Pro-Environmental Waste Receptacle Labeling Can Increase Recycling Contamination

Early research has suggested that point-of-disposal signage and recycling receptacle modifications can increase recycling rates (Austin et at, 1993). However, the once steady increase in overall recycling rates over the past few decades appears to be stalling, with the most recent data showing only...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Hauptverfasser: Catlin, Jesse R, Wang, Yitong, Manuel, Rommel J
Format: Tagungsbericht
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 502
container_issue
container_start_page 501
container_title
container_volume 46
creator Catlin, Jesse R
Wang, Yitong
Manuel, Rommel J
description Early research has suggested that point-of-disposal signage and recycling receptacle modifications can increase recycling rates (Austin et at, 1993). However, the once steady increase in overall recycling rates over the past few decades appears to be stalling, with the most recent data showing only a 0.6% increase from four years prior (US EPA, 2014). At the same time, there has been a substantial rise in the rate of contamination from placing unrecyclable materials in recycling receptacles, which has created significant additional costs in the follow-up processing (Groden, 2015). In four field and online experiments, we show that point-of-disposal signage aiming to encourage recycling (such as the term "landfill" on garbage bins) could also increase recycling contamination. Prior research suggests people are generally averse to being wasteful (Bolton & Alba, 2012) and that failure to recycle could engender feelings of guilt (Viscusi et al., 2011). Therefore, we reason that labeling garbage bins with pro-environmental terms could make emotions evoked by the negative consequences of garbage more salient and thus increase the likelihood that individuals will incorrectly categorize unrecyclable items as being recyclable. In study 1 (и = 259 observations), as part of a new product sampling event by a well-known brand of coffee and related products, we offered passersby on a university campus the opportunity to sample a carbonated beverage (served in an unrecyclable foam cup). The study site featured two swing-top bins to dispose of their used sample cups. The label on one bin was experimentally manipulated to be either "Landfill" or "Trash". The other bin was labeled as "Recycle" in both conditions. The rate of (incorrect) recycling increased to 54.5% (67 out of 123 cups) when the "Landfill" label was used compared to 25.0% (34 out of 136 cups) for the "Trash" label (X2(l) = 23.58, p < .001). In study 2 (n = 122 observations; minimum of 33 observations per condition), during a separate beverage sampling event several weeks after the study 1, using the same procedures and unrecyclable cups as study 1, we tested additional types of labels including (i) control: "Trash" and "Recycle", (ii) avoid garbage: "Don't Destroy the Earth, Trash Less" and "Recycle", (iii) encourage recycling: "Trash" and "Save the Earth, Recycle More". Compared to the control condition, the recycling rate of the foam cups significantly increased in the avoid garbage condition (19.6% vs. 60
format Conference Proceeding
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_journals_3084482281</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>3084482281</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-proquest_journals_30844822813</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqNi8sKwjAQRbNQsD7-IeC6kD6s6bpUFFwUEVyWMYyS0k5qkgr-vUX9AFcXzjl3wgIhchnm8UbO2Ny5Rohom2ZZwKrKmrCkp7aGOiQPLb-A88hPqLD3oFrkR7hiq-nOCyB-IGUR3Cd4qS8246_TBF4bWrLpDVqHq98u2HpXnot92FvzGND5ujGDpVHViZBpKuNYRsl_1Rv_5j6I</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>conference_proceeding</recordtype><pqid>3084482281</pqid></control><display><type>conference_proceeding</type><title>Pro-Environmental Waste Receptacle Labeling Can Increase Recycling Contamination</title><source>EBSCOhost Business Source Complete</source><creator>Catlin, Jesse R ; Wang, Yitong ; Manuel, Rommel J</creator><creatorcontrib>Catlin, Jesse R ; Wang, Yitong ; Manuel, Rommel J</creatorcontrib><description><![CDATA[Early research has suggested that point-of-disposal signage and recycling receptacle modifications can increase recycling rates (Austin et at, 1993). However, the once steady increase in overall recycling rates over the past few decades appears to be stalling, with the most recent data showing only a 0.6% increase from four years prior (US EPA, 2014). At the same time, there has been a substantial rise in the rate of contamination from placing unrecyclable materials in recycling receptacles, which has created significant additional costs in the follow-up processing (Groden, 2015). In four field and online experiments, we show that point-of-disposal signage aiming to encourage recycling (such as the term "landfill" on garbage bins) could also increase recycling contamination. Prior research suggests people are generally averse to being wasteful (Bolton & Alba, 2012) and that failure to recycle could engender feelings of guilt (Viscusi et al., 2011). Therefore, we reason that labeling garbage bins with pro-environmental terms could make emotions evoked by the negative consequences of garbage more salient and thus increase the likelihood that individuals will incorrectly categorize unrecyclable items as being recyclable. In study 1 (и = 259 observations), as part of a new product sampling event by a well-known brand of coffee and related products, we offered passersby on a university campus the opportunity to sample a carbonated beverage (served in an unrecyclable foam cup). The study site featured two swing-top bins to dispose of their used sample cups. The label on one bin was experimentally manipulated to be either "Landfill" or "Trash". The other bin was labeled as "Recycle" in both conditions. The rate of (incorrect) recycling increased to 54.5% (67 out of 123 cups) when the "Landfill" label was used compared to 25.0% (34 out of 136 cups) for the "Trash" label (X2(l) = 23.58, p < .001). In study 2 (n = 122 observations; minimum of 33 observations per condition), during a separate beverage sampling event several weeks after the study 1, using the same procedures and unrecyclable cups as study 1, we tested additional types of labels including (i) control: "Trash" and "Recycle", (ii) avoid garbage: "Don't Destroy the Earth, Trash Less" and "Recycle", (iii) encourage recycling: "Trash" and "Save the Earth, Recycle More". Compared to the control condition, the recycling rate of the foam cups significantly increased in the avoid garbage condition (19.6% vs. 60.5% respectively; X2(1) = 15.58,p < .001) and marginally significantly increased in the encourage recycling condition (19.6% vs. 39.4% respectively; 2^(1) = 3.76, p = .052). Study 3 (n= 225; minimum of 51 participants per condition) provided some preliminary evidence of the underlying mechanism. Participants were randomly shown one pair of receptacles used in Studies 1 or 2 and an image of an empty potato chip bag. They were asked to imagine that they had disposed of the chip bag in the trash/landfill bin and to indicate how much they felt ashamed/guilty/embarrassed/bad/happy/good/proud/pleased (scale endpoints: 1Strongly disagree; 7 - Strongly agree; Donnelly et al. (2017) and Sun & Trudel (2017)). Participants then were asked how likely they were to retrieve the potato chip bag and instead dispose of it in the Recycling Bin (scale endpoints: 1 - Very unlikely; 7 - Very likely; Trudel et al. (2016)). We found that the mean intention to recycle of the "Saving the Earth" condition was not significantly different from the baseline condition (F(l, 102) = 1.92, p = .17) but the mean intention to recycle in the "Don't Destroy the Earth" condition (F(l, 105) = 6.59, p=.01, ηp2 = .06) and the "Landfill" condition (F(l, 109) = 7.09, p=.01, ηp2 = .06) were significantly higher than the baseline condition. Similarly, the negative emotion index in the "Saving the Earth" condition was not significantly different from the baseline condition (F(l, 102) = 0.048, p = .83) but the mean intention to recycle of the "Don't Destroy the Earth" condition (F(l, 105) = 4.09, p=.046, ηp2 = .04) and the "Landfill" condition (F(l, 109) = 15.11, p<.001, ηp2 = .12) were significantly higher than the baseline condition. Lastly, the positive emotion in the "Saving the Earth" condition (F(l, 102) = .09, p = .76) and the "Don't Destroy the Earth" condition (F(l, 105) = .73, p = .40) were not significantly different from the Baseline condition, but the mean positive emotion index of the "Landfill" condition (F(l, 105) = 15.49, p < .001, ηp2 = AT) was significantly higher than the baseline condition. We tested the mediation role of positive and negative emotions in each pairwise comparison. The results showed that across all mediation analyses, negative emotions were consistently mediating the effect while the results for positive emotions were less consistent. Study 4 (n = 60 observations; 30 per condition) explored a way to mitigate the previously observed contamination by incorrect recycling of unrecyclable items. Specifically, we conducted a before-and-after field experiment involving six different garbage/recycling bin pairs on a university campus to test the mitigating effect of a pictorial guide illustrating proper item disposal. We measured the contents of these bins in the first week as the baseline condition. In the next week, both garbage bins and recycling bins were labeled with pictorial guides featuring appropriate items to be placed in each bin. The results show that the proportion of unrecyclable items (excluding wrappers) placed in the recycle bin was marginally significantly reduced when the bins featured pictorial guides (26.7% vs. 32.5%; t(29) = -1.48, p = .075 (one-tailed)). The proportion of unrecyclable wrappers placed in the recycle bin was also marginally significantly reduced when the bins featured pictorial guides (21.4% vs. 27.7%; t(29) = -1.54, p = .067 (one-tailed)). And there was no statistically significant change in the proportion of recyclable items placed in the recycle bin (80.2% vs. 79.7%; t(29) = .09, p = .464 (one-tailed)). In sum, our studies suggest that if proper steps to educate people about accurate disposal are not taken common efforts to increase recycling rates through receptacle labeling (e.g., using the term "landfill" on garbage bins) could lead to higher recycling contamination because of the emotions evoked by these labels.]]></description><identifier>ISSN: 0098-9258</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Urbana: Association for Consumer Research</publisher><subject>Contamination ; Recycling ; Signs</subject><ispartof>Advances in consumer research, 2018, Vol.46, p.501-502</ispartof><rights>Copyright Association for Consumer Research 2018</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>309,310,780,784,789,790,23928,23929,25138</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Catlin, Jesse R</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Wang, Yitong</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Manuel, Rommel J</creatorcontrib><title>Pro-Environmental Waste Receptacle Labeling Can Increase Recycling Contamination</title><title>Advances in consumer research</title><description><![CDATA[Early research has suggested that point-of-disposal signage and recycling receptacle modifications can increase recycling rates (Austin et at, 1993). However, the once steady increase in overall recycling rates over the past few decades appears to be stalling, with the most recent data showing only a 0.6% increase from four years prior (US EPA, 2014). At the same time, there has been a substantial rise in the rate of contamination from placing unrecyclable materials in recycling receptacles, which has created significant additional costs in the follow-up processing (Groden, 2015). In four field and online experiments, we show that point-of-disposal signage aiming to encourage recycling (such as the term "landfill" on garbage bins) could also increase recycling contamination. Prior research suggests people are generally averse to being wasteful (Bolton & Alba, 2012) and that failure to recycle could engender feelings of guilt (Viscusi et al., 2011). Therefore, we reason that labeling garbage bins with pro-environmental terms could make emotions evoked by the negative consequences of garbage more salient and thus increase the likelihood that individuals will incorrectly categorize unrecyclable items as being recyclable. In study 1 (и = 259 observations), as part of a new product sampling event by a well-known brand of coffee and related products, we offered passersby on a university campus the opportunity to sample a carbonated beverage (served in an unrecyclable foam cup). The study site featured two swing-top bins to dispose of their used sample cups. The label on one bin was experimentally manipulated to be either "Landfill" or "Trash". The other bin was labeled as "Recycle" in both conditions. The rate of (incorrect) recycling increased to 54.5% (67 out of 123 cups) when the "Landfill" label was used compared to 25.0% (34 out of 136 cups) for the "Trash" label (X2(l) = 23.58, p < .001). In study 2 (n = 122 observations; minimum of 33 observations per condition), during a separate beverage sampling event several weeks after the study 1, using the same procedures and unrecyclable cups as study 1, we tested additional types of labels including (i) control: "Trash" and "Recycle", (ii) avoid garbage: "Don't Destroy the Earth, Trash Less" and "Recycle", (iii) encourage recycling: "Trash" and "Save the Earth, Recycle More". Compared to the control condition, the recycling rate of the foam cups significantly increased in the avoid garbage condition (19.6% vs. 60.5% respectively; X2(1) = 15.58,p < .001) and marginally significantly increased in the encourage recycling condition (19.6% vs. 39.4% respectively; 2^(1) = 3.76, p = .052). Study 3 (n= 225; minimum of 51 participants per condition) provided some preliminary evidence of the underlying mechanism. Participants were randomly shown one pair of receptacles used in Studies 1 or 2 and an image of an empty potato chip bag. They were asked to imagine that they had disposed of the chip bag in the trash/landfill bin and to indicate how much they felt ashamed/guilty/embarrassed/bad/happy/good/proud/pleased (scale endpoints: 1Strongly disagree; 7 - Strongly agree; Donnelly et al. (2017) and Sun & Trudel (2017)). Participants then were asked how likely they were to retrieve the potato chip bag and instead dispose of it in the Recycling Bin (scale endpoints: 1 - Very unlikely; 7 - Very likely; Trudel et al. (2016)). We found that the mean intention to recycle of the "Saving the Earth" condition was not significantly different from the baseline condition (F(l, 102) = 1.92, p = .17) but the mean intention to recycle in the "Don't Destroy the Earth" condition (F(l, 105) = 6.59, p=.01, ηp2 = .06) and the "Landfill" condition (F(l, 109) = 7.09, p=.01, ηp2 = .06) were significantly higher than the baseline condition. Similarly, the negative emotion index in the "Saving the Earth" condition was not significantly different from the baseline condition (F(l, 102) = 0.048, p = .83) but the mean intention to recycle of the "Don't Destroy the Earth" condition (F(l, 105) = 4.09, p=.046, ηp2 = .04) and the "Landfill" condition (F(l, 109) = 15.11, p<.001, ηp2 = .12) were significantly higher than the baseline condition. Lastly, the positive emotion in the "Saving the Earth" condition (F(l, 102) = .09, p = .76) and the "Don't Destroy the Earth" condition (F(l, 105) = .73, p = .40) were not significantly different from the Baseline condition, but the mean positive emotion index of the "Landfill" condition (F(l, 105) = 15.49, p < .001, ηp2 = AT) was significantly higher than the baseline condition. We tested the mediation role of positive and negative emotions in each pairwise comparison. The results showed that across all mediation analyses, negative emotions were consistently mediating the effect while the results for positive emotions were less consistent. Study 4 (n = 60 observations; 30 per condition) explored a way to mitigate the previously observed contamination by incorrect recycling of unrecyclable items. Specifically, we conducted a before-and-after field experiment involving six different garbage/recycling bin pairs on a university campus to test the mitigating effect of a pictorial guide illustrating proper item disposal. We measured the contents of these bins in the first week as the baseline condition. In the next week, both garbage bins and recycling bins were labeled with pictorial guides featuring appropriate items to be placed in each bin. The results show that the proportion of unrecyclable items (excluding wrappers) placed in the recycle bin was marginally significantly reduced when the bins featured pictorial guides (26.7% vs. 32.5%; t(29) = -1.48, p = .075 (one-tailed)). The proportion of unrecyclable wrappers placed in the recycle bin was also marginally significantly reduced when the bins featured pictorial guides (21.4% vs. 27.7%; t(29) = -1.54, p = .067 (one-tailed)). And there was no statistically significant change in the proportion of recyclable items placed in the recycle bin (80.2% vs. 79.7%; t(29) = .09, p = .464 (one-tailed)). In sum, our studies suggest that if proper steps to educate people about accurate disposal are not taken common efforts to increase recycling rates through receptacle labeling (e.g., using the term "landfill" on garbage bins) could lead to higher recycling contamination because of the emotions evoked by these labels.]]></description><subject>Contamination</subject><subject>Recycling</subject><subject>Signs</subject><issn>0098-9258</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>conference_proceeding</rsrctype><creationdate>2018</creationdate><recordtype>conference_proceeding</recordtype><sourceid>ABUWG</sourceid><sourceid>AFKRA</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>CCPQU</sourceid><sourceid>DWQXO</sourceid><recordid>eNqNi8sKwjAQRbNQsD7-IeC6kD6s6bpUFFwUEVyWMYyS0k5qkgr-vUX9AFcXzjl3wgIhchnm8UbO2Ny5Rohom2ZZwKrKmrCkp7aGOiQPLb-A88hPqLD3oFrkR7hiq-nOCyB-IGUR3Cd4qS8246_TBF4bWrLpDVqHq98u2HpXnot92FvzGND5ujGDpVHViZBpKuNYRsl_1Rv_5j6I</recordid><startdate>20180101</startdate><enddate>20180101</enddate><creator>Catlin, Jesse R</creator><creator>Wang, Yitong</creator><creator>Manuel, Rommel J</creator><general>Association for Consumer Research</general><scope>0U~</scope><scope>1-H</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7WY</scope><scope>7WZ</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>87Z</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>8FL</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BEZIV</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FRNLG</scope><scope>F~G</scope><scope>K60</scope><scope>K6~</scope><scope>L.-</scope><scope>L.0</scope><scope>M0C</scope><scope>PQBIZ</scope><scope>PQBZA</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>Q9U</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20180101</creationdate><title>Pro-Environmental Waste Receptacle Labeling Can Increase Recycling Contamination</title><author>Catlin, Jesse R ; Wang, Yitong ; Manuel, Rommel J</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-proquest_journals_30844822813</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>conference_proceedings</rsrctype><prefilter>conference_proceedings</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2018</creationdate><topic>Contamination</topic><topic>Recycling</topic><topic>Signs</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Catlin, Jesse R</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Wang, Yitong</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Manuel, Rommel J</creatorcontrib><collection>Global News &amp; ABI/Inform Professional</collection><collection>Trade PRO</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>ABI/INFORM Collection</collection><collection>ABI/INFORM Global (PDF only)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>ABI/INFORM Global (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>ABI/INFORM Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Business Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Business Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ABI/INFORM Global (Corporate)</collection><collection>ProQuest Business Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Business Collection</collection><collection>ABI/INFORM Professional Advanced</collection><collection>ABI/INFORM Professional Standard</collection><collection>ABI/INFORM Global</collection><collection>ProQuest One Business</collection><collection>ProQuest One Business (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Catlin, Jesse R</au><au>Wang, Yitong</au><au>Manuel, Rommel J</au><format>book</format><genre>proceeding</genre><ristype>CONF</ristype><atitle>Pro-Environmental Waste Receptacle Labeling Can Increase Recycling Contamination</atitle><btitle>Advances in consumer research</btitle><date>2018-01-01</date><risdate>2018</risdate><volume>46</volume><spage>501</spage><epage>502</epage><pages>501-502</pages><issn>0098-9258</issn><abstract><![CDATA[Early research has suggested that point-of-disposal signage and recycling receptacle modifications can increase recycling rates (Austin et at, 1993). However, the once steady increase in overall recycling rates over the past few decades appears to be stalling, with the most recent data showing only a 0.6% increase from four years prior (US EPA, 2014). At the same time, there has been a substantial rise in the rate of contamination from placing unrecyclable materials in recycling receptacles, which has created significant additional costs in the follow-up processing (Groden, 2015). In four field and online experiments, we show that point-of-disposal signage aiming to encourage recycling (such as the term "landfill" on garbage bins) could also increase recycling contamination. Prior research suggests people are generally averse to being wasteful (Bolton & Alba, 2012) and that failure to recycle could engender feelings of guilt (Viscusi et al., 2011). Therefore, we reason that labeling garbage bins with pro-environmental terms could make emotions evoked by the negative consequences of garbage more salient and thus increase the likelihood that individuals will incorrectly categorize unrecyclable items as being recyclable. In study 1 (и = 259 observations), as part of a new product sampling event by a well-known brand of coffee and related products, we offered passersby on a university campus the opportunity to sample a carbonated beverage (served in an unrecyclable foam cup). The study site featured two swing-top bins to dispose of their used sample cups. The label on one bin was experimentally manipulated to be either "Landfill" or "Trash". The other bin was labeled as "Recycle" in both conditions. The rate of (incorrect) recycling increased to 54.5% (67 out of 123 cups) when the "Landfill" label was used compared to 25.0% (34 out of 136 cups) for the "Trash" label (X2(l) = 23.58, p < .001). In study 2 (n = 122 observations; minimum of 33 observations per condition), during a separate beverage sampling event several weeks after the study 1, using the same procedures and unrecyclable cups as study 1, we tested additional types of labels including (i) control: "Trash" and "Recycle", (ii) avoid garbage: "Don't Destroy the Earth, Trash Less" and "Recycle", (iii) encourage recycling: "Trash" and "Save the Earth, Recycle More". Compared to the control condition, the recycling rate of the foam cups significantly increased in the avoid garbage condition (19.6% vs. 60.5% respectively; X2(1) = 15.58,p < .001) and marginally significantly increased in the encourage recycling condition (19.6% vs. 39.4% respectively; 2^(1) = 3.76, p = .052). Study 3 (n= 225; minimum of 51 participants per condition) provided some preliminary evidence of the underlying mechanism. Participants were randomly shown one pair of receptacles used in Studies 1 or 2 and an image of an empty potato chip bag. They were asked to imagine that they had disposed of the chip bag in the trash/landfill bin and to indicate how much they felt ashamed/guilty/embarrassed/bad/happy/good/proud/pleased (scale endpoints: 1Strongly disagree; 7 - Strongly agree; Donnelly et al. (2017) and Sun & Trudel (2017)). Participants then were asked how likely they were to retrieve the potato chip bag and instead dispose of it in the Recycling Bin (scale endpoints: 1 - Very unlikely; 7 - Very likely; Trudel et al. (2016)). We found that the mean intention to recycle of the "Saving the Earth" condition was not significantly different from the baseline condition (F(l, 102) = 1.92, p = .17) but the mean intention to recycle in the "Don't Destroy the Earth" condition (F(l, 105) = 6.59, p=.01, ηp2 = .06) and the "Landfill" condition (F(l, 109) = 7.09, p=.01, ηp2 = .06) were significantly higher than the baseline condition. Similarly, the negative emotion index in the "Saving the Earth" condition was not significantly different from the baseline condition (F(l, 102) = 0.048, p = .83) but the mean intention to recycle of the "Don't Destroy the Earth" condition (F(l, 105) = 4.09, p=.046, ηp2 = .04) and the "Landfill" condition (F(l, 109) = 15.11, p<.001, ηp2 = .12) were significantly higher than the baseline condition. Lastly, the positive emotion in the "Saving the Earth" condition (F(l, 102) = .09, p = .76) and the "Don't Destroy the Earth" condition (F(l, 105) = .73, p = .40) were not significantly different from the Baseline condition, but the mean positive emotion index of the "Landfill" condition (F(l, 105) = 15.49, p < .001, ηp2 = AT) was significantly higher than the baseline condition. We tested the mediation role of positive and negative emotions in each pairwise comparison. The results showed that across all mediation analyses, negative emotions were consistently mediating the effect while the results for positive emotions were less consistent. Study 4 (n = 60 observations; 30 per condition) explored a way to mitigate the previously observed contamination by incorrect recycling of unrecyclable items. Specifically, we conducted a before-and-after field experiment involving six different garbage/recycling bin pairs on a university campus to test the mitigating effect of a pictorial guide illustrating proper item disposal. We measured the contents of these bins in the first week as the baseline condition. In the next week, both garbage bins and recycling bins were labeled with pictorial guides featuring appropriate items to be placed in each bin. The results show that the proportion of unrecyclable items (excluding wrappers) placed in the recycle bin was marginally significantly reduced when the bins featured pictorial guides (26.7% vs. 32.5%; t(29) = -1.48, p = .075 (one-tailed)). The proportion of unrecyclable wrappers placed in the recycle bin was also marginally significantly reduced when the bins featured pictorial guides (21.4% vs. 27.7%; t(29) = -1.54, p = .067 (one-tailed)). And there was no statistically significant change in the proportion of recyclable items placed in the recycle bin (80.2% vs. 79.7%; t(29) = .09, p = .464 (one-tailed)). In sum, our studies suggest that if proper steps to educate people about accurate disposal are not taken common efforts to increase recycling rates through receptacle labeling (e.g., using the term "landfill" on garbage bins) could lead to higher recycling contamination because of the emotions evoked by these labels.]]></abstract><cop>Urbana</cop><pub>Association for Consumer Research</pub></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0098-9258
ispartof Advances in consumer research, 2018, Vol.46, p.501-502
issn 0098-9258
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_journals_3084482281
source EBSCOhost Business Source Complete
subjects Contamination
Recycling
Signs
title Pro-Environmental Waste Receptacle Labeling Can Increase Recycling Contamination
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-14T10%3A34%3A50IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:book&rft.genre=proceeding&rft.atitle=Pro-Environmental%20Waste%20Receptacle%20Labeling%20Can%20Increase%20Recycling%20Contamination&rft.btitle=Advances%20in%20consumer%20research&rft.au=Catlin,%20Jesse%20R&rft.date=2018-01-01&rft.volume=46&rft.spage=501&rft.epage=502&rft.pages=501-502&rft.issn=0098-9258&rft_id=info:doi/&rft_dat=%3Cproquest%3E3084482281%3C/proquest%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=3084482281&rft_id=info:pmid/&rfr_iscdi=true