How time gets spatial: factors determining the stability and instability of the mental time line

Left-to-right readers classify faster past events with motor responses on the left side of space and future events with responses on the right side. This suggests a left-to-right spatial organization in the mental representation of time. Here, we show that the significance and reliability of this re...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Attention, perception & psychophysics perception & psychophysics, 2023-10, Vol.85 (7), p.2321-2336
Hauptverfasser: Scozia, Gabriele, Pinto, Mario, Pellegrino, Michele, Lozito, Silvana, Pia, Lorenzo, Lasaponara, Stefano, Doricchi, Fabrizio
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 2336
container_issue 7
container_start_page 2321
container_title Attention, perception & psychophysics
container_volume 85
creator Scozia, Gabriele
Pinto, Mario
Pellegrino, Michele
Lozito, Silvana
Pia, Lorenzo
Lasaponara, Stefano
Doricchi, Fabrizio
description Left-to-right readers classify faster past events with motor responses on the left side of space and future events with responses on the right side. This suggests a left-to-right spatial organization in the mental representation of time. Here, we show that the significance and reliability of this representation are linked to the joint use of temporal and spatial codes in the task at hand. In a first unimanual Go/No-Go Implicit Association Test (IAT), attending selectively to "past" or to "future" words did not activate corresponding "left" or "right" spatial concepts and vice versa. In a second IAT, attending to both temporal (i.e., "past" and "future") words and spatial targets (i.e., "left" and "right") pointing arrows produced faster responses for congruent rather than incongruent combinations of temporal and spatial concepts in task instructions (e.g., congruent = "Go with past words and left-pointing arrows"; incongruent = "Go with past words and right-pointing arrows"). This effect increased markedly in a STEARC task where spatial codes defined the selection between "left-side" and "right-side" button presses that were associated with "past" and "future" words. Two control experiments showed only partial or unreliable space-time congruency effects when (a) participants attended to superordinate semantic codes that included both spatial "left"/"right" or temporal "past/future" subordinate codes; (b) a primary speeded response was assigned to one dimension (e.g., "past vs. future") and a nonspeeded one to the other dimension (e.g., "left" vs. "right"). These results help to define the conditions that trigger a stable and reliable spatial representation of time-related concepts.
doi_str_mv 10.3758/sl3414-023-02746-w
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_journals_2883016117</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>2883016117</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-proquest_journals_28830161173</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqNjMFuwjAQRK0KpNLSH-hpJc4BO5smgWtFxQdw4BZc2IAjx06zi1D_HtRW9MphNDOap1Hq1egpFm_ljD1mJkt0ilcVWZ6cH9TIzDNMcI6bwS2n5lE9MTda55gXeqS2q3gGcS3BgYSBOyvO-gXUdiexZ9iTUN-64MIB5EjAYj-dd_INNuzBhf8e6x-gpSDW_156F2ishrX1TC9__qwmH8v1-yrp-vh1Ipaqiac-XKcqLUvUJjemwPuoC-OCTHA</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2883016117</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>How time gets spatial: factors determining the stability and instability of the mental time line</title><source>Springer Nature - Complete Springer Journals</source><source>Alma/SFX Local Collection</source><creator>Scozia, Gabriele ; Pinto, Mario ; Pellegrino, Michele ; Lozito, Silvana ; Pia, Lorenzo ; Lasaponara, Stefano ; Doricchi, Fabrizio</creator><creatorcontrib>Scozia, Gabriele ; Pinto, Mario ; Pellegrino, Michele ; Lozito, Silvana ; Pia, Lorenzo ; Lasaponara, Stefano ; Doricchi, Fabrizio</creatorcontrib><description>Left-to-right readers classify faster past events with motor responses on the left side of space and future events with responses on the right side. This suggests a left-to-right spatial organization in the mental representation of time. Here, we show that the significance and reliability of this representation are linked to the joint use of temporal and spatial codes in the task at hand. In a first unimanual Go/No-Go Implicit Association Test (IAT), attending selectively to "past" or to "future" words did not activate corresponding "left" or "right" spatial concepts and vice versa. In a second IAT, attending to both temporal (i.e., "past" and "future") words and spatial targets (i.e., "left" and "right") pointing arrows produced faster responses for congruent rather than incongruent combinations of temporal and spatial concepts in task instructions (e.g., congruent = "Go with past words and left-pointing arrows"; incongruent = "Go with past words and right-pointing arrows"). This effect increased markedly in a STEARC task where spatial codes defined the selection between "left-side" and "right-side" button presses that were associated with "past" and "future" words. Two control experiments showed only partial or unreliable space-time congruency effects when (a) participants attended to superordinate semantic codes that included both spatial "left"/"right" or temporal "past/future" subordinate codes; (b) a primary speeded response was assigned to one dimension (e.g., "past vs. future") and a nonspeeded one to the other dimension (e.g., "left" vs. "right"). These results help to define the conditions that trigger a stable and reliable spatial representation of time-related concepts.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1943-3921</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1943-393X</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.3758/sl3414-023-02746-w</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Austin: Springer Nature B.V</publisher><subject>Association Measures ; Codes ; Feedback (Response) ; Meta Analysis ; Numbers ; Pictorial Stimuli ; Reading ; Reading Habits ; Semantics ; Semiotics ; Semitic Languages ; Stimuli</subject><ispartof>Attention, perception &amp; psychophysics, 2023-10, Vol.85 (7), p.2321-2336</ispartof><rights>Copyright Springer Nature B.V. Oct 2023</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,27901,27902</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Scozia, Gabriele</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Pinto, Mario</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Pellegrino, Michele</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Lozito, Silvana</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Pia, Lorenzo</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Lasaponara, Stefano</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Doricchi, Fabrizio</creatorcontrib><title>How time gets spatial: factors determining the stability and instability of the mental time line</title><title>Attention, perception &amp; psychophysics</title><description>Left-to-right readers classify faster past events with motor responses on the left side of space and future events with responses on the right side. This suggests a left-to-right spatial organization in the mental representation of time. Here, we show that the significance and reliability of this representation are linked to the joint use of temporal and spatial codes in the task at hand. In a first unimanual Go/No-Go Implicit Association Test (IAT), attending selectively to "past" or to "future" words did not activate corresponding "left" or "right" spatial concepts and vice versa. In a second IAT, attending to both temporal (i.e., "past" and "future") words and spatial targets (i.e., "left" and "right") pointing arrows produced faster responses for congruent rather than incongruent combinations of temporal and spatial concepts in task instructions (e.g., congruent = "Go with past words and left-pointing arrows"; incongruent = "Go with past words and right-pointing arrows"). This effect increased markedly in a STEARC task where spatial codes defined the selection between "left-side" and "right-side" button presses that were associated with "past" and "future" words. Two control experiments showed only partial or unreliable space-time congruency effects when (a) participants attended to superordinate semantic codes that included both spatial "left"/"right" or temporal "past/future" subordinate codes; (b) a primary speeded response was assigned to one dimension (e.g., "past vs. future") and a nonspeeded one to the other dimension (e.g., "left" vs. "right"). These results help to define the conditions that trigger a stable and reliable spatial representation of time-related concepts.</description><subject>Association Measures</subject><subject>Codes</subject><subject>Feedback (Response)</subject><subject>Meta Analysis</subject><subject>Numbers</subject><subject>Pictorial Stimuli</subject><subject>Reading</subject><subject>Reading Habits</subject><subject>Semantics</subject><subject>Semiotics</subject><subject>Semitic Languages</subject><subject>Stimuli</subject><issn>1943-3921</issn><issn>1943-393X</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2023</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>8G5</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>GUQSH</sourceid><sourceid>M2O</sourceid><recordid>eNqNjMFuwjAQRK0KpNLSH-hpJc4BO5smgWtFxQdw4BZc2IAjx06zi1D_HtRW9MphNDOap1Hq1egpFm_ljD1mJkt0ilcVWZ6cH9TIzDNMcI6bwS2n5lE9MTda55gXeqS2q3gGcS3BgYSBOyvO-gXUdiexZ9iTUN-64MIB5EjAYj-dd_INNuzBhf8e6x-gpSDW_156F2ishrX1TC9__qwmH8v1-yrp-vh1Ipaqiac-XKcqLUvUJjemwPuoC-OCTHA</recordid><startdate>20231001</startdate><enddate>20231001</enddate><creator>Scozia, Gabriele</creator><creator>Pinto, Mario</creator><creator>Pellegrino, Michele</creator><creator>Lozito, Silvana</creator><creator>Pia, Lorenzo</creator><creator>Lasaponara, Stefano</creator><creator>Doricchi, Fabrizio</creator><general>Springer Nature B.V</general><scope>0-V</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>4T-</scope><scope>4U-</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88B</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>88J</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>8G5</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ALSLI</scope><scope>AN0</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>CJNVE</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>GUQSH</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>M0P</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>M2O</scope><scope>M2R</scope><scope>MBDVC</scope><scope>PHGZM</scope><scope>PHGZT</scope><scope>PJZUB</scope><scope>PKEHL</scope><scope>POGQB</scope><scope>PPXIY</scope><scope>PQEDU</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PRQQA</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>Q9U</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20231001</creationdate><title>How time gets spatial: factors determining the stability and instability of the mental time line</title><author>Scozia, Gabriele ; Pinto, Mario ; Pellegrino, Michele ; Lozito, Silvana ; Pia, Lorenzo ; Lasaponara, Stefano ; Doricchi, Fabrizio</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-proquest_journals_28830161173</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2023</creationdate><topic>Association Measures</topic><topic>Codes</topic><topic>Feedback (Response)</topic><topic>Meta Analysis</topic><topic>Numbers</topic><topic>Pictorial Stimuli</topic><topic>Reading</topic><topic>Reading Habits</topic><topic>Semantics</topic><topic>Semiotics</topic><topic>Semitic Languages</topic><topic>Stimuli</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Scozia, Gabriele</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Pinto, Mario</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Pellegrino, Michele</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Lozito, Silvana</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Pia, Lorenzo</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Lasaponara, Stefano</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Doricchi, Fabrizio</creatorcontrib><collection>ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Docstoc</collection><collection>University Readers</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Education Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>Social Science Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Research Library (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>Social Science Premium Collection</collection><collection>British Nursing Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>Education Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>Research Library Prep</collection><collection>ProQuest Health &amp; Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Education Database</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Psychology</collection><collection>Research Library</collection><collection>Social Science Database</collection><collection>Research Library (Corporate)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (New)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic (New)</collection><collection>ProQuest Health &amp; Medical Research Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New)</collection><collection>ProQuest Sociology &amp; Social Sciences Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest One Health &amp; Nursing</collection><collection>ProQuest One Education</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Social Sciences</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><jtitle>Attention, perception &amp; psychophysics</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Scozia, Gabriele</au><au>Pinto, Mario</au><au>Pellegrino, Michele</au><au>Lozito, Silvana</au><au>Pia, Lorenzo</au><au>Lasaponara, Stefano</au><au>Doricchi, Fabrizio</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>How time gets spatial: factors determining the stability and instability of the mental time line</atitle><jtitle>Attention, perception &amp; psychophysics</jtitle><date>2023-10-01</date><risdate>2023</risdate><volume>85</volume><issue>7</issue><spage>2321</spage><epage>2336</epage><pages>2321-2336</pages><issn>1943-3921</issn><eissn>1943-393X</eissn><abstract>Left-to-right readers classify faster past events with motor responses on the left side of space and future events with responses on the right side. This suggests a left-to-right spatial organization in the mental representation of time. Here, we show that the significance and reliability of this representation are linked to the joint use of temporal and spatial codes in the task at hand. In a first unimanual Go/No-Go Implicit Association Test (IAT), attending selectively to "past" or to "future" words did not activate corresponding "left" or "right" spatial concepts and vice versa. In a second IAT, attending to both temporal (i.e., "past" and "future") words and spatial targets (i.e., "left" and "right") pointing arrows produced faster responses for congruent rather than incongruent combinations of temporal and spatial concepts in task instructions (e.g., congruent = "Go with past words and left-pointing arrows"; incongruent = "Go with past words and right-pointing arrows"). This effect increased markedly in a STEARC task where spatial codes defined the selection between "left-side" and "right-side" button presses that were associated with "past" and "future" words. Two control experiments showed only partial or unreliable space-time congruency effects when (a) participants attended to superordinate semantic codes that included both spatial "left"/"right" or temporal "past/future" subordinate codes; (b) a primary speeded response was assigned to one dimension (e.g., "past vs. future") and a nonspeeded one to the other dimension (e.g., "left" vs. "right"). These results help to define the conditions that trigger a stable and reliable spatial representation of time-related concepts.</abstract><cop>Austin</cop><pub>Springer Nature B.V</pub><doi>10.3758/sl3414-023-02746-w</doi></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 1943-3921
ispartof Attention, perception & psychophysics, 2023-10, Vol.85 (7), p.2321-2336
issn 1943-3921
1943-393X
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_journals_2883016117
source Springer Nature - Complete Springer Journals; Alma/SFX Local Collection
subjects Association Measures
Codes
Feedback (Response)
Meta Analysis
Numbers
Pictorial Stimuli
Reading
Reading Habits
Semantics
Semiotics
Semitic Languages
Stimuli
title How time gets spatial: factors determining the stability and instability of the mental time line
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-19T07%3A44%3A38IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=How%20time%20gets%20spatial:%20factors%20determining%20the%20stability%20and%20instability%20of%20the%20mental%20time%20line&rft.jtitle=Attention,%20perception%20&%20psychophysics&rft.au=Scozia,%20Gabriele&rft.date=2023-10-01&rft.volume=85&rft.issue=7&rft.spage=2321&rft.epage=2336&rft.pages=2321-2336&rft.issn=1943-3921&rft.eissn=1943-393X&rft_id=info:doi/10.3758/sl3414-023-02746-w&rft_dat=%3Cproquest%3E2883016117%3C/proquest%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2883016117&rft_id=info:pmid/&rfr_iscdi=true