Prehension force as a measure of psychomotor skill for bare and gloved hands

"The task consisted of a simple grasp, transport, and release of a cylindrical object into designated holes of a form-board. This object was instrumented with a pressure transducer permitting continuous recording of grasp force variations . . . . (a) handcoverings; (b) object weight; (c) distan...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Journal of applied psychology 1958-02, Vol.42 (1), p.18-21
Hauptverfasser: Lyman, John, Groth, Hilde
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 21
container_issue 1
container_start_page 18
container_title Journal of applied psychology
container_volume 42
creator Lyman, John
Groth, Hilde
description "The task consisted of a simple grasp, transport, and release of a cylindrical object into designated holes of a form-board. This object was instrumented with a pressure transducer permitting continuous recording of grasp force variations . . . . (a) handcoverings; (b) object weight; (c) distance moved; (d) direction of movement . . . . [were investigated in a factorial design with 6 Ss] handcoverings, weight, and distance exert a significant effect upon prehension force during a given task . . . this measurement seems to have potential value as an index of motor skills in that it is probably sensitive to changes in the amount of tactile sensory information available as well as to physical variables of the task."
doi_str_mv 10.1037/h0044064
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_journals_614250893</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>1290387666</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-a308t-99e24d0ab9ee6f308175f63c378ee3051a0ff46f01e60375da0724ac5cfc28e73</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp1kE1PwzAMhiMEEmMg8RMiuHApOE2aNkc08SVNggOcIy91aEe3lKRD2r-n00CcONmyH72WH8bOBVwLkOVNA6AUaHXAJsJIk4mqUIdsApCLzICAY3aS0hJAKGlgwuYvkRpapzasuQ_REcfEka8I0yYSD573aeuasApDiDx9tF234_gCxy2ua_7ehS-qeTP26ZQdeewSnf3UKXu7v3udPWbz54en2e08QwnVkBlDuaoBF4ZI-3EkysJr6WRZEUkoBIL3SnsQpMeXihqhzBW6wnmXV1TKKbvY5_YxfG4oDXYZNnE9nrRaqLyAysgRuvwPErkBWZVa65G62lMuhpQiedvHdoVxawXYnVD7K_QvEHu0OysYh9Z1lCz2nVW5FVZU8huDTHJm</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>614250893</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Prehension force as a measure of psychomotor skill for bare and gloved hands</title><source>EBSCOhost APA PsycARTICLES</source><source>Periodicals Index Online</source><creator>Lyman, John ; Groth, Hilde</creator><creatorcontrib>Lyman, John ; Groth, Hilde</creatorcontrib><description>"The task consisted of a simple grasp, transport, and release of a cylindrical object into designated holes of a form-board. This object was instrumented with a pressure transducer permitting continuous recording of grasp force variations . . . . (a) handcoverings; (b) object weight; (c) distance moved; (d) direction of movement . . . . [were investigated in a factorial design with 6 Ss] handcoverings, weight, and distance exert a significant effect upon prehension force during a given task . . . this measurement seems to have potential value as an index of motor skills in that it is probably sensitive to changes in the amount of tactile sensory information available as well as to physical variables of the task."</description><identifier>ISSN: 0021-9010</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1939-1854</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1037/h0044064</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Washington, etc: American Psychological Association</publisher><subject>Human ; Perceptual Motor Processes ; Tactual Perception</subject><ispartof>Journal of applied psychology, 1958-02, Vol.42 (1), p.18-21</ispartof><rights>1958 American Psychological Association</rights><rights>1958, American Psychological Association</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-a308t-99e24d0ab9ee6f308175f63c378ee3051a0ff46f01e60375da0724ac5cfc28e73</citedby></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,27846,27901,27902</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Lyman, John</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Groth, Hilde</creatorcontrib><title>Prehension force as a measure of psychomotor skill for bare and gloved hands</title><title>Journal of applied psychology</title><description>"The task consisted of a simple grasp, transport, and release of a cylindrical object into designated holes of a form-board. This object was instrumented with a pressure transducer permitting continuous recording of grasp force variations . . . . (a) handcoverings; (b) object weight; (c) distance moved; (d) direction of movement . . . . [were investigated in a factorial design with 6 Ss] handcoverings, weight, and distance exert a significant effect upon prehension force during a given task . . . this measurement seems to have potential value as an index of motor skills in that it is probably sensitive to changes in the amount of tactile sensory information available as well as to physical variables of the task."</description><subject>Human</subject><subject>Perceptual Motor Processes</subject><subject>Tactual Perception</subject><issn>0021-9010</issn><issn>1939-1854</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1958</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>K30</sourceid><recordid>eNp1kE1PwzAMhiMEEmMg8RMiuHApOE2aNkc08SVNggOcIy91aEe3lKRD2r-n00CcONmyH72WH8bOBVwLkOVNA6AUaHXAJsJIk4mqUIdsApCLzICAY3aS0hJAKGlgwuYvkRpapzasuQ_REcfEka8I0yYSD573aeuasApDiDx9tF234_gCxy2ua_7ehS-qeTP26ZQdeewSnf3UKXu7v3udPWbz54en2e08QwnVkBlDuaoBF4ZI-3EkysJr6WRZEUkoBIL3SnsQpMeXihqhzBW6wnmXV1TKKbvY5_YxfG4oDXYZNnE9nrRaqLyAysgRuvwPErkBWZVa65G62lMuhpQiedvHdoVxawXYnVD7K_QvEHu0OysYh9Z1lCz2nVW5FVZU8huDTHJm</recordid><startdate>195802</startdate><enddate>195802</enddate><creator>Lyman, John</creator><creator>Groth, Hilde</creator><general>American Psychological Association</general><general>American Psychological Association, etc</general><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>EOLOZ</scope><scope>FKUCP</scope><scope>IOIBA</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope><scope>7RZ</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope></search><sort><creationdate>195802</creationdate><title>Prehension force as a measure of psychomotor skill for bare and gloved hands</title><author>Lyman, John ; Groth, Hilde</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a308t-99e24d0ab9ee6f308175f63c378ee3051a0ff46f01e60375da0724ac5cfc28e73</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1958</creationdate><topic>Human</topic><topic>Perceptual Motor Processes</topic><topic>Tactual Perception</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Lyman, John</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Groth, Hilde</creatorcontrib><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 01</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 04</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 29</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><collection>APA PsycArticles®</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><jtitle>Journal of applied psychology</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Lyman, John</au><au>Groth, Hilde</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Prehension force as a measure of psychomotor skill for bare and gloved hands</atitle><jtitle>Journal of applied psychology</jtitle><date>1958-02</date><risdate>1958</risdate><volume>42</volume><issue>1</issue><spage>18</spage><epage>21</epage><pages>18-21</pages><issn>0021-9010</issn><eissn>1939-1854</eissn><abstract>"The task consisted of a simple grasp, transport, and release of a cylindrical object into designated holes of a form-board. This object was instrumented with a pressure transducer permitting continuous recording of grasp force variations . . . . (a) handcoverings; (b) object weight; (c) distance moved; (d) direction of movement . . . . [were investigated in a factorial design with 6 Ss] handcoverings, weight, and distance exert a significant effect upon prehension force during a given task . . . this measurement seems to have potential value as an index of motor skills in that it is probably sensitive to changes in the amount of tactile sensory information available as well as to physical variables of the task."</abstract><cop>Washington, etc</cop><pub>American Psychological Association</pub><doi>10.1037/h0044064</doi><tpages>4</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0021-9010
ispartof Journal of applied psychology, 1958-02, Vol.42 (1), p.18-21
issn 0021-9010
1939-1854
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_journals_614250893
source EBSCOhost APA PsycARTICLES; Periodicals Index Online
subjects Human
Perceptual Motor Processes
Tactual Perception
title Prehension force as a measure of psychomotor skill for bare and gloved hands
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-05T11%3A17%3A55IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Prehension%20force%20as%20a%20measure%20of%20psychomotor%20skill%20for%20bare%20and%20gloved%20hands&rft.jtitle=Journal%20of%20applied%20psychology&rft.au=Lyman,%20John&rft.date=1958-02&rft.volume=42&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=18&rft.epage=21&rft.pages=18-21&rft.issn=0021-9010&rft.eissn=1939-1854&rft_id=info:doi/10.1037/h0044064&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E1290387666%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=614250893&rft_id=info:pmid/&rfr_iscdi=true