Is tuba masculine or feminine? The timing of grammatical gender

We used computer mouse tracking to investigate gender ratings of musical instruments in monolingual Spanish and English speakers. Musical instruments are tied to conceptual representations of gender. Consistent with previous research, English speakers rated the instruments male and female following...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Mind & language 2019-11, Vol.34 (5), p.667-680
Hauptverfasser: Incera, Sara, McLennan, Conor T., Stronsick, Lisa M., Zetzer, Emily E.
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 680
container_issue 5
container_start_page 667
container_title Mind & language
container_volume 34
creator Incera, Sara
McLennan, Conor T.
Stronsick, Lisa M.
Zetzer, Emily E.
description We used computer mouse tracking to investigate gender ratings of musical instruments in monolingual Spanish and English speakers. Musical instruments are tied to conceptual representations of gender. Consistent with previous research, English speakers rated the instruments male and female following these relatively stable conceptual representations, while Spanish speakers used grammar—in addition to their conceptual representations of gender—to perform their ratings. Interestingly, the responses of the Spanish speakers were influenced first by the grammatical information and 60 ms later by the conceptual representations. Measuring how participants' responses unfold over time provides novel information regarding the timing of grammatical gender.
doi_str_mv 10.1111/mila.12223
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_journals_2327547519</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>2327547519</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c3013-890bcc83e663c72e115f6c0d1d62f46a221a85c5973ed44d0c6ebd97cb9a51a83</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9kE9PwzAMxSMEEmNw4RNE4obUYSdt2p6maWIwqYjLOEdpmo5M_TOSVmjfnoxyxhc_yz8_S4-Qe4QFhnpqbaMWyBjjF2SGscgiAEwvyQxY0AgiviY33h8AgHMOM7LcejqMpaKt8npsbGdo72htWtsFvaS7T0MHG6Y97Wu6d6pt1WC1aujedJVxt-SqVo03d399Tj42z7v1a1S8v2zXqyLSHJBHWQ6l1hk3QnCdMoOY1EJDhZVgdSwUY6iyRCd5yk0VxxVoYcoqT3WZqySs-Jw8TL5H13-Nxg_y0I-uCy8l4yxN4jTBPFCPE6Vd770ztTw62yp3kgjyHJA8ByR_AwowTvC3bczpH1K-bYvVdPMD_eBm1A</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2327547519</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Is tuba masculine or feminine? The timing of grammatical gender</title><source>Wiley Online Library All Journals</source><creator>Incera, Sara ; McLennan, Conor T. ; Stronsick, Lisa M. ; Zetzer, Emily E.</creator><creatorcontrib>Incera, Sara ; McLennan, Conor T. ; Stronsick, Lisa M. ; Zetzer, Emily E.</creatorcontrib><description>We used computer mouse tracking to investigate gender ratings of musical instruments in monolingual Spanish and English speakers. Musical instruments are tied to conceptual representations of gender. Consistent with previous research, English speakers rated the instruments male and female following these relatively stable conceptual representations, while Spanish speakers used grammar—in addition to their conceptual representations of gender—to perform their ratings. Interestingly, the responses of the Spanish speakers were influenced first by the grammatical information and 60 ms later by the conceptual representations. Measuring how participants' responses unfold over time provides novel information regarding the timing of grammatical gender.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0268-1064</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1468-0017</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1111/mila.12223</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishing Ltd</publisher><subject>English language ; Grammar ; Grammatical gender ; Linguistic relativity ; Masculinity ; mouse tracking ; Musical instruments ; Spanish language ; timing ; Tracking</subject><ispartof>Mind &amp; language, 2019-11, Vol.34 (5), p.667-680</ispartof><rights>2018 John Wiley &amp; Sons Ltd</rights><rights>2019 John Wiley &amp; Sons Ltd</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c3013-890bcc83e663c72e115f6c0d1d62f46a221a85c5973ed44d0c6ebd97cb9a51a83</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c3013-890bcc83e663c72e115f6c0d1d62f46a221a85c5973ed44d0c6ebd97cb9a51a83</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/pdf/10.1111%2Fmila.12223$$EPDF$$P50$$Gwiley$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1111%2Fmila.12223$$EHTML$$P50$$Gwiley$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,1417,27923,27924,45573,45574</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Incera, Sara</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>McLennan, Conor T.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Stronsick, Lisa M.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Zetzer, Emily E.</creatorcontrib><title>Is tuba masculine or feminine? The timing of grammatical gender</title><title>Mind &amp; language</title><description>We used computer mouse tracking to investigate gender ratings of musical instruments in monolingual Spanish and English speakers. Musical instruments are tied to conceptual representations of gender. Consistent with previous research, English speakers rated the instruments male and female following these relatively stable conceptual representations, while Spanish speakers used grammar—in addition to their conceptual representations of gender—to perform their ratings. Interestingly, the responses of the Spanish speakers were influenced first by the grammatical information and 60 ms later by the conceptual representations. Measuring how participants' responses unfold over time provides novel information regarding the timing of grammatical gender.</description><subject>English language</subject><subject>Grammar</subject><subject>Grammatical gender</subject><subject>Linguistic relativity</subject><subject>Masculinity</subject><subject>mouse tracking</subject><subject>Musical instruments</subject><subject>Spanish language</subject><subject>timing</subject><subject>Tracking</subject><issn>0268-1064</issn><issn>1468-0017</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2019</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><recordid>eNp9kE9PwzAMxSMEEmNw4RNE4obUYSdt2p6maWIwqYjLOEdpmo5M_TOSVmjfnoxyxhc_yz8_S4-Qe4QFhnpqbaMWyBjjF2SGscgiAEwvyQxY0AgiviY33h8AgHMOM7LcejqMpaKt8npsbGdo72htWtsFvaS7T0MHG6Y97Wu6d6pt1WC1aujedJVxt-SqVo03d399Tj42z7v1a1S8v2zXqyLSHJBHWQ6l1hk3QnCdMoOY1EJDhZVgdSwUY6iyRCd5yk0VxxVoYcoqT3WZqySs-Jw8TL5H13-Nxg_y0I-uCy8l4yxN4jTBPFCPE6Vd770ztTw62yp3kgjyHJA8ByR_AwowTvC3bczpH1K-bYvVdPMD_eBm1A</recordid><startdate>201911</startdate><enddate>201911</enddate><creator>Incera, Sara</creator><creator>McLennan, Conor T.</creator><creator>Stronsick, Lisa M.</creator><creator>Zetzer, Emily E.</creator><general>Blackwell Publishing Ltd</general><general>Wiley Subscription Services, Inc</general><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7T9</scope><scope>8BJ</scope><scope>8BM</scope><scope>FQK</scope><scope>JBE</scope></search><sort><creationdate>201911</creationdate><title>Is tuba masculine or feminine? The timing of grammatical gender</title><author>Incera, Sara ; McLennan, Conor T. ; Stronsick, Lisa M. ; Zetzer, Emily E.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c3013-890bcc83e663c72e115f6c0d1d62f46a221a85c5973ed44d0c6ebd97cb9a51a83</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2019</creationdate><topic>English language</topic><topic>Grammar</topic><topic>Grammatical gender</topic><topic>Linguistic relativity</topic><topic>Masculinity</topic><topic>mouse tracking</topic><topic>Musical instruments</topic><topic>Spanish language</topic><topic>timing</topic><topic>Tracking</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Incera, Sara</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>McLennan, Conor T.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Stronsick, Lisa M.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Zetzer, Emily E.</creatorcontrib><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS)</collection><collection>ComDisDome</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences</collection><jtitle>Mind &amp; language</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Incera, Sara</au><au>McLennan, Conor T.</au><au>Stronsick, Lisa M.</au><au>Zetzer, Emily E.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Is tuba masculine or feminine? The timing of grammatical gender</atitle><jtitle>Mind &amp; language</jtitle><date>2019-11</date><risdate>2019</risdate><volume>34</volume><issue>5</issue><spage>667</spage><epage>680</epage><pages>667-680</pages><issn>0268-1064</issn><eissn>1468-0017</eissn><abstract>We used computer mouse tracking to investigate gender ratings of musical instruments in monolingual Spanish and English speakers. Musical instruments are tied to conceptual representations of gender. Consistent with previous research, English speakers rated the instruments male and female following these relatively stable conceptual representations, while Spanish speakers used grammar—in addition to their conceptual representations of gender—to perform their ratings. Interestingly, the responses of the Spanish speakers were influenced first by the grammatical information and 60 ms later by the conceptual representations. Measuring how participants' responses unfold over time provides novel information regarding the timing of grammatical gender.</abstract><cop>Oxford, UK</cop><pub>Blackwell Publishing Ltd</pub><doi>10.1111/mila.12223</doi><tpages>14</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0268-1064
ispartof Mind & language, 2019-11, Vol.34 (5), p.667-680
issn 0268-1064
1468-0017
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_journals_2327547519
source Wiley Online Library All Journals
subjects English language
Grammar
Grammatical gender
Linguistic relativity
Masculinity
mouse tracking
Musical instruments
Spanish language
timing
Tracking
title Is tuba masculine or feminine? The timing of grammatical gender
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-09T04%3A12%3A05IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Is%20tuba%20masculine%20or%20feminine?%20The%20timing%20of%20grammatical%20gender&rft.jtitle=Mind%20&%20language&rft.au=Incera,%20Sara&rft.date=2019-11&rft.volume=34&rft.issue=5&rft.spage=667&rft.epage=680&rft.pages=667-680&rft.issn=0268-1064&rft.eissn=1468-0017&rft_id=info:doi/10.1111/mila.12223&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E2327547519%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2327547519&rft_id=info:pmid/&rfr_iscdi=true