Normative responding on cognitive bias tasks: Some evidence for a weak rationality factor that is mostly explained by numeracy and actively open-minded thinking

We conducted two studies with two goals in mind. First, we investigated the dimensionality of several prominent cognitive bias tasks to see whether a single rationality factor can explain a performance on these tasks. Second, we validated this factor by correlating it with a number of constructs fro...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Intelligence (Norwood) 2022-01, Vol.90, p.101619, Article 101619
Hauptverfasser: Erceg, Nikola, Galić, Zvonimir, Bubić, Andreja
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page
container_issue
container_start_page 101619
container_title Intelligence (Norwood)
container_volume 90
creator Erceg, Nikola
Galić, Zvonimir
Bubić, Andreja
description We conducted two studies with two goals in mind. First, we investigated the dimensionality of several prominent cognitive bias tasks to see whether a single rationality factor can explain a performance on these tasks. Second, we validated this factor by correlating it with a number of constructs from its nomological network (fluid intelligence, numeracy, actively open-minded thinking, conspiracy and superstitious thinking, personality traits) and several real-life outcomes (decision-outcome inventory, job and career satisfaction, peer-rated decision-making quality). Although in both studies one-factor solution was the most appropriate for our tasks, this factor (i.e., “rationality factor”) was weak and only able to account for modest portion of variance among the tasks. Across both studies, the two strongest correlates of this rationality factor were numeracy and actively open-minded thinking. We conclude that cognitive bias tasks are highly heterogeneous, having very little in common. What they had in common, however, was largely underpinned by abilities and dispositions assessed with numeracy and actively open-minded thinking. We discuss how our findings relate to the dual-process theories and offer our view on the place of rationality in a broader model of human intelligence. •A weak rationality factor underpins performance on several prominent cognitive bias tasks.•This factor exhibits relatively high correlations with numeracy and actively open-minded thinking.•Conversely, its correlations with real-life outcomes are mostly low or non-existent.•Both abilities and dispositions are crucial for rationality, as predicted by tripartite theories.•We argue for inclusion of rationality in the CHC model of intelligence.
doi_str_mv 10.1016/j.intell.2021.101619
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_journals_2636863447</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><els_id>S0160289621001033</els_id><sourcerecordid>2636863447</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c380t-df26ad71f66a8c6c0d1ac31299358407e166c454775a0fa3b70c392be9646cf3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9kc2O1DAQhC0EEsPCG3BoiXMGO844CQcktOJPWsGBvVs9dmfXM4kdbM8seRseFc9mz5wslb8uu7oYeyv4VnCh3h-2zmcax23Na_Eoif4Z24iulVXby91ztikar-quVy_Zq5QOnPOdaLoN-_sjxAmzOxNESnPw1vk7CB5MuPPuUd87TJAxHdMH-BUmAjo7S94QDCECwgPhEWLxCB5HlxcY0ORyk-8xg0swhZTHBejPPKLzZGG_gD9NFNEsgN5Cwcs7BQkz-Wpy3hYo3zt_LH95zV4MOCZ683Resdsvn2-vv1U3P79-v_50UxnZ8VzZoVZoWzEohZ1RhluBRoq6L_G7hrcklDLNrmnbHfIB5b7lRvb1nnrVKDPIK_ZutZ1j-H2ilPUhnGIJlHStpOqUbJq2UM1KmRhSijToOboJ46IF15e964Neq9CXKvRaRRn7uI5RCXB2FHUy7rJB6yKZrG1w_zf4B0Nwl1g</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2636863447</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Normative responding on cognitive bias tasks: Some evidence for a weak rationality factor that is mostly explained by numeracy and actively open-minded thinking</title><source>Applied Social Sciences Index &amp; Abstracts (ASSIA)</source><source>Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals</source><creator>Erceg, Nikola ; Galić, Zvonimir ; Bubić, Andreja</creator><creatorcontrib>Erceg, Nikola ; Galić, Zvonimir ; Bubić, Andreja</creatorcontrib><description>We conducted two studies with two goals in mind. First, we investigated the dimensionality of several prominent cognitive bias tasks to see whether a single rationality factor can explain a performance on these tasks. Second, we validated this factor by correlating it with a number of constructs from its nomological network (fluid intelligence, numeracy, actively open-minded thinking, conspiracy and superstitious thinking, personality traits) and several real-life outcomes (decision-outcome inventory, job and career satisfaction, peer-rated decision-making quality). Although in both studies one-factor solution was the most appropriate for our tasks, this factor (i.e., “rationality factor”) was weak and only able to account for modest portion of variance among the tasks. Across both studies, the two strongest correlates of this rationality factor were numeracy and actively open-minded thinking. We conclude that cognitive bias tasks are highly heterogeneous, having very little in common. What they had in common, however, was largely underpinned by abilities and dispositions assessed with numeracy and actively open-minded thinking. We discuss how our findings relate to the dual-process theories and offer our view on the place of rationality in a broader model of human intelligence. •A weak rationality factor underpins performance on several prominent cognitive bias tasks.•This factor exhibits relatively high correlations with numeracy and actively open-minded thinking.•Conversely, its correlations with real-life outcomes are mostly low or non-existent.•Both abilities and dispositions are crucial for rationality, as predicted by tripartite theories.•We argue for inclusion of rationality in the CHC model of intelligence.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0160-2896</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1873-7935</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2021.101619</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>New York: Elsevier Inc</publisher><subject>Actively open-minded thinking ; Bias ; Cognitive ability ; Cognitive bias ; Decision making ; Fluid intelligence ; Intelligence ; Job satisfaction ; Numeracy ; Personality traits ; Rationality ; Superstition ; Task performance</subject><ispartof>Intelligence (Norwood), 2022-01, Vol.90, p.101619, Article 101619</ispartof><rights>2021 Elsevier Inc.</rights><rights>Copyright Elsevier Science Ltd. Jan/Feb 2022</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c380t-df26ad71f66a8c6c0d1ac31299358407e166c454775a0fa3b70c392be9646cf3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c380t-df26ad71f66a8c6c0d1ac31299358407e166c454775a0fa3b70c392be9646cf3</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0160289621001033$$EHTML$$P50$$Gelsevier$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,3537,27901,27902,30976,65306</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Erceg, Nikola</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Galić, Zvonimir</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Bubić, Andreja</creatorcontrib><title>Normative responding on cognitive bias tasks: Some evidence for a weak rationality factor that is mostly explained by numeracy and actively open-minded thinking</title><title>Intelligence (Norwood)</title><description>We conducted two studies with two goals in mind. First, we investigated the dimensionality of several prominent cognitive bias tasks to see whether a single rationality factor can explain a performance on these tasks. Second, we validated this factor by correlating it with a number of constructs from its nomological network (fluid intelligence, numeracy, actively open-minded thinking, conspiracy and superstitious thinking, personality traits) and several real-life outcomes (decision-outcome inventory, job and career satisfaction, peer-rated decision-making quality). Although in both studies one-factor solution was the most appropriate for our tasks, this factor (i.e., “rationality factor”) was weak and only able to account for modest portion of variance among the tasks. Across both studies, the two strongest correlates of this rationality factor were numeracy and actively open-minded thinking. We conclude that cognitive bias tasks are highly heterogeneous, having very little in common. What they had in common, however, was largely underpinned by abilities and dispositions assessed with numeracy and actively open-minded thinking. We discuss how our findings relate to the dual-process theories and offer our view on the place of rationality in a broader model of human intelligence. •A weak rationality factor underpins performance on several prominent cognitive bias tasks.•This factor exhibits relatively high correlations with numeracy and actively open-minded thinking.•Conversely, its correlations with real-life outcomes are mostly low or non-existent.•Both abilities and dispositions are crucial for rationality, as predicted by tripartite theories.•We argue for inclusion of rationality in the CHC model of intelligence.</description><subject>Actively open-minded thinking</subject><subject>Bias</subject><subject>Cognitive ability</subject><subject>Cognitive bias</subject><subject>Decision making</subject><subject>Fluid intelligence</subject><subject>Intelligence</subject><subject>Job satisfaction</subject><subject>Numeracy</subject><subject>Personality traits</subject><subject>Rationality</subject><subject>Superstition</subject><subject>Task performance</subject><issn>0160-2896</issn><issn>1873-7935</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2022</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>7QJ</sourceid><recordid>eNp9kc2O1DAQhC0EEsPCG3BoiXMGO844CQcktOJPWsGBvVs9dmfXM4kdbM8seRseFc9mz5wslb8uu7oYeyv4VnCh3h-2zmcax23Na_Eoif4Z24iulVXby91ztikar-quVy_Zq5QOnPOdaLoN-_sjxAmzOxNESnPw1vk7CB5MuPPuUd87TJAxHdMH-BUmAjo7S94QDCECwgPhEWLxCB5HlxcY0ORyk-8xg0swhZTHBejPPKLzZGG_gD9NFNEsgN5Cwcs7BQkz-Wpy3hYo3zt_LH95zV4MOCZ683Resdsvn2-vv1U3P79-v_50UxnZ8VzZoVZoWzEohZ1RhluBRoq6L_G7hrcklDLNrmnbHfIB5b7lRvb1nnrVKDPIK_ZutZ1j-H2ilPUhnGIJlHStpOqUbJq2UM1KmRhSijToOboJ46IF15e964Neq9CXKvRaRRn7uI5RCXB2FHUy7rJB6yKZrG1w_zf4B0Nwl1g</recordid><startdate>202201</startdate><enddate>202201</enddate><creator>Erceg, Nikola</creator><creator>Galić, Zvonimir</creator><creator>Bubić, Andreja</creator><general>Elsevier Inc</general><general>Elsevier Science Ltd</general><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7QJ</scope><scope>7TK</scope></search><sort><creationdate>202201</creationdate><title>Normative responding on cognitive bias tasks: Some evidence for a weak rationality factor that is mostly explained by numeracy and actively open-minded thinking</title><author>Erceg, Nikola ; Galić, Zvonimir ; Bubić, Andreja</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c380t-df26ad71f66a8c6c0d1ac31299358407e166c454775a0fa3b70c392be9646cf3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2022</creationdate><topic>Actively open-minded thinking</topic><topic>Bias</topic><topic>Cognitive ability</topic><topic>Cognitive bias</topic><topic>Decision making</topic><topic>Fluid intelligence</topic><topic>Intelligence</topic><topic>Job satisfaction</topic><topic>Numeracy</topic><topic>Personality traits</topic><topic>Rationality</topic><topic>Superstition</topic><topic>Task performance</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Erceg, Nikola</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Galić, Zvonimir</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Bubić, Andreja</creatorcontrib><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Applied Social Sciences Index &amp; Abstracts (ASSIA)</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><jtitle>Intelligence (Norwood)</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Erceg, Nikola</au><au>Galić, Zvonimir</au><au>Bubić, Andreja</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Normative responding on cognitive bias tasks: Some evidence for a weak rationality factor that is mostly explained by numeracy and actively open-minded thinking</atitle><jtitle>Intelligence (Norwood)</jtitle><date>2022-01</date><risdate>2022</risdate><volume>90</volume><spage>101619</spage><pages>101619-</pages><artnum>101619</artnum><issn>0160-2896</issn><eissn>1873-7935</eissn><abstract>We conducted two studies with two goals in mind. First, we investigated the dimensionality of several prominent cognitive bias tasks to see whether a single rationality factor can explain a performance on these tasks. Second, we validated this factor by correlating it with a number of constructs from its nomological network (fluid intelligence, numeracy, actively open-minded thinking, conspiracy and superstitious thinking, personality traits) and several real-life outcomes (decision-outcome inventory, job and career satisfaction, peer-rated decision-making quality). Although in both studies one-factor solution was the most appropriate for our tasks, this factor (i.e., “rationality factor”) was weak and only able to account for modest portion of variance among the tasks. Across both studies, the two strongest correlates of this rationality factor were numeracy and actively open-minded thinking. We conclude that cognitive bias tasks are highly heterogeneous, having very little in common. What they had in common, however, was largely underpinned by abilities and dispositions assessed with numeracy and actively open-minded thinking. We discuss how our findings relate to the dual-process theories and offer our view on the place of rationality in a broader model of human intelligence. •A weak rationality factor underpins performance on several prominent cognitive bias tasks.•This factor exhibits relatively high correlations with numeracy and actively open-minded thinking.•Conversely, its correlations with real-life outcomes are mostly low or non-existent.•Both abilities and dispositions are crucial for rationality, as predicted by tripartite theories.•We argue for inclusion of rationality in the CHC model of intelligence.</abstract><cop>New York</cop><pub>Elsevier Inc</pub><doi>10.1016/j.intell.2021.101619</doi><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0160-2896
ispartof Intelligence (Norwood), 2022-01, Vol.90, p.101619, Article 101619
issn 0160-2896
1873-7935
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_journals_2636863447
source Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA); Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals
subjects Actively open-minded thinking
Bias
Cognitive ability
Cognitive bias
Decision making
Fluid intelligence
Intelligence
Job satisfaction
Numeracy
Personality traits
Rationality
Superstition
Task performance
title Normative responding on cognitive bias tasks: Some evidence for a weak rationality factor that is mostly explained by numeracy and actively open-minded thinking
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-10T02%3A38%3A55IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Normative%20responding%20on%20cognitive%20bias%20tasks:%20Some%20evidence%20for%20a%20weak%20rationality%20factor%20that%20is%20mostly%20explained%20by%20numeracy%20and%20actively%20open-minded%20thinking&rft.jtitle=Intelligence%20(Norwood)&rft.au=Erceg,%20Nikola&rft.date=2022-01&rft.volume=90&rft.spage=101619&rft.pages=101619-&rft.artnum=101619&rft.issn=0160-2896&rft.eissn=1873-7935&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/j.intell.2021.101619&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E2636863447%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2636863447&rft_id=info:pmid/&rft_els_id=S0160289621001033&rfr_iscdi=true