Less than one percent of words would be affected by gender-inclusive language in German press texts

Research on gender and language is tightly knitted to social debates on gender equality and non-discriminatory language use. Psycholinguistic scholars have made significant contributions in this field. However, corpus-based studies that investigate these matters within the context of language use ar...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:arXiv.org 2024-02
Hauptverfasser: Müller-Spitzer, Carolin, Ochs, Samira, Koplenig, Alexander, Jan-Oliver, Rüdiger, Wolfer, Sascha
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page
container_issue
container_start_page
container_title arXiv.org
container_volume
creator Müller-Spitzer, Carolin
Ochs, Samira
Koplenig, Alexander
Jan-Oliver, Rüdiger
Wolfer, Sascha
description Research on gender and language is tightly knitted to social debates on gender equality and non-discriminatory language use. Psycholinguistic scholars have made significant contributions in this field. However, corpus-based studies that investigate these matters within the context of language use are still rare. In our study, we address the question of how much textual material would actually have to be changed if non-gender-inclusive texts were rewritten to be gender-inclusive. This quantitative measure is an important empirical insight, as a recurring argument against the use of gender-inclusive German is that it supposedly makes written texts too long and complicated. It is also argued that gender-inclusive language has negative effects on language learners. However, such effects are only likely if gender-inclusive texts are very different from those that are not gender-inclusive. In our corpus-linguistic study, we manually annotated German press texts to identify the parts that would have to be changed. Our results show that, on average, less than 1% of all tokens would be affected by gender-inclusive language. This small proportion calls into question whether gender-inclusive German presents a substantial barrier to understanding and learning the language, particularly when we take into account the potential complexities of interpreting masculine generics.
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_journals_2923182066</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>2923182066</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-proquest_journals_29231820663</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqNjMsKwjAURIMgWLT_cMF1oU1srWvxsXDpvsT2trbEpOYmPv7eIH6Am5mBM5wJi7gQWVKuOJ-xmGhI05QXa57nImL1CYnAXaUGoxFGtDVqB6aFp7ENhfSqgQuCbFusHYb9hg51gzbpda089Q8EJXXnZYfQazigvQXbaL9ifDlasGkrFWH86zlb7nfn7TEZrbl7JFcNxlsdUMU3XGQlT4tC_Pf6AK8vRdw</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2923182066</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Less than one percent of words would be affected by gender-inclusive language in German press texts</title><source>Free E- Journals</source><creator>Müller-Spitzer, Carolin ; Ochs, Samira ; Koplenig, Alexander ; Jan-Oliver, Rüdiger ; Wolfer, Sascha</creator><creatorcontrib>Müller-Spitzer, Carolin ; Ochs, Samira ; Koplenig, Alexander ; Jan-Oliver, Rüdiger ; Wolfer, Sascha</creatorcontrib><description>Research on gender and language is tightly knitted to social debates on gender equality and non-discriminatory language use. Psycholinguistic scholars have made significant contributions in this field. However, corpus-based studies that investigate these matters within the context of language use are still rare. In our study, we address the question of how much textual material would actually have to be changed if non-gender-inclusive texts were rewritten to be gender-inclusive. This quantitative measure is an important empirical insight, as a recurring argument against the use of gender-inclusive German is that it supposedly makes written texts too long and complicated. It is also argued that gender-inclusive language has negative effects on language learners. However, such effects are only likely if gender-inclusive texts are very different from those that are not gender-inclusive. In our corpus-linguistic study, we manually annotated German press texts to identify the parts that would have to be changed. Our results show that, on average, less than 1% of all tokens would be affected by gender-inclusive language. This small proportion calls into question whether gender-inclusive German presents a substantial barrier to understanding and learning the language, particularly when we take into account the potential complexities of interpreting masculine generics.</description><identifier>EISSN: 2331-8422</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Ithaca: Cornell University Library, arXiv.org</publisher><subject>Gender ; Language ; Questions ; Texts</subject><ispartof>arXiv.org, 2024-02</ispartof><rights>2024. This work is published under http://arxiv.org/licenses/nonexclusive-distrib/1.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.</rights><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>777,781</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Müller-Spitzer, Carolin</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Ochs, Samira</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Koplenig, Alexander</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Jan-Oliver, Rüdiger</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Wolfer, Sascha</creatorcontrib><title>Less than one percent of words would be affected by gender-inclusive language in German press texts</title><title>arXiv.org</title><description>Research on gender and language is tightly knitted to social debates on gender equality and non-discriminatory language use. Psycholinguistic scholars have made significant contributions in this field. However, corpus-based studies that investigate these matters within the context of language use are still rare. In our study, we address the question of how much textual material would actually have to be changed if non-gender-inclusive texts were rewritten to be gender-inclusive. This quantitative measure is an important empirical insight, as a recurring argument against the use of gender-inclusive German is that it supposedly makes written texts too long and complicated. It is also argued that gender-inclusive language has negative effects on language learners. However, such effects are only likely if gender-inclusive texts are very different from those that are not gender-inclusive. In our corpus-linguistic study, we manually annotated German press texts to identify the parts that would have to be changed. Our results show that, on average, less than 1% of all tokens would be affected by gender-inclusive language. This small proportion calls into question whether gender-inclusive German presents a substantial barrier to understanding and learning the language, particularly when we take into account the potential complexities of interpreting masculine generics.</description><subject>Gender</subject><subject>Language</subject><subject>Questions</subject><subject>Texts</subject><issn>2331-8422</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2024</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>ABUWG</sourceid><sourceid>AFKRA</sourceid><sourceid>AZQEC</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>CCPQU</sourceid><sourceid>DWQXO</sourceid><recordid>eNqNjMsKwjAURIMgWLT_cMF1oU1srWvxsXDpvsT2trbEpOYmPv7eIH6Am5mBM5wJi7gQWVKuOJ-xmGhI05QXa57nImL1CYnAXaUGoxFGtDVqB6aFp7ENhfSqgQuCbFusHYb9hg51gzbpda089Q8EJXXnZYfQazigvQXbaL9ifDlasGkrFWH86zlb7nfn7TEZrbl7JFcNxlsdUMU3XGQlT4tC_Pf6AK8vRdw</recordid><startdate>20240206</startdate><enddate>20240206</enddate><creator>Müller-Spitzer, Carolin</creator><creator>Ochs, Samira</creator><creator>Koplenig, Alexander</creator><creator>Jan-Oliver, Rüdiger</creator><creator>Wolfer, Sascha</creator><general>Cornell University Library, arXiv.org</general><scope>8FE</scope><scope>8FG</scope><scope>ABJCF</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BGLVJ</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>L6V</scope><scope>M7S</scope><scope>PIMPY</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PTHSS</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20240206</creationdate><title>Less than one percent of words would be affected by gender-inclusive language in German press texts</title><author>Müller-Spitzer, Carolin ; Ochs, Samira ; Koplenig, Alexander ; Jan-Oliver, Rüdiger ; Wolfer, Sascha</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-proquest_journals_29231820663</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2024</creationdate><topic>Gender</topic><topic>Language</topic><topic>Questions</topic><topic>Texts</topic><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Müller-Spitzer, Carolin</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Ochs, Samira</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Koplenig, Alexander</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Jan-Oliver, Rüdiger</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Wolfer, Sascha</creatorcontrib><collection>ProQuest SciTech Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Technology Collection</collection><collection>Materials Science &amp; Engineering Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Technology Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Engineering Collection</collection><collection>Engineering Database</collection><collection>Publicly Available Content Database</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>Engineering Collection</collection></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Müller-Spitzer, Carolin</au><au>Ochs, Samira</au><au>Koplenig, Alexander</au><au>Jan-Oliver, Rüdiger</au><au>Wolfer, Sascha</au><format>book</format><genre>document</genre><ristype>GEN</ristype><atitle>Less than one percent of words would be affected by gender-inclusive language in German press texts</atitle><jtitle>arXiv.org</jtitle><date>2024-02-06</date><risdate>2024</risdate><eissn>2331-8422</eissn><abstract>Research on gender and language is tightly knitted to social debates on gender equality and non-discriminatory language use. Psycholinguistic scholars have made significant contributions in this field. However, corpus-based studies that investigate these matters within the context of language use are still rare. In our study, we address the question of how much textual material would actually have to be changed if non-gender-inclusive texts were rewritten to be gender-inclusive. This quantitative measure is an important empirical insight, as a recurring argument against the use of gender-inclusive German is that it supposedly makes written texts too long and complicated. It is also argued that gender-inclusive language has negative effects on language learners. However, such effects are only likely if gender-inclusive texts are very different from those that are not gender-inclusive. In our corpus-linguistic study, we manually annotated German press texts to identify the parts that would have to be changed. Our results show that, on average, less than 1% of all tokens would be affected by gender-inclusive language. This small proportion calls into question whether gender-inclusive German presents a substantial barrier to understanding and learning the language, particularly when we take into account the potential complexities of interpreting masculine generics.</abstract><cop>Ithaca</cop><pub>Cornell University Library, arXiv.org</pub><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier EISSN: 2331-8422
ispartof arXiv.org, 2024-02
issn 2331-8422
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_journals_2923182066
source Free E- Journals
subjects Gender
Language
Questions
Texts
title Less than one percent of words would be affected by gender-inclusive language in German press texts
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-20T12%3A46%3A37IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:book&rft.genre=document&rft.atitle=Less%20than%20one%20percent%20of%20words%20would%20be%20affected%20by%20gender-inclusive%20language%20in%20German%20press%20texts&rft.jtitle=arXiv.org&rft.au=M%C3%BCller-Spitzer,%20Carolin&rft.date=2024-02-06&rft.eissn=2331-8422&rft_id=info:doi/&rft_dat=%3Cproquest%3E2923182066%3C/proquest%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2923182066&rft_id=info:pmid/&rfr_iscdi=true