Development of synchrony‐dominant expectations in observers

Movement synchrony is an essential feature of rituals and collective displays around the world. Previous studies have shown that synchronous movements increase observers’ perceived group entitativity and fighting capacity in agonistic contexts; however, little is known about the developmental roots...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Social development (Oxford, England) England), 2022-05, Vol.31 (2), p.497-509
Hauptverfasser: Meng, Xianwei, Kato, Masaharu, Itakura, Shoji
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 509
container_issue 2
container_start_page 497
container_title Social development (Oxford, England)
container_volume 31
creator Meng, Xianwei
Kato, Masaharu
Itakura, Shoji
description Movement synchrony is an essential feature of rituals and collective displays around the world. Previous studies have shown that synchronous movements increase observers’ perceived group entitativity and fighting capacity in agonistic contexts; however, little is known about the developmental roots of synchrony‐dominant attributions. In this study, adult participants (Exp 1), 5‐ to 6‐year‐old children (Exp 2) and 7‐ to 8‐year‐old children (Exp 3) predicted outcomes of zero‐sum conflicts between two groups of animated agents after each group exhibited either synchronised or non‐synchronised movements. Predictions were made for conflicts between two individual members from different groups (individual‐conflict condition) and between all members of the two groups (group‐conflict condition). Results showed that none of the participant groups showed a synchrony‐dominant preference in the individual‐conflict condition. In the group‐conflict condition, most adult participants expected the groups exhibiting synchronised movements to be socially dominant. Although 5‐ to 6‐year‐old children did not show more biased expectations than can be accounted for by chance, adult‐like synchrony‐dominant preference was found in 7‐ to 8‐year‐old children. Moreover, justifications of the synchrony‐dominant expectation in both adults and 7‐ to 8‐year‐old children showed that this expectation was driven by the perceived entitativity of the synchronous group. These findings show that the expectation of group dominance based on synchronous movements is strongly developed in school‐age children, explaining the developmental origins of intergroup attitudes as well as the cognitive mechanisms children employ when making judgments in intergroup contexts.
doi_str_mv 10.1111/sode.12556
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_journals_2653190116</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>2653190116</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c3676-ec4d34ecf504aba35040bdbf164ff461ffe7aaa1c5d99fd27e17cf9a014f26dc3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9kMtKw0AUhgdRsFY3PkHAnZA6J5kLWbiQtl6g0IUK7obJzBlMaTNxJq1m5yP4jD6JqXHt2fxw-P5z4CPkHOgE-rmK3uIEMs7FARkBEzItOJWHZEQLAWlG-csxOYlxRSllMpcjcj3DHa59s8G6TbxLYleb1-Dr7vvzy_pNVet-jx8Nmla3la9jUtWJLyOGHYZ4So6cXkc8-8sxeb6dP03v08Xy7mF6s0hNLqRI0TCbMzSOU6ZLnfdBS1s6EMw5JsA5lFprMNwWhbOZRJDGFZoCc5mwJh-Ti-FuE_zbFmOrVn4b6v6lygTPoaAAoqcuB8oEH2NAp5pQbXToFFC116P2etSvnh6GAX6v1tj9Q6rH5Ww-dH4AEDBqZg</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2653190116</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Development of synchrony‐dominant expectations in observers</title><source>Wiley-Blackwell Journals</source><source>Sociological Abstracts</source><source>Applied Social Sciences Index &amp; Abstracts (ASSIA)</source><creator>Meng, Xianwei ; Kato, Masaharu ; Itakura, Shoji</creator><creatorcontrib>Meng, Xianwei ; Kato, Masaharu ; Itakura, Shoji</creatorcontrib><description>Movement synchrony is an essential feature of rituals and collective displays around the world. Previous studies have shown that synchronous movements increase observers’ perceived group entitativity and fighting capacity in agonistic contexts; however, little is known about the developmental roots of synchrony‐dominant attributions. In this study, adult participants (Exp 1), 5‐ to 6‐year‐old children (Exp 2) and 7‐ to 8‐year‐old children (Exp 3) predicted outcomes of zero‐sum conflicts between two groups of animated agents after each group exhibited either synchronised or non‐synchronised movements. Predictions were made for conflicts between two individual members from different groups (individual‐conflict condition) and between all members of the two groups (group‐conflict condition). Results showed that none of the participant groups showed a synchrony‐dominant preference in the individual‐conflict condition. In the group‐conflict condition, most adult participants expected the groups exhibiting synchronised movements to be socially dominant. Although 5‐ to 6‐year‐old children did not show more biased expectations than can be accounted for by chance, adult‐like synchrony‐dominant preference was found in 7‐ to 8‐year‐old children. Moreover, justifications of the synchrony‐dominant expectation in both adults and 7‐ to 8‐year‐old children showed that this expectation was driven by the perceived entitativity of the synchronous group. These findings show that the expectation of group dominance based on synchronous movements is strongly developed in school‐age children, explaining the developmental origins of intergroup attitudes as well as the cognitive mechanisms children employ when making judgments in intergroup contexts.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0961-205X</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1467-9507</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1111/sode.12556</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Oxford: Blackwell Publishing Ltd</publisher><subject>Adults ; Attribution ; child development ; Children ; Conflict ; Dominance ; entitativity ; group‐conflict ; movement synchrony ; Observation ; Rituals ; Synchrony</subject><ispartof>Social development (Oxford, England), 2022-05, Vol.31 (2), p.497-509</ispartof><rights>2021 John Wiley &amp; Sons Ltd.</rights><rights>2022 John Wiley &amp; Sons Ltd.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c3676-ec4d34ecf504aba35040bdbf164ff461ffe7aaa1c5d99fd27e17cf9a014f26dc3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c3676-ec4d34ecf504aba35040bdbf164ff461ffe7aaa1c5d99fd27e17cf9a014f26dc3</cites><orcidid>0000-0002-7873-7518</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/pdf/10.1111%2Fsode.12556$$EPDF$$P50$$Gwiley$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1111%2Fsode.12556$$EHTML$$P50$$Gwiley$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,1416,27915,27916,30990,33765,45565,45566</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Meng, Xianwei</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kato, Masaharu</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Itakura, Shoji</creatorcontrib><title>Development of synchrony‐dominant expectations in observers</title><title>Social development (Oxford, England)</title><description>Movement synchrony is an essential feature of rituals and collective displays around the world. Previous studies have shown that synchronous movements increase observers’ perceived group entitativity and fighting capacity in agonistic contexts; however, little is known about the developmental roots of synchrony‐dominant attributions. In this study, adult participants (Exp 1), 5‐ to 6‐year‐old children (Exp 2) and 7‐ to 8‐year‐old children (Exp 3) predicted outcomes of zero‐sum conflicts between two groups of animated agents after each group exhibited either synchronised or non‐synchronised movements. Predictions were made for conflicts between two individual members from different groups (individual‐conflict condition) and between all members of the two groups (group‐conflict condition). Results showed that none of the participant groups showed a synchrony‐dominant preference in the individual‐conflict condition. In the group‐conflict condition, most adult participants expected the groups exhibiting synchronised movements to be socially dominant. Although 5‐ to 6‐year‐old children did not show more biased expectations than can be accounted for by chance, adult‐like synchrony‐dominant preference was found in 7‐ to 8‐year‐old children. Moreover, justifications of the synchrony‐dominant expectation in both adults and 7‐ to 8‐year‐old children showed that this expectation was driven by the perceived entitativity of the synchronous group. These findings show that the expectation of group dominance based on synchronous movements is strongly developed in school‐age children, explaining the developmental origins of intergroup attitudes as well as the cognitive mechanisms children employ when making judgments in intergroup contexts.</description><subject>Adults</subject><subject>Attribution</subject><subject>child development</subject><subject>Children</subject><subject>Conflict</subject><subject>Dominance</subject><subject>entitativity</subject><subject>group‐conflict</subject><subject>movement synchrony</subject><subject>Observation</subject><subject>Rituals</subject><subject>Synchrony</subject><issn>0961-205X</issn><issn>1467-9507</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2022</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>7QJ</sourceid><sourceid>BHHNA</sourceid><recordid>eNp9kMtKw0AUhgdRsFY3PkHAnZA6J5kLWbiQtl6g0IUK7obJzBlMaTNxJq1m5yP4jD6JqXHt2fxw-P5z4CPkHOgE-rmK3uIEMs7FARkBEzItOJWHZEQLAWlG-csxOYlxRSllMpcjcj3DHa59s8G6TbxLYleb1-Dr7vvzy_pNVet-jx8Nmla3la9jUtWJLyOGHYZ4So6cXkc8-8sxeb6dP03v08Xy7mF6s0hNLqRI0TCbMzSOU6ZLnfdBS1s6EMw5JsA5lFprMNwWhbOZRJDGFZoCc5mwJh-Ti-FuE_zbFmOrVn4b6v6lygTPoaAAoqcuB8oEH2NAp5pQbXToFFC116P2etSvnh6GAX6v1tj9Q6rH5Ww-dH4AEDBqZg</recordid><startdate>202205</startdate><enddate>202205</enddate><creator>Meng, Xianwei</creator><creator>Kato, Masaharu</creator><creator>Itakura, Shoji</creator><general>Blackwell Publishing Ltd</general><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7QJ</scope><scope>7U4</scope><scope>BHHNA</scope><scope>DWI</scope><scope>WZK</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7873-7518</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>202205</creationdate><title>Development of synchrony‐dominant expectations in observers</title><author>Meng, Xianwei ; Kato, Masaharu ; Itakura, Shoji</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c3676-ec4d34ecf504aba35040bdbf164ff461ffe7aaa1c5d99fd27e17cf9a014f26dc3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2022</creationdate><topic>Adults</topic><topic>Attribution</topic><topic>child development</topic><topic>Children</topic><topic>Conflict</topic><topic>Dominance</topic><topic>entitativity</topic><topic>group‐conflict</topic><topic>movement synchrony</topic><topic>Observation</topic><topic>Rituals</topic><topic>Synchrony</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Meng, Xianwei</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kato, Masaharu</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Itakura, Shoji</creatorcontrib><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Applied Social Sciences Index &amp; Abstracts (ASSIA)</collection><collection>Sociological Abstracts (pre-2017)</collection><collection>Sociological Abstracts</collection><collection>Sociological Abstracts</collection><collection>Sociological Abstracts (Ovid)</collection><jtitle>Social development (Oxford, England)</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Meng, Xianwei</au><au>Kato, Masaharu</au><au>Itakura, Shoji</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Development of synchrony‐dominant expectations in observers</atitle><jtitle>Social development (Oxford, England)</jtitle><date>2022-05</date><risdate>2022</risdate><volume>31</volume><issue>2</issue><spage>497</spage><epage>509</epage><pages>497-509</pages><issn>0961-205X</issn><eissn>1467-9507</eissn><abstract>Movement synchrony is an essential feature of rituals and collective displays around the world. Previous studies have shown that synchronous movements increase observers’ perceived group entitativity and fighting capacity in agonistic contexts; however, little is known about the developmental roots of synchrony‐dominant attributions. In this study, adult participants (Exp 1), 5‐ to 6‐year‐old children (Exp 2) and 7‐ to 8‐year‐old children (Exp 3) predicted outcomes of zero‐sum conflicts between two groups of animated agents after each group exhibited either synchronised or non‐synchronised movements. Predictions were made for conflicts between two individual members from different groups (individual‐conflict condition) and between all members of the two groups (group‐conflict condition). Results showed that none of the participant groups showed a synchrony‐dominant preference in the individual‐conflict condition. In the group‐conflict condition, most adult participants expected the groups exhibiting synchronised movements to be socially dominant. Although 5‐ to 6‐year‐old children did not show more biased expectations than can be accounted for by chance, adult‐like synchrony‐dominant preference was found in 7‐ to 8‐year‐old children. Moreover, justifications of the synchrony‐dominant expectation in both adults and 7‐ to 8‐year‐old children showed that this expectation was driven by the perceived entitativity of the synchronous group. These findings show that the expectation of group dominance based on synchronous movements is strongly developed in school‐age children, explaining the developmental origins of intergroup attitudes as well as the cognitive mechanisms children employ when making judgments in intergroup contexts.</abstract><cop>Oxford</cop><pub>Blackwell Publishing Ltd</pub><doi>10.1111/sode.12556</doi><tpages>13</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7873-7518</orcidid></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0961-205X
ispartof Social development (Oxford, England), 2022-05, Vol.31 (2), p.497-509
issn 0961-205X
1467-9507
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_journals_2653190116
source Wiley-Blackwell Journals; Sociological Abstracts; Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA)
subjects Adults
Attribution
child development
Children
Conflict
Dominance
entitativity
group‐conflict
movement synchrony
Observation
Rituals
Synchrony
title Development of synchrony‐dominant expectations in observers
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-14T23%3A41%3A15IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Development%20of%20synchrony%E2%80%90dominant%20expectations%20in%20observers&rft.jtitle=Social%20development%20(Oxford,%20England)&rft.au=Meng,%20Xianwei&rft.date=2022-05&rft.volume=31&rft.issue=2&rft.spage=497&rft.epage=509&rft.pages=497-509&rft.issn=0961-205X&rft.eissn=1467-9507&rft_id=info:doi/10.1111/sode.12556&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E2653190116%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2653190116&rft_id=info:pmid/&rfr_iscdi=true