Prehension force as a measure of psychomotor skill for bare and gloved hands
"The task consisted of a simple grasp, transport, and release of a cylindrical object into designated holes of a form-board. This object was instrumented with a pressure transducer permitting continuous recording of grasp force variations . . . . (a) handcoverings; (b) object weight; (c) distan...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Journal of applied psychology 1958-02, Vol.42 (1), p.18-21 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 21 |
---|---|
container_issue | 1 |
container_start_page | 18 |
container_title | Journal of applied psychology |
container_volume | 42 |
creator | Lyman, John Groth, Hilde |
description | "The task consisted of a simple grasp, transport, and release of a cylindrical object into designated holes of a form-board. This object was instrumented with a pressure transducer permitting continuous recording of grasp force variations . . . . (a) handcoverings; (b) object weight; (c) distance moved; (d) direction of movement . . . . [were investigated in a factorial design with 6 Ss] handcoverings, weight, and distance exert a significant effect upon prehension force during a given task . . . this measurement seems to have potential value as an index of motor skills in that it is probably sensitive to changes in the amount of tactile sensory information available as well as to physical variables of the task." |
doi_str_mv | 10.1037/h0044064 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_journals_614250893</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>1290387666</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-a308t-99e24d0ab9ee6f308175f63c378ee3051a0ff46f01e60375da0724ac5cfc28e73</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp1kE1PwzAMhiMEEmMg8RMiuHApOE2aNkc08SVNggOcIy91aEe3lKRD2r-n00CcONmyH72WH8bOBVwLkOVNA6AUaHXAJsJIk4mqUIdsApCLzICAY3aS0hJAKGlgwuYvkRpapzasuQ_REcfEka8I0yYSD573aeuasApDiDx9tF234_gCxy2ua_7ehS-qeTP26ZQdeewSnf3UKXu7v3udPWbz54en2e08QwnVkBlDuaoBF4ZI-3EkysJr6WRZEUkoBIL3SnsQpMeXihqhzBW6wnmXV1TKKbvY5_YxfG4oDXYZNnE9nrRaqLyAysgRuvwPErkBWZVa65G62lMuhpQiedvHdoVxawXYnVD7K_QvEHu0OysYh9Z1lCz2nVW5FVZU8huDTHJm</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>614250893</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Prehension force as a measure of psychomotor skill for bare and gloved hands</title><source>EBSCOhost APA PsycARTICLES</source><source>Periodicals Index Online</source><creator>Lyman, John ; Groth, Hilde</creator><creatorcontrib>Lyman, John ; Groth, Hilde</creatorcontrib><description>"The task consisted of a simple grasp, transport, and release of a cylindrical object into designated holes of a form-board. This object was instrumented with a pressure transducer permitting continuous recording of grasp force variations . . . . (a) handcoverings; (b) object weight; (c) distance moved; (d) direction of movement . . . . [were investigated in a factorial design with 6 Ss] handcoverings, weight, and distance exert a significant effect upon prehension force during a given task . . . this measurement seems to have potential value as an index of motor skills in that it is probably sensitive to changes in the amount of tactile sensory information available as well as to physical variables of the task."</description><identifier>ISSN: 0021-9010</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1939-1854</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1037/h0044064</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Washington, etc: American Psychological Association</publisher><subject>Human ; Perceptual Motor Processes ; Tactual Perception</subject><ispartof>Journal of applied psychology, 1958-02, Vol.42 (1), p.18-21</ispartof><rights>1958 American Psychological Association</rights><rights>1958, American Psychological Association</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-a308t-99e24d0ab9ee6f308175f63c378ee3051a0ff46f01e60375da0724ac5cfc28e73</citedby></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,27846,27901,27902</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Lyman, John</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Groth, Hilde</creatorcontrib><title>Prehension force as a measure of psychomotor skill for bare and gloved hands</title><title>Journal of applied psychology</title><description>"The task consisted of a simple grasp, transport, and release of a cylindrical object into designated holes of a form-board. This object was instrumented with a pressure transducer permitting continuous recording of grasp force variations . . . . (a) handcoverings; (b) object weight; (c) distance moved; (d) direction of movement . . . . [were investigated in a factorial design with 6 Ss] handcoverings, weight, and distance exert a significant effect upon prehension force during a given task . . . this measurement seems to have potential value as an index of motor skills in that it is probably sensitive to changes in the amount of tactile sensory information available as well as to physical variables of the task."</description><subject>Human</subject><subject>Perceptual Motor Processes</subject><subject>Tactual Perception</subject><issn>0021-9010</issn><issn>1939-1854</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1958</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>K30</sourceid><recordid>eNp1kE1PwzAMhiMEEmMg8RMiuHApOE2aNkc08SVNggOcIy91aEe3lKRD2r-n00CcONmyH72WH8bOBVwLkOVNA6AUaHXAJsJIk4mqUIdsApCLzICAY3aS0hJAKGlgwuYvkRpapzasuQ_REcfEka8I0yYSD573aeuasApDiDx9tF234_gCxy2ua_7ehS-qeTP26ZQdeewSnf3UKXu7v3udPWbz54en2e08QwnVkBlDuaoBF4ZI-3EkysJr6WRZEUkoBIL3SnsQpMeXihqhzBW6wnmXV1TKKbvY5_YxfG4oDXYZNnE9nrRaqLyAysgRuvwPErkBWZVa65G62lMuhpQiedvHdoVxawXYnVD7K_QvEHu0OysYh9Z1lCz2nVW5FVZU8huDTHJm</recordid><startdate>195802</startdate><enddate>195802</enddate><creator>Lyman, John</creator><creator>Groth, Hilde</creator><general>American Psychological Association</general><general>American Psychological Association, etc</general><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>EOLOZ</scope><scope>FKUCP</scope><scope>IOIBA</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope><scope>7RZ</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope></search><sort><creationdate>195802</creationdate><title>Prehension force as a measure of psychomotor skill for bare and gloved hands</title><author>Lyman, John ; Groth, Hilde</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a308t-99e24d0ab9ee6f308175f63c378ee3051a0ff46f01e60375da0724ac5cfc28e73</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1958</creationdate><topic>Human</topic><topic>Perceptual Motor Processes</topic><topic>Tactual Perception</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Lyman, John</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Groth, Hilde</creatorcontrib><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 01</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 04</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 29</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><collection>APA PsycArticles®</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><jtitle>Journal of applied psychology</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Lyman, John</au><au>Groth, Hilde</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Prehension force as a measure of psychomotor skill for bare and gloved hands</atitle><jtitle>Journal of applied psychology</jtitle><date>1958-02</date><risdate>1958</risdate><volume>42</volume><issue>1</issue><spage>18</spage><epage>21</epage><pages>18-21</pages><issn>0021-9010</issn><eissn>1939-1854</eissn><abstract>"The task consisted of a simple grasp, transport, and release of a cylindrical object into designated holes of a form-board. This object was instrumented with a pressure transducer permitting continuous recording of grasp force variations . . . . (a) handcoverings; (b) object weight; (c) distance moved; (d) direction of movement . . . . [were investigated in a factorial design with 6 Ss] handcoverings, weight, and distance exert a significant effect upon prehension force during a given task . . . this measurement seems to have potential value as an index of motor skills in that it is probably sensitive to changes in the amount of tactile sensory information available as well as to physical variables of the task."</abstract><cop>Washington, etc</cop><pub>American Psychological Association</pub><doi>10.1037/h0044064</doi><tpages>4</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0021-9010 |
ispartof | Journal of applied psychology, 1958-02, Vol.42 (1), p.18-21 |
issn | 0021-9010 1939-1854 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_journals_614250893 |
source | EBSCOhost APA PsycARTICLES; Periodicals Index Online |
subjects | Human Perceptual Motor Processes Tactual Perception |
title | Prehension force as a measure of psychomotor skill for bare and gloved hands |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-05T11%3A17%3A55IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Prehension%20force%20as%20a%20measure%20of%20psychomotor%20skill%20for%20bare%20and%20gloved%20hands&rft.jtitle=Journal%20of%20applied%20psychology&rft.au=Lyman,%20John&rft.date=1958-02&rft.volume=42&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=18&rft.epage=21&rft.pages=18-21&rft.issn=0021-9010&rft.eissn=1939-1854&rft_id=info:doi/10.1037/h0044064&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E1290387666%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=614250893&rft_id=info:pmid/&rfr_iscdi=true |