How time gets spatial: factors determining the stability and instability of the mental time line
Left-to-right readers classify faster past events with motor responses on the left side of space and future events with responses on the right side. This suggests a left-to-right spatial organization in the mental representation of time. Here, we show that the significance and reliability of this re...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Attention, perception & psychophysics perception & psychophysics, 2023-10, Vol.85 (7), p.2321-2336 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , , , , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 2336 |
---|---|
container_issue | 7 |
container_start_page | 2321 |
container_title | Attention, perception & psychophysics |
container_volume | 85 |
creator | Scozia, Gabriele Pinto, Mario Pellegrino, Michele Lozito, Silvana Pia, Lorenzo Lasaponara, Stefano Doricchi, Fabrizio |
description | Left-to-right readers classify faster past events with motor responses on the left side of space and future events with responses on the right side. This suggests a left-to-right spatial organization in the mental representation of time. Here, we show that the significance and reliability of this representation are linked to the joint use of temporal and spatial codes in the task at hand. In a first unimanual Go/No-Go Implicit Association Test (IAT), attending selectively to "past" or to "future" words did not activate corresponding "left" or "right" spatial concepts and vice versa. In a second IAT, attending to both temporal (i.e., "past" and "future") words and spatial targets (i.e., "left" and "right") pointing arrows produced faster responses for congruent rather than incongruent combinations of temporal and spatial concepts in task instructions (e.g., congruent = "Go with past words and left-pointing arrows"; incongruent = "Go with past words and right-pointing arrows"). This effect increased markedly in a STEARC task where spatial codes defined the selection between "left-side" and "right-side" button presses that were associated with "past" and "future" words. Two control experiments showed only partial or unreliable space-time congruency effects when (a) participants attended to superordinate semantic codes that included both spatial "left"/"right" or temporal "past/future" subordinate codes; (b) a primary speeded response was assigned to one dimension (e.g., "past vs. future") and a nonspeeded one to the other dimension (e.g., "left" vs. "right"). These results help to define the conditions that trigger a stable and reliable spatial representation of time-related concepts. |
doi_str_mv | 10.3758/sl3414-023-02746-w |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_journals_2883016117</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>2883016117</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-proquest_journals_28830161173</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqNjMFuwjAQRK0KpNLSH-hpJc4BO5smgWtFxQdw4BZc2IAjx06zi1D_HtRW9MphNDOap1Hq1egpFm_ljD1mJkt0ilcVWZ6cH9TIzDNMcI6bwS2n5lE9MTda55gXeqS2q3gGcS3BgYSBOyvO-gXUdiexZ9iTUN-64MIB5EjAYj-dd_INNuzBhf8e6x-gpSDW_156F2ishrX1TC9__qwmH8v1-yrp-vh1Ipaqiac-XKcqLUvUJjemwPuoC-OCTHA</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2883016117</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>How time gets spatial: factors determining the stability and instability of the mental time line</title><source>Springer Nature - Complete Springer Journals</source><source>Alma/SFX Local Collection</source><creator>Scozia, Gabriele ; Pinto, Mario ; Pellegrino, Michele ; Lozito, Silvana ; Pia, Lorenzo ; Lasaponara, Stefano ; Doricchi, Fabrizio</creator><creatorcontrib>Scozia, Gabriele ; Pinto, Mario ; Pellegrino, Michele ; Lozito, Silvana ; Pia, Lorenzo ; Lasaponara, Stefano ; Doricchi, Fabrizio</creatorcontrib><description>Left-to-right readers classify faster past events with motor responses on the left side of space and future events with responses on the right side. This suggests a left-to-right spatial organization in the mental representation of time. Here, we show that the significance and reliability of this representation are linked to the joint use of temporal and spatial codes in the task at hand. In a first unimanual Go/No-Go Implicit Association Test (IAT), attending selectively to "past" or to "future" words did not activate corresponding "left" or "right" spatial concepts and vice versa. In a second IAT, attending to both temporal (i.e., "past" and "future") words and spatial targets (i.e., "left" and "right") pointing arrows produced faster responses for congruent rather than incongruent combinations of temporal and spatial concepts in task instructions (e.g., congruent = "Go with past words and left-pointing arrows"; incongruent = "Go with past words and right-pointing arrows"). This effect increased markedly in a STEARC task where spatial codes defined the selection between "left-side" and "right-side" button presses that were associated with "past" and "future" words. Two control experiments showed only partial or unreliable space-time congruency effects when (a) participants attended to superordinate semantic codes that included both spatial "left"/"right" or temporal "past/future" subordinate codes; (b) a primary speeded response was assigned to one dimension (e.g., "past vs. future") and a nonspeeded one to the other dimension (e.g., "left" vs. "right"). These results help to define the conditions that trigger a stable and reliable spatial representation of time-related concepts.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1943-3921</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1943-393X</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.3758/sl3414-023-02746-w</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Austin: Springer Nature B.V</publisher><subject>Association Measures ; Codes ; Feedback (Response) ; Meta Analysis ; Numbers ; Pictorial Stimuli ; Reading ; Reading Habits ; Semantics ; Semiotics ; Semitic Languages ; Stimuli</subject><ispartof>Attention, perception & psychophysics, 2023-10, Vol.85 (7), p.2321-2336</ispartof><rights>Copyright Springer Nature B.V. Oct 2023</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,27901,27902</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Scozia, Gabriele</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Pinto, Mario</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Pellegrino, Michele</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Lozito, Silvana</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Pia, Lorenzo</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Lasaponara, Stefano</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Doricchi, Fabrizio</creatorcontrib><title>How time gets spatial: factors determining the stability and instability of the mental time line</title><title>Attention, perception & psychophysics</title><description>Left-to-right readers classify faster past events with motor responses on the left side of space and future events with responses on the right side. This suggests a left-to-right spatial organization in the mental representation of time. Here, we show that the significance and reliability of this representation are linked to the joint use of temporal and spatial codes in the task at hand. In a first unimanual Go/No-Go Implicit Association Test (IAT), attending selectively to "past" or to "future" words did not activate corresponding "left" or "right" spatial concepts and vice versa. In a second IAT, attending to both temporal (i.e., "past" and "future") words and spatial targets (i.e., "left" and "right") pointing arrows produced faster responses for congruent rather than incongruent combinations of temporal and spatial concepts in task instructions (e.g., congruent = "Go with past words and left-pointing arrows"; incongruent = "Go with past words and right-pointing arrows"). This effect increased markedly in a STEARC task where spatial codes defined the selection between "left-side" and "right-side" button presses that were associated with "past" and "future" words. Two control experiments showed only partial or unreliable space-time congruency effects when (a) participants attended to superordinate semantic codes that included both spatial "left"/"right" or temporal "past/future" subordinate codes; (b) a primary speeded response was assigned to one dimension (e.g., "past vs. future") and a nonspeeded one to the other dimension (e.g., "left" vs. "right"). These results help to define the conditions that trigger a stable and reliable spatial representation of time-related concepts.</description><subject>Association Measures</subject><subject>Codes</subject><subject>Feedback (Response)</subject><subject>Meta Analysis</subject><subject>Numbers</subject><subject>Pictorial Stimuli</subject><subject>Reading</subject><subject>Reading Habits</subject><subject>Semantics</subject><subject>Semiotics</subject><subject>Semitic Languages</subject><subject>Stimuli</subject><issn>1943-3921</issn><issn>1943-393X</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2023</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>8G5</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>GUQSH</sourceid><sourceid>M2O</sourceid><recordid>eNqNjMFuwjAQRK0KpNLSH-hpJc4BO5smgWtFxQdw4BZc2IAjx06zi1D_HtRW9MphNDOap1Hq1egpFm_ljD1mJkt0ilcVWZ6cH9TIzDNMcI6bwS2n5lE9MTda55gXeqS2q3gGcS3BgYSBOyvO-gXUdiexZ9iTUN-64MIB5EjAYj-dd_INNuzBhf8e6x-gpSDW_156F2ishrX1TC9__qwmH8v1-yrp-vh1Ipaqiac-XKcqLUvUJjemwPuoC-OCTHA</recordid><startdate>20231001</startdate><enddate>20231001</enddate><creator>Scozia, Gabriele</creator><creator>Pinto, Mario</creator><creator>Pellegrino, Michele</creator><creator>Lozito, Silvana</creator><creator>Pia, Lorenzo</creator><creator>Lasaponara, Stefano</creator><creator>Doricchi, Fabrizio</creator><general>Springer Nature B.V</general><scope>0-V</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>4T-</scope><scope>4U-</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88B</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>88J</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>8G5</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ALSLI</scope><scope>AN0</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>CJNVE</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>GUQSH</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>M0P</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>M2O</scope><scope>M2R</scope><scope>MBDVC</scope><scope>PHGZM</scope><scope>PHGZT</scope><scope>PJZUB</scope><scope>PKEHL</scope><scope>POGQB</scope><scope>PPXIY</scope><scope>PQEDU</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PRQQA</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>Q9U</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20231001</creationdate><title>How time gets spatial: factors determining the stability and instability of the mental time line</title><author>Scozia, Gabriele ; Pinto, Mario ; Pellegrino, Michele ; Lozito, Silvana ; Pia, Lorenzo ; Lasaponara, Stefano ; Doricchi, Fabrizio</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-proquest_journals_28830161173</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2023</creationdate><topic>Association Measures</topic><topic>Codes</topic><topic>Feedback (Response)</topic><topic>Meta Analysis</topic><topic>Numbers</topic><topic>Pictorial Stimuli</topic><topic>Reading</topic><topic>Reading Habits</topic><topic>Semantics</topic><topic>Semiotics</topic><topic>Semitic Languages</topic><topic>Stimuli</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Scozia, Gabriele</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Pinto, Mario</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Pellegrino, Michele</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Lozito, Silvana</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Pia, Lorenzo</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Lasaponara, Stefano</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Doricchi, Fabrizio</creatorcontrib><collection>ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Docstoc</collection><collection>University Readers</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Education Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>Social Science Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Research Library (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>Social Science Premium Collection</collection><collection>British Nursing Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>Education Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>Research Library Prep</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Education Database</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Psychology</collection><collection>Research Library</collection><collection>Social Science Database</collection><collection>Research Library (Corporate)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (New)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic (New)</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Research Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New)</collection><collection>ProQuest Sociology & Social Sciences Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest One Health & Nursing</collection><collection>ProQuest One Education</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Social Sciences</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><jtitle>Attention, perception & psychophysics</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Scozia, Gabriele</au><au>Pinto, Mario</au><au>Pellegrino, Michele</au><au>Lozito, Silvana</au><au>Pia, Lorenzo</au><au>Lasaponara, Stefano</au><au>Doricchi, Fabrizio</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>How time gets spatial: factors determining the stability and instability of the mental time line</atitle><jtitle>Attention, perception & psychophysics</jtitle><date>2023-10-01</date><risdate>2023</risdate><volume>85</volume><issue>7</issue><spage>2321</spage><epage>2336</epage><pages>2321-2336</pages><issn>1943-3921</issn><eissn>1943-393X</eissn><abstract>Left-to-right readers classify faster past events with motor responses on the left side of space and future events with responses on the right side. This suggests a left-to-right spatial organization in the mental representation of time. Here, we show that the significance and reliability of this representation are linked to the joint use of temporal and spatial codes in the task at hand. In a first unimanual Go/No-Go Implicit Association Test (IAT), attending selectively to "past" or to "future" words did not activate corresponding "left" or "right" spatial concepts and vice versa. In a second IAT, attending to both temporal (i.e., "past" and "future") words and spatial targets (i.e., "left" and "right") pointing arrows produced faster responses for congruent rather than incongruent combinations of temporal and spatial concepts in task instructions (e.g., congruent = "Go with past words and left-pointing arrows"; incongruent = "Go with past words and right-pointing arrows"). This effect increased markedly in a STEARC task where spatial codes defined the selection between "left-side" and "right-side" button presses that were associated with "past" and "future" words. Two control experiments showed only partial or unreliable space-time congruency effects when (a) participants attended to superordinate semantic codes that included both spatial "left"/"right" or temporal "past/future" subordinate codes; (b) a primary speeded response was assigned to one dimension (e.g., "past vs. future") and a nonspeeded one to the other dimension (e.g., "left" vs. "right"). These results help to define the conditions that trigger a stable and reliable spatial representation of time-related concepts.</abstract><cop>Austin</cop><pub>Springer Nature B.V</pub><doi>10.3758/sl3414-023-02746-w</doi></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 1943-3921 |
ispartof | Attention, perception & psychophysics, 2023-10, Vol.85 (7), p.2321-2336 |
issn | 1943-3921 1943-393X |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_journals_2883016117 |
source | Springer Nature - Complete Springer Journals; Alma/SFX Local Collection |
subjects | Association Measures Codes Feedback (Response) Meta Analysis Numbers Pictorial Stimuli Reading Reading Habits Semantics Semiotics Semitic Languages Stimuli |
title | How time gets spatial: factors determining the stability and instability of the mental time line |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-19T07%3A44%3A38IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=How%20time%20gets%20spatial:%20factors%20determining%20the%20stability%20and%20instability%20of%20the%20mental%20time%20line&rft.jtitle=Attention,%20perception%20&%20psychophysics&rft.au=Scozia,%20Gabriele&rft.date=2023-10-01&rft.volume=85&rft.issue=7&rft.spage=2321&rft.epage=2336&rft.pages=2321-2336&rft.issn=1943-3921&rft.eissn=1943-393X&rft_id=info:doi/10.3758/sl3414-023-02746-w&rft_dat=%3Cproquest%3E2883016117%3C/proquest%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2883016117&rft_id=info:pmid/&rfr_iscdi=true |