When and how are lies told? And the role of culture and intentions in intelligence‐gathering interviews
Purpose Lie‐tellers tend to tell embedded lies within interviews. In the context of intelligence‐gathering interviews, human sources may disclose information about multiple events, some of which may be false. In two studies, we examined when lie‐tellers from low‐ and high‐context cultures start repo...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Legal and criminological psychology 2020-09, Vol.25 (2), p.257-277 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 277 |
---|---|
container_issue | 2 |
container_start_page | 257 |
container_title | Legal and criminological psychology |
container_volume | 25 |
creator | Deeb, Haneen Vrij, Aldert Leal, Sharon Verigin, Brianna L. Kleinman, Steven M. |
description | Purpose
Lie‐tellers tend to tell embedded lies within interviews. In the context of intelligence‐gathering interviews, human sources may disclose information about multiple events, some of which may be false. In two studies, we examined when lie‐tellers from low‐ and high‐context cultures start reporting false events in interviews and to what extent they provide a similar amount of detail for the false and truthful events. Study 1 focused on lie‐tellers' intentions, and Study 2 focused on their actual responses.
Methods
Participants were asked to think of one false event and three truthful events. Study 1 (N = 100) was an online study in which participants responded to a questionnaire about where they would position the false event when interviewed and they rated the amount of detail they would provide for the events. Study 2 (N = 126) was an experimental study that involved interviewing participants about the events.
Results
Although there was no clear preference for lie position, participants seemed to report the false event at the end rather than at the beginning of the interview. Also, participants provided a similar amount of detail across events. Results on intentions (Study 1) partially overlapped with results on actual responses (Study 2). No differences emerged between low‐ and high‐context cultures.
Conclusions
This research is a first step towards understanding verbal cues that assist investigative practitioners in saving their cognitive and time resources when detecting deception regardless of interviewees' cultural background. More research on similar cues is encouraged. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1111/lcrp.12171 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_journals_2433249383</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>2433249383</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c3371-7f43141ad88ee8633d5457a0e35fc6eaf9627f6fd93fcde523a787751084bf933</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9kN1KwzAUx4MoOHQ3PkHAO6Gz6Wna9ErG8AsGiihehqw92SIxnUnr2J2P4DP6JGar156b8_U7H_wJOWPphEW7tLVfT1jGSnZARlma54kAgEMyYsB5Ahmvjsk4BLNIMwYi9soRMa8rdFS5hq7aDVUeqTUYaNfa5opOY7lbIfWtRdpqWve26yOyw43r0HWmdSGG-8xas0RX48_X91LFMW_cct_wnwY34ZQcaWUDjv_8CXm5uX6e3SXzh9v72XSe1PEhlpQ6B5Yz1QiBKAqAhue8VCkC13WBSldFVupCNxXoukGegSpFWXKWinyhK4ATcj7sXfv2o8fQybe29y6elFkOkOUViB11MVC1b0PwqOXam3flt5Klcqem3Kkp92pGmA3wxljc_kPK-ezpcZj5BQ4BeKs</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2433249383</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>When and how are lies told? And the role of culture and intentions in intelligence‐gathering interviews</title><source>Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA)</source><source>Wiley Online Library Journals Frontfile Complete</source><source>HeinOnline Law Journal Library</source><creator>Deeb, Haneen ; Vrij, Aldert ; Leal, Sharon ; Verigin, Brianna L. ; Kleinman, Steven M.</creator><creatorcontrib>Deeb, Haneen ; Vrij, Aldert ; Leal, Sharon ; Verigin, Brianna L. ; Kleinman, Steven M.</creatorcontrib><description>Purpose
Lie‐tellers tend to tell embedded lies within interviews. In the context of intelligence‐gathering interviews, human sources may disclose information about multiple events, some of which may be false. In two studies, we examined when lie‐tellers from low‐ and high‐context cultures start reporting false events in interviews and to what extent they provide a similar amount of detail for the false and truthful events. Study 1 focused on lie‐tellers' intentions, and Study 2 focused on their actual responses.
Methods
Participants were asked to think of one false event and three truthful events. Study 1 (N = 100) was an online study in which participants responded to a questionnaire about where they would position the false event when interviewed and they rated the amount of detail they would provide for the events. Study 2 (N = 126) was an experimental study that involved interviewing participants about the events.
Results
Although there was no clear preference for lie position, participants seemed to report the false event at the end rather than at the beginning of the interview. Also, participants provided a similar amount of detail across events. Results on intentions (Study 1) partially overlapped with results on actual responses (Study 2). No differences emerged between low‐ and high‐context cultures.
Conclusions
This research is a first step towards understanding verbal cues that assist investigative practitioners in saving their cognitive and time resources when detecting deception regardless of interviewees' cultural background. More research on similar cues is encouraged.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1355-3259</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 2044-8333</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1111/lcrp.12171</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Leicester: British Psychological Society</publisher><subject>consistency ; Cues ; culture ; Deception ; Intelligence ; intentions ; Interviews ; lie position ; Studies</subject><ispartof>Legal and criminological psychology, 2020-09, Vol.25 (2), p.257-277</ispartof><rights>2020 The Authors. Legal and Criminological Psychology published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of British Psychological Society</rights><rights>2020. This article is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c3371-7f43141ad88ee8633d5457a0e35fc6eaf9627f6fd93fcde523a787751084bf933</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c3371-7f43141ad88ee8633d5457a0e35fc6eaf9627f6fd93fcde523a787751084bf933</cites><orcidid>0000-0003-1260-5034 ; 0000-0001-8941-8398 ; 0000-0001-8647-7763 ; 0000-0003-3668-6844 ; 0000-0002-6635-8975</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/pdf/10.1111%2Flcrp.12171$$EPDF$$P50$$Gwiley$$Hfree_for_read</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1111%2Flcrp.12171$$EHTML$$P50$$Gwiley$$Hfree_for_read</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,1411,27901,27902,30976,45550,45551</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Deeb, Haneen</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Vrij, Aldert</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Leal, Sharon</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Verigin, Brianna L.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kleinman, Steven M.</creatorcontrib><title>When and how are lies told? And the role of culture and intentions in intelligence‐gathering interviews</title><title>Legal and criminological psychology</title><description>Purpose
Lie‐tellers tend to tell embedded lies within interviews. In the context of intelligence‐gathering interviews, human sources may disclose information about multiple events, some of which may be false. In two studies, we examined when lie‐tellers from low‐ and high‐context cultures start reporting false events in interviews and to what extent they provide a similar amount of detail for the false and truthful events. Study 1 focused on lie‐tellers' intentions, and Study 2 focused on their actual responses.
Methods
Participants were asked to think of one false event and three truthful events. Study 1 (N = 100) was an online study in which participants responded to a questionnaire about where they would position the false event when interviewed and they rated the amount of detail they would provide for the events. Study 2 (N = 126) was an experimental study that involved interviewing participants about the events.
Results
Although there was no clear preference for lie position, participants seemed to report the false event at the end rather than at the beginning of the interview. Also, participants provided a similar amount of detail across events. Results on intentions (Study 1) partially overlapped with results on actual responses (Study 2). No differences emerged between low‐ and high‐context cultures.
Conclusions
This research is a first step towards understanding verbal cues that assist investigative practitioners in saving their cognitive and time resources when detecting deception regardless of interviewees' cultural background. More research on similar cues is encouraged.</description><subject>consistency</subject><subject>Cues</subject><subject>culture</subject><subject>Deception</subject><subject>Intelligence</subject><subject>intentions</subject><subject>Interviews</subject><subject>lie position</subject><subject>Studies</subject><issn>1355-3259</issn><issn>2044-8333</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2020</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>24P</sourceid><sourceid>7QJ</sourceid><recordid>eNp9kN1KwzAUx4MoOHQ3PkHAO6Gz6Wna9ErG8AsGiihehqw92SIxnUnr2J2P4DP6JGar156b8_U7H_wJOWPphEW7tLVfT1jGSnZARlma54kAgEMyYsB5Ahmvjsk4BLNIMwYi9soRMa8rdFS5hq7aDVUeqTUYaNfa5opOY7lbIfWtRdpqWve26yOyw43r0HWmdSGG-8xas0RX48_X91LFMW_cct_wnwY34ZQcaWUDjv_8CXm5uX6e3SXzh9v72XSe1PEhlpQ6B5Yz1QiBKAqAhue8VCkC13WBSldFVupCNxXoukGegSpFWXKWinyhK4ATcj7sXfv2o8fQybe29y6elFkOkOUViB11MVC1b0PwqOXam3flt5Klcqem3Kkp92pGmA3wxljc_kPK-ezpcZj5BQ4BeKs</recordid><startdate>202009</startdate><enddate>202009</enddate><creator>Deeb, Haneen</creator><creator>Vrij, Aldert</creator><creator>Leal, Sharon</creator><creator>Verigin, Brianna L.</creator><creator>Kleinman, Steven M.</creator><general>British Psychological Society</general><scope>24P</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7QJ</scope><scope>K7.</scope><scope>K9.</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-1260-5034</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8941-8398</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8647-7763</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3668-6844</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6635-8975</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>202009</creationdate><title>When and how are lies told? And the role of culture and intentions in intelligence‐gathering interviews</title><author>Deeb, Haneen ; Vrij, Aldert ; Leal, Sharon ; Verigin, Brianna L. ; Kleinman, Steven M.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c3371-7f43141ad88ee8633d5457a0e35fc6eaf9627f6fd93fcde523a787751084bf933</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2020</creationdate><topic>consistency</topic><topic>Cues</topic><topic>culture</topic><topic>Deception</topic><topic>Intelligence</topic><topic>intentions</topic><topic>Interviews</topic><topic>lie position</topic><topic>Studies</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Deeb, Haneen</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Vrij, Aldert</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Leal, Sharon</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Verigin, Brianna L.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kleinman, Steven M.</creatorcontrib><collection>Wiley Online Library Open Access</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA)</collection><collection>ProQuest Criminal Justice (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><jtitle>Legal and criminological psychology</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Deeb, Haneen</au><au>Vrij, Aldert</au><au>Leal, Sharon</au><au>Verigin, Brianna L.</au><au>Kleinman, Steven M.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>When and how are lies told? And the role of culture and intentions in intelligence‐gathering interviews</atitle><jtitle>Legal and criminological psychology</jtitle><date>2020-09</date><risdate>2020</risdate><volume>25</volume><issue>2</issue><spage>257</spage><epage>277</epage><pages>257-277</pages><issn>1355-3259</issn><eissn>2044-8333</eissn><abstract>Purpose
Lie‐tellers tend to tell embedded lies within interviews. In the context of intelligence‐gathering interviews, human sources may disclose information about multiple events, some of which may be false. In two studies, we examined when lie‐tellers from low‐ and high‐context cultures start reporting false events in interviews and to what extent they provide a similar amount of detail for the false and truthful events. Study 1 focused on lie‐tellers' intentions, and Study 2 focused on their actual responses.
Methods
Participants were asked to think of one false event and three truthful events. Study 1 (N = 100) was an online study in which participants responded to a questionnaire about where they would position the false event when interviewed and they rated the amount of detail they would provide for the events. Study 2 (N = 126) was an experimental study that involved interviewing participants about the events.
Results
Although there was no clear preference for lie position, participants seemed to report the false event at the end rather than at the beginning of the interview. Also, participants provided a similar amount of detail across events. Results on intentions (Study 1) partially overlapped with results on actual responses (Study 2). No differences emerged between low‐ and high‐context cultures.
Conclusions
This research is a first step towards understanding verbal cues that assist investigative practitioners in saving their cognitive and time resources when detecting deception regardless of interviewees' cultural background. More research on similar cues is encouraged.</abstract><cop>Leicester</cop><pub>British Psychological Society</pub><doi>10.1111/lcrp.12171</doi><tpages>21</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-1260-5034</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8941-8398</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8647-7763</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3668-6844</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6635-8975</orcidid><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 1355-3259 |
ispartof | Legal and criminological psychology, 2020-09, Vol.25 (2), p.257-277 |
issn | 1355-3259 2044-8333 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_journals_2433249383 |
source | Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA); Wiley Online Library Journals Frontfile Complete; HeinOnline Law Journal Library |
subjects | consistency Cues culture Deception Intelligence intentions Interviews lie position Studies |
title | When and how are lies told? And the role of culture and intentions in intelligence‐gathering interviews |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-30T03%3A43%3A14IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=When%20and%20how%20are%20lies%20told?%20And%20the%20role%20of%20culture%20and%20intentions%20in%20intelligence%E2%80%90gathering%20interviews&rft.jtitle=Legal%20and%20criminological%20psychology&rft.au=Deeb,%20Haneen&rft.date=2020-09&rft.volume=25&rft.issue=2&rft.spage=257&rft.epage=277&rft.pages=257-277&rft.issn=1355-3259&rft.eissn=2044-8333&rft_id=info:doi/10.1111/lcrp.12171&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E2433249383%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2433249383&rft_id=info:pmid/&rfr_iscdi=true |