206</startdate><enddate>198206</enddate><creator>Levine, Marvin</creator><creator>Jankovic, Irwin N</creator><creator>Palij, Michael</creator><general>American Psychological Association</general><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>FUVTR</scope><scope>IZSXY</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope><scope>7RZ</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope></search><sort><creationdate>198206</creationdate><title>Principles of spatial problem solving</title><author>Levine, Marvin ; Jankovic, Irwin N ; Palij, Michael</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a332t-817977b79acd4f0363609b16959a051a95bcf89fa9f376a2d3d5d9949224c6c03</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1982</creationdate><topic>Cognitive Maps</topic><topic>Human</topic><topic>Problem Solving</topic><topic>Spatial Imagery</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Levine, Marvin</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Jankovic, Irwin N</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Palij, Michael</creatorcontrib><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 06</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 30</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><collection>APA PsycArticles®</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><jtitle>Journal of experimental psychology. General</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Levine, Marvin</au><au>Jankovic, Irwin N</au><au>Palij, Michael</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Principles of spatial problem solving</atitle><jtitle>Journal of experimental psychology. General</jtitle><date>1982-06</date><risdate>1982</risdate><volume>111</volume><issue>2</issue><spage>157</spage><epage>175</epage><pages>157-175</pages><issn>0096-3445</issn><eissn>1939-2222</eissn><abstract>Evaluated the hypotheses that (a) spatial learning produces a cognitive map and (b) this map is picturelike. It was also hypothesized that special properties of pictures would be demonstrated by the behavior of the Ss. The special properties were (a) simultaneous representation of sequentially placed points and (b) orientation. 106 blindfolded college students learned simple paths either by moving their fingers over the successive points of a map of the path, walking through the path laid out on the floor, or (with blindfold temporarily removed) viewing a map of the path. They were tested for knowledge of the path by having to locate a target; still blindfolded, they were placed at a point on that path and required to move to another point on the path. This required either moving toward the next point in the sequence or taking a shortcut. It is concluded that Ss had an internal pictorial map since they took shortcuts with the same ease as they took the originally learned path segments. The manifestation of orientation was particularly dramatic, with Ss moving in the wrong direction (angle error greater than 90°) on more than 25% of the specified trials. (39 ref)</abstract><cop>Washington, etc</cop><pub>American Psychological Association</pub><doi>10.1037/0096-3445.111.2.157</doi><tpages>19</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0096-3445 |
ispartof | Journal of experimental psychology. General, 1982-06, Vol.111 (2), p.157-175 |
issn | 0096-3445 1939-2222 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_journals_614302932 |
source | EBSCOhost APA PsycARTICLES; Periodicals Index Online |
subjects | Cognitive Maps Human Problem Solving Spatial Imagery |
title | Principles of spatial problem solving |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-16T23%3A30%3A38IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Principles%20of%20spatial%20problem%20solving&rft.jtitle=Journal%20of%20experimental%20psychology.%20General&rft.au=Levine,%20Marvin&rft.date=1982-06&rft.volume=111&rft.issue=2&rft.spage=157&rft.epage=175&rft.pages=157-175&rft.issn=0096-3445&rft.eissn=1939-2222&rft_id=info:doi/10.1037/0096-3445.111.2.157&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E1297422476%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=614302932&rft_id=info:pmid/&rfr_iscdi=true |