Forget the audience: tadpoles release similar disturbance cues regardless of kinship or familiarity
Group-living prey rely on social information such as alarm signals and other social cues to avoid predation. By definition, “signals” imply that a message is voluntarily directed at receivers (i.e., the audience), whereas “cues” are released incidentally regardless of the audience composition. Thus,...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Behavioral ecology and sociobiology 2020-12, Vol.74 (12), p.1-10, Article 147 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , , , , , , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 10 |
---|---|
container_issue | 12 |
container_start_page | 1 |
container_title | Behavioral ecology and sociobiology |
container_volume | 74 |
creator | Bairos-Novak, Kevin R. Crane, Adam L. Achtymichuk, Gabrielle H. Hsin, Jonathan Rivera-Hernández, Ita A. E. Simko, Olena M. Wrynn, Theresa E. Chivers, Douglas P. Ferrari, Maud C. O. |
description | Group-living prey rely on social information such as alarm signals and other social cues to avoid predation. By definition, “signals” imply that a message is voluntarily directed at receivers (i.e., the audience), whereas “cues” are released incidentally regardless of the audience composition. Thus, audience effects can be used to differentiate between signals and cues when communication is difficult to observe or quantify. In at least two fish species, chemical disturbance cues are released during a predator attack to signal to familiar audiences about predation risk. Here, we examined whether audience composition affects disturbance cue release in wood frog (Lithobates sylvaticus) tadpoles to better understand the function of disturbance cues across aquatic prey. Groups of tadpoles underwent simulated predator attacks to obtain disturbance cues. The groups were either familiar and related, unfamiliar and related, familiar and unrelated, or unfamiliar and unrelated. To assess the relative potency of each cue, we used a behavioral bioassay design involving activity changes in independent tadpole receivers (unfamiliar and unrelated to the donors). If tadpoles use disturbance cues to signal related and/or familiar individuals, we predicted increased fright responses in receivers to cues obtained from those groups. However, we detected no effect of audience composition, indicating that tadpoles release similar disturbance cues regardless of audience kinship or familiarity. Nevertheless, disturbance cues evoked a consistent antipredator response in receivers indicating that these chemicals still act as reliable risk cues. Further comparative studies using audience effects are necessary to understand how disturbance cues have evolved across aquatic prey. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1007/s00265-020-02936-8 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>jstor_proqu</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_journals_2473397161</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><jstor_id>48727847</jstor_id><sourcerecordid>48727847</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c341t-daf2ff739b68af62f1e0742706814b46154625e13c75d9b73588fda4e80ee3003</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9jzFPwzAQRi0EEqUwsSEhVWI2nH2O7YyoooBUiQVmy0nOpREkxU4G_j2mQbAxnG753nf3GLsQcC0AzE0CkLrgICFPiZrbAzYTCiUHo-UhmwEq4IVSeMxOUmoBQAtrZ-x81ccNDYvhlRZ-bLbU1XTKjoJ_S3T2s-fsZXX3vHzg66f7x-XtmteoxMAbH2QIBstKWx-0DILAKGlAW6EqpUWhtCxIYG2KpqwMFtaGxiuyQIQAOGdXU-8u9h8jpcG1_Ri7fNJJZRBLI7TIKTml6tinFCm4Xdy--_jpBLhveTfJuyzv9vLOZggnKOVwt6H4V_0vdTlRbRr6-HtHWSONzR99AZQrYtM</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2473397161</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Forget the audience: tadpoles release similar disturbance cues regardless of kinship or familiarity</title><source>Jstor Complete Legacy</source><source>SpringerLink Journals - AutoHoldings</source><creator>Bairos-Novak, Kevin R. ; Crane, Adam L. ; Achtymichuk, Gabrielle H. ; Hsin, Jonathan ; Rivera-Hernández, Ita A. E. ; Simko, Olena M. ; Wrynn, Theresa E. ; Chivers, Douglas P. ; Ferrari, Maud C. O.</creator><creatorcontrib>Bairos-Novak, Kevin R. ; Crane, Adam L. ; Achtymichuk, Gabrielle H. ; Hsin, Jonathan ; Rivera-Hernández, Ita A. E. ; Simko, Olena M. ; Wrynn, Theresa E. ; Chivers, Douglas P. ; Ferrari, Maud C. O.</creatorcontrib><description>Group-living prey rely on social information such as alarm signals and other social cues to avoid predation. By definition, “signals” imply that a message is voluntarily directed at receivers (i.e., the audience), whereas “cues” are released incidentally regardless of the audience composition. Thus, audience effects can be used to differentiate between signals and cues when communication is difficult to observe or quantify. In at least two fish species, chemical disturbance cues are released during a predator attack to signal to familiar audiences about predation risk. Here, we examined whether audience composition affects disturbance cue release in wood frog (Lithobates sylvaticus) tadpoles to better understand the function of disturbance cues across aquatic prey. Groups of tadpoles underwent simulated predator attacks to obtain disturbance cues. The groups were either familiar and related, unfamiliar and related, familiar and unrelated, or unfamiliar and unrelated. To assess the relative potency of each cue, we used a behavioral bioassay design involving activity changes in independent tadpole receivers (unfamiliar and unrelated to the donors). If tadpoles use disturbance cues to signal related and/or familiar individuals, we predicted increased fright responses in receivers to cues obtained from those groups. However, we detected no effect of audience composition, indicating that tadpoles release similar disturbance cues regardless of audience kinship or familiarity. Nevertheless, disturbance cues evoked a consistent antipredator response in receivers indicating that these chemicals still act as reliable risk cues. Further comparative studies using audience effects are necessary to understand how disturbance cues have evolved across aquatic prey.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0340-5443</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1432-0762</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1007/s00265-020-02936-8</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer Science + Business Media</publisher><subject>Amphibians ; Animal behavior ; Animal Ecology ; Anti-predator behavior ; Audiences ; Behavioral Sciences ; Bioassays ; Biomedical and Life Sciences ; Chemicals ; Comparative studies ; Composition effects ; Cues ; Disturbance ; Familiarity ; Frogs ; Juveniles ; Life Sciences ; ORIGINAL ARTICLE ; Predation ; Predator-prey simulation ; Predators ; Prey ; Receivers ; Siblings ; Smell ; Zoology</subject><ispartof>Behavioral ecology and sociobiology, 2020-12, Vol.74 (12), p.1-10, Article 147</ispartof><rights>Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2020</rights><rights>Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2020.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c341t-daf2ff739b68af62f1e0742706814b46154625e13c75d9b73588fda4e80ee3003</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c341t-daf2ff739b68af62f1e0742706814b46154625e13c75d9b73588fda4e80ee3003</cites><orcidid>0000-0002-0152-1452 ; 0000-0002-8645-8180</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.jstor.org/stable/pdf/48727847$$EPDF$$P50$$Gjstor$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.jstor.org/stable/48727847$$EHTML$$P50$$Gjstor$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,778,782,801,27907,27908,41471,42540,51302,58000,58233</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Bairos-Novak, Kevin R.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Crane, Adam L.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Achtymichuk, Gabrielle H.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Hsin, Jonathan</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Rivera-Hernández, Ita A. E.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Simko, Olena M.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Wrynn, Theresa E.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Chivers, Douglas P.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Ferrari, Maud C. O.</creatorcontrib><title>Forget the audience: tadpoles release similar disturbance cues regardless of kinship or familiarity</title><title>Behavioral ecology and sociobiology</title><addtitle>Behav Ecol Sociobiol</addtitle><description>Group-living prey rely on social information such as alarm signals and other social cues to avoid predation. By definition, “signals” imply that a message is voluntarily directed at receivers (i.e., the audience), whereas “cues” are released incidentally regardless of the audience composition. Thus, audience effects can be used to differentiate between signals and cues when communication is difficult to observe or quantify. In at least two fish species, chemical disturbance cues are released during a predator attack to signal to familiar audiences about predation risk. Here, we examined whether audience composition affects disturbance cue release in wood frog (Lithobates sylvaticus) tadpoles to better understand the function of disturbance cues across aquatic prey. Groups of tadpoles underwent simulated predator attacks to obtain disturbance cues. The groups were either familiar and related, unfamiliar and related, familiar and unrelated, or unfamiliar and unrelated. To assess the relative potency of each cue, we used a behavioral bioassay design involving activity changes in independent tadpole receivers (unfamiliar and unrelated to the donors). If tadpoles use disturbance cues to signal related and/or familiar individuals, we predicted increased fright responses in receivers to cues obtained from those groups. However, we detected no effect of audience composition, indicating that tadpoles release similar disturbance cues regardless of audience kinship or familiarity. Nevertheless, disturbance cues evoked a consistent antipredator response in receivers indicating that these chemicals still act as reliable risk cues. Further comparative studies using audience effects are necessary to understand how disturbance cues have evolved across aquatic prey.</description><subject>Amphibians</subject><subject>Animal behavior</subject><subject>Animal Ecology</subject><subject>Anti-predator behavior</subject><subject>Audiences</subject><subject>Behavioral Sciences</subject><subject>Bioassays</subject><subject>Biomedical and Life Sciences</subject><subject>Chemicals</subject><subject>Comparative studies</subject><subject>Composition effects</subject><subject>Cues</subject><subject>Disturbance</subject><subject>Familiarity</subject><subject>Frogs</subject><subject>Juveniles</subject><subject>Life Sciences</subject><subject>ORIGINAL ARTICLE</subject><subject>Predation</subject><subject>Predator-prey simulation</subject><subject>Predators</subject><subject>Prey</subject><subject>Receivers</subject><subject>Siblings</subject><subject>Smell</subject><subject>Zoology</subject><issn>0340-5443</issn><issn>1432-0762</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2020</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>ABUWG</sourceid><sourceid>AFKRA</sourceid><sourceid>AZQEC</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>CCPQU</sourceid><sourceid>DWQXO</sourceid><sourceid>GNUQQ</sourceid><recordid>eNp9jzFPwzAQRi0EEqUwsSEhVWI2nH2O7YyoooBUiQVmy0nOpREkxU4G_j2mQbAxnG753nf3GLsQcC0AzE0CkLrgICFPiZrbAzYTCiUHo-UhmwEq4IVSeMxOUmoBQAtrZ-x81ccNDYvhlRZ-bLbU1XTKjoJ_S3T2s-fsZXX3vHzg66f7x-XtmteoxMAbH2QIBstKWx-0DILAKGlAW6EqpUWhtCxIYG2KpqwMFtaGxiuyQIQAOGdXU-8u9h8jpcG1_Ri7fNJJZRBLI7TIKTml6tinFCm4Xdy--_jpBLhveTfJuyzv9vLOZggnKOVwt6H4V_0vdTlRbRr6-HtHWSONzR99AZQrYtM</recordid><startdate>20201201</startdate><enddate>20201201</enddate><creator>Bairos-Novak, Kevin R.</creator><creator>Crane, Adam L.</creator><creator>Achtymichuk, Gabrielle H.</creator><creator>Hsin, Jonathan</creator><creator>Rivera-Hernández, Ita A. E.</creator><creator>Simko, Olena M.</creator><creator>Wrynn, Theresa E.</creator><creator>Chivers, Douglas P.</creator><creator>Ferrari, Maud C. O.</creator><general>Springer Science + Business Media</general><general>Springer Berlin Heidelberg</general><general>Springer Nature B.V</general><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>0-V</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>7QR</scope><scope>7SN</scope><scope>7SS</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>88I</scope><scope>88J</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FE</scope><scope>8FH</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AEUYN</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ALSLI</scope><scope>ATCPS</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BBNVY</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BHPHI</scope><scope>C1K</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>HEHIP</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>LK8</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>M2P</scope><scope>M2R</scope><scope>M2S</scope><scope>M7P</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PATMY</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>PYCSY</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>RC3</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0152-1452</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-8645-8180</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>20201201</creationdate><title>Forget the audience</title><author>Bairos-Novak, Kevin R. ; Crane, Adam L. ; Achtymichuk, Gabrielle H. ; Hsin, Jonathan ; Rivera-Hernández, Ita A. E. ; Simko, Olena M. ; Wrynn, Theresa E. ; Chivers, Douglas P. ; Ferrari, Maud C. O.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c341t-daf2ff739b68af62f1e0742706814b46154625e13c75d9b73588fda4e80ee3003</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2020</creationdate><topic>Amphibians</topic><topic>Animal behavior</topic><topic>Animal Ecology</topic><topic>Anti-predator behavior</topic><topic>Audiences</topic><topic>Behavioral Sciences</topic><topic>Bioassays</topic><topic>Biomedical and Life Sciences</topic><topic>Chemicals</topic><topic>Comparative studies</topic><topic>Composition effects</topic><topic>Cues</topic><topic>Disturbance</topic><topic>Familiarity</topic><topic>Frogs</topic><topic>Juveniles</topic><topic>Life Sciences</topic><topic>ORIGINAL ARTICLE</topic><topic>Predation</topic><topic>Predator-prey simulation</topic><topic>Predators</topic><topic>Prey</topic><topic>Receivers</topic><topic>Siblings</topic><topic>Smell</topic><topic>Zoology</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Bairos-Novak, Kevin R.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Crane, Adam L.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Achtymichuk, Gabrielle H.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Hsin, Jonathan</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Rivera-Hernández, Ita A. E.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Simko, Olena M.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Wrynn, Theresa E.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Chivers, Douglas P.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Ferrari, Maud C. O.</creatorcontrib><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>Chemoreception Abstracts</collection><collection>Ecology Abstracts</collection><collection>Entomology Abstracts (Full archive)</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>Science Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Social Science Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>ProQuest SciTech Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Sustainability</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>Social Science Premium Collection</collection><collection>Agricultural & Environmental Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection</collection><collection>Sociology Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Psychology</collection><collection>Science Database</collection><collection>Social Science Database</collection><collection>Sociology Database</collection><collection>Biological Science Database</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>Environmental Science Database</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>Environmental Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><jtitle>Behavioral ecology and sociobiology</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Bairos-Novak, Kevin R.</au><au>Crane, Adam L.</au><au>Achtymichuk, Gabrielle H.</au><au>Hsin, Jonathan</au><au>Rivera-Hernández, Ita A. E.</au><au>Simko, Olena M.</au><au>Wrynn, Theresa E.</au><au>Chivers, Douglas P.</au><au>Ferrari, Maud C. O.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Forget the audience: tadpoles release similar disturbance cues regardless of kinship or familiarity</atitle><jtitle>Behavioral ecology and sociobiology</jtitle><stitle>Behav Ecol Sociobiol</stitle><date>2020-12-01</date><risdate>2020</risdate><volume>74</volume><issue>12</issue><spage>1</spage><epage>10</epage><pages>1-10</pages><artnum>147</artnum><issn>0340-5443</issn><eissn>1432-0762</eissn><abstract>Group-living prey rely on social information such as alarm signals and other social cues to avoid predation. By definition, “signals” imply that a message is voluntarily directed at receivers (i.e., the audience), whereas “cues” are released incidentally regardless of the audience composition. Thus, audience effects can be used to differentiate between signals and cues when communication is difficult to observe or quantify. In at least two fish species, chemical disturbance cues are released during a predator attack to signal to familiar audiences about predation risk. Here, we examined whether audience composition affects disturbance cue release in wood frog (Lithobates sylvaticus) tadpoles to better understand the function of disturbance cues across aquatic prey. Groups of tadpoles underwent simulated predator attacks to obtain disturbance cues. The groups were either familiar and related, unfamiliar and related, familiar and unrelated, or unfamiliar and unrelated. To assess the relative potency of each cue, we used a behavioral bioassay design involving activity changes in independent tadpole receivers (unfamiliar and unrelated to the donors). If tadpoles use disturbance cues to signal related and/or familiar individuals, we predicted increased fright responses in receivers to cues obtained from those groups. However, we detected no effect of audience composition, indicating that tadpoles release similar disturbance cues regardless of audience kinship or familiarity. Nevertheless, disturbance cues evoked a consistent antipredator response in receivers indicating that these chemicals still act as reliable risk cues. Further comparative studies using audience effects are necessary to understand how disturbance cues have evolved across aquatic prey.</abstract><cop>Berlin/Heidelberg</cop><pub>Springer Science + Business Media</pub><doi>10.1007/s00265-020-02936-8</doi><tpages>10</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0152-1452</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-8645-8180</orcidid></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0340-5443 |
ispartof | Behavioral ecology and sociobiology, 2020-12, Vol.74 (12), p.1-10, Article 147 |
issn | 0340-5443 1432-0762 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_journals_2473397161 |
source | Jstor Complete Legacy; SpringerLink Journals - AutoHoldings |
subjects | Amphibians Animal behavior Animal Ecology Anti-predator behavior Audiences Behavioral Sciences Bioassays Biomedical and Life Sciences Chemicals Comparative studies Composition effects Cues Disturbance Familiarity Frogs Juveniles Life Sciences ORIGINAL ARTICLE Predation Predator-prey simulation Predators Prey Receivers Siblings Smell Zoology |
title | Forget the audience: tadpoles release similar disturbance cues regardless of kinship or familiarity |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-17T01%3A57%3A34IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-jstor_proqu&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Forget%20the%20audience:%20tadpoles%20release%20similar%20disturbance%20cues%20regardless%20of%20kinship%20or%20familiarity&rft.jtitle=Behavioral%20ecology%20and%20sociobiology&rft.au=Bairos-Novak,%20Kevin%20R.&rft.date=2020-12-01&rft.volume=74&rft.issue=12&rft.spage=1&rft.epage=10&rft.pages=1-10&rft.artnum=147&rft.issn=0340-5443&rft.eissn=1432-0762&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007/s00265-020-02936-8&rft_dat=%3Cjstor_proqu%3E48727847%3C/jstor_proqu%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2473397161&rft_id=info:pmid/&rft_jstor_id=48727847&rfr_iscdi=true |