Estimating the Prevalence of Gender-Biased Language in Undergraduates’ Everyday Speech
Research has shown that language can be gender-biased; however, little research has investigated the prevalence of this bias in everyday speech. Using recordings sampled from undergraduates’ daily conversations, we investigated two forms of gender bias: paternalism through use of the infantilizing l...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Sex roles 2020, Vol.82 (1-2), p.81-93 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 93 |
---|---|
container_issue | 1-2 |
container_start_page | 81 |
container_title | Sex roles |
container_volume | 82 |
creator | MacArthur, Heather J. Cundiff, Jessica L. Mehl, Matthias R. |
description | Research has shown that language can be gender-biased; however, little research has investigated the prevalence of this bias in everyday speech. Using recordings sampled from undergraduates’ daily conversations, we investigated two forms of gender bias: paternalism through use of the infantilizing label
girl
to refer to women and androcentrism through a tendency to use more masculine (e.g.,
man, guy
) than feminine (e.g.,
girl, woman
) labels in everyday speech. U.S. participants (
n
= 175) wore the Electronically Activated Recorder (EAR), a device that recorded sound samples from their environments for 30 s every 12.5 min, for up to 4 days. Verbatim transcripts were then analyzed for instances of commonly used labels for females and males (e.g.,
girl, woman, boy, man
). Results indicated that the label
girl
surpassed all other labels for women, as well as
boy
labels for men. We also found evidence of a masculine-label bias: Participants used masculine labels more frequently than feminine labels overall. These findings indicate the need for future research to investigate the potential consequences of infantilizing and androcentric language as well as the need for teachers, professors, clinicians, and practitioners of all types to be mindful of how their speech may include, exclude, or infantilize people based on gender. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1007/s11199-019-01033-z |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_journals_2194975723</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>2194975723</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c363t-769fbe6a805dbec32b115b3e4d02af4717d4dfafcea759d1acc0bd18e32a70c63</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9kMFKw0AQhhdRsFZfwNOC59XZ3STbHLXUVigoaMHbstmdpCk1qbtJoT35Gr6eT2JqBG8ehjnM988wHyGXHK45gLoJnPM0ZcAPBVKy_REZ8FhJJlQijskAZAIMQMSn5CyEFUCHQTQgr5PQlG-mKauCNkukTx63Zo2VRVrndIqVQ8_uShPQ0bmpitYUSMuKLg6DwhvXmgbD18cnnWzR75zZ0ecNol2ek5PcrANe_PYhWdxPXsYzNn-cPoxv58zKRDZMJWmeYWJGELsMrRQZ53EmMXIgTB4prlzkcpNbNCpOHTfWQub4CKUwCmwih-Sq37vx9XuLodGruvVVd1ILnkapipWQHSV6yvo6BI-53vjubb_THPTBoO4N6s6g_jGo911I9qHQwVWB_m_1P6lv9H12Ug</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2194975723</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Estimating the Prevalence of Gender-Biased Language in Undergraduates’ Everyday Speech</title><source>Education Source (EBSCOhost)</source><source>Sociological Abstracts</source><source>SpringerLink Journals - AutoHoldings</source><creator>MacArthur, Heather J. ; Cundiff, Jessica L. ; Mehl, Matthias R.</creator><creatorcontrib>MacArthur, Heather J. ; Cundiff, Jessica L. ; Mehl, Matthias R.</creatorcontrib><description>Research has shown that language can be gender-biased; however, little research has investigated the prevalence of this bias in everyday speech. Using recordings sampled from undergraduates’ daily conversations, we investigated two forms of gender bias: paternalism through use of the infantilizing label
girl
to refer to women and androcentrism through a tendency to use more masculine (e.g.,
man, guy
) than feminine (e.g.,
girl, woman
) labels in everyday speech. U.S. participants (
n
= 175) wore the Electronically Activated Recorder (EAR), a device that recorded sound samples from their environments for 30 s every 12.5 min, for up to 4 days. Verbatim transcripts were then analyzed for instances of commonly used labels for females and males (e.g.,
girl, woman, boy, man
). Results indicated that the label
girl
surpassed all other labels for women, as well as
boy
labels for men. We also found evidence of a masculine-label bias: Participants used masculine labels more frequently than feminine labels overall. These findings indicate the need for future research to investigate the potential consequences of infantilizing and androcentric language as well as the need for teachers, professors, clinicians, and practitioners of all types to be mindful of how their speech may include, exclude, or infantilize people based on gender.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0360-0025</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1573-2762</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1007/s11199-019-01033-z</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>New York: Springer US</publisher><subject>Behavioral Science and Psychology ; Bias ; College students ; Females ; Gender ; Gender Studies ; Masculinity ; Medicine/Public Health ; Original Article ; Paternalism ; Psychology ; Sociology ; Speech ; Teachers ; Undergraduate Students ; Women</subject><ispartof>Sex roles, 2020, Vol.82 (1-2), p.81-93</ispartof><rights>Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019</rights><rights>Sex Roles is a copyright of Springer, (2019). All Rights Reserved.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c363t-769fbe6a805dbec32b115b3e4d02af4717d4dfafcea759d1acc0bd18e32a70c63</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c363t-769fbe6a805dbec32b115b3e4d02af4717d4dfafcea759d1acc0bd18e32a70c63</cites><orcidid>0000-0001-6178-2008</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s11199-019-01033-z$$EPDF$$P50$$Gspringer$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://link.springer.com/10.1007/s11199-019-01033-z$$EHTML$$P50$$Gspringer$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,27321,27901,27902,33751,41464,42533,51294</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>MacArthur, Heather J.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Cundiff, Jessica L.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Mehl, Matthias R.</creatorcontrib><title>Estimating the Prevalence of Gender-Biased Language in Undergraduates’ Everyday Speech</title><title>Sex roles</title><addtitle>Sex Roles</addtitle><description>Research has shown that language can be gender-biased; however, little research has investigated the prevalence of this bias in everyday speech. Using recordings sampled from undergraduates’ daily conversations, we investigated two forms of gender bias: paternalism through use of the infantilizing label
girl
to refer to women and androcentrism through a tendency to use more masculine (e.g.,
man, guy
) than feminine (e.g.,
girl, woman
) labels in everyday speech. U.S. participants (
n
= 175) wore the Electronically Activated Recorder (EAR), a device that recorded sound samples from their environments for 30 s every 12.5 min, for up to 4 days. Verbatim transcripts were then analyzed for instances of commonly used labels for females and males (e.g.,
girl, woman, boy, man
). Results indicated that the label
girl
surpassed all other labels for women, as well as
boy
labels for men. We also found evidence of a masculine-label bias: Participants used masculine labels more frequently than feminine labels overall. These findings indicate the need for future research to investigate the potential consequences of infantilizing and androcentric language as well as the need for teachers, professors, clinicians, and practitioners of all types to be mindful of how their speech may include, exclude, or infantilize people based on gender.</description><subject>Behavioral Science and Psychology</subject><subject>Bias</subject><subject>College students</subject><subject>Females</subject><subject>Gender</subject><subject>Gender Studies</subject><subject>Masculinity</subject><subject>Medicine/Public Health</subject><subject>Original Article</subject><subject>Paternalism</subject><subject>Psychology</subject><subject>Sociology</subject><subject>Speech</subject><subject>Teachers</subject><subject>Undergraduate Students</subject><subject>Women</subject><issn>0360-0025</issn><issn>1573-2762</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2020</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>8G5</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>BHHNA</sourceid><sourceid>GUQSH</sourceid><sourceid>M2O</sourceid><sourceid>QXPDG</sourceid><recordid>eNp9kMFKw0AQhhdRsFZfwNOC59XZ3STbHLXUVigoaMHbstmdpCk1qbtJoT35Gr6eT2JqBG8ehjnM988wHyGXHK45gLoJnPM0ZcAPBVKy_REZ8FhJJlQijskAZAIMQMSn5CyEFUCHQTQgr5PQlG-mKauCNkukTx63Zo2VRVrndIqVQ8_uShPQ0bmpitYUSMuKLg6DwhvXmgbD18cnnWzR75zZ0ecNol2ek5PcrANe_PYhWdxPXsYzNn-cPoxv58zKRDZMJWmeYWJGELsMrRQZ53EmMXIgTB4prlzkcpNbNCpOHTfWQub4CKUwCmwih-Sq37vx9XuLodGruvVVd1ILnkapipWQHSV6yvo6BI-53vjubb_THPTBoO4N6s6g_jGo911I9qHQwVWB_m_1P6lv9H12Ug</recordid><startdate>2020</startdate><enddate>2020</enddate><creator>MacArthur, Heather J.</creator><creator>Cundiff, Jessica L.</creator><creator>Mehl, Matthias R.</creator><general>Springer US</general><general>Springer Nature B.V</general><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>0-V</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7R6</scope><scope>7U4</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>888</scope><scope>88B</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>88J</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>8G5</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ALSLI</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BHHNA</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>CJNVE</scope><scope>DWI</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>GUQSH</scope><scope>HEHIP</scope><scope>M0P</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>M2O</scope><scope>M2R</scope><scope>M2S</scope><scope>MBDVC</scope><scope>PQEDU</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQGEN</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>QXPDG</scope><scope>WZK</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-6178-2008</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>2020</creationdate><title>Estimating the Prevalence of Gender-Biased Language in Undergraduates’ Everyday Speech</title><author>MacArthur, Heather J. ; Cundiff, Jessica L. ; Mehl, Matthias R.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c363t-769fbe6a805dbec32b115b3e4d02af4717d4dfafcea759d1acc0bd18e32a70c63</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2020</creationdate><topic>Behavioral Science and Psychology</topic><topic>Bias</topic><topic>College students</topic><topic>Females</topic><topic>Gender</topic><topic>Gender Studies</topic><topic>Masculinity</topic><topic>Medicine/Public Health</topic><topic>Original Article</topic><topic>Paternalism</topic><topic>Psychology</topic><topic>Sociology</topic><topic>Speech</topic><topic>Teachers</topic><topic>Undergraduate Students</topic><topic>Women</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>MacArthur, Heather J.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Cundiff, Jessica L.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Mehl, Matthias R.</creatorcontrib><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>GenderWatch</collection><collection>Sociological Abstracts (pre-2017)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>GenderWatch (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Education Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>Social Science Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Research Library (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>Social Science Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Sociological Abstracts</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>Education Collection</collection><collection>Sociological Abstracts</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>Research Library Prep</collection><collection>Sociology Collection</collection><collection>Education Database (ProQuest)</collection><collection>ProQuest Psychology</collection><collection>Research Library</collection><collection>Social Science Database</collection><collection>Sociology Database (ProQuest)</collection><collection>Research Library (Corporate)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Education</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest Women's & Gender Studies</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>Diversity Collection</collection><collection>Sociological Abstracts (Ovid)</collection><jtitle>Sex roles</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>MacArthur, Heather J.</au><au>Cundiff, Jessica L.</au><au>Mehl, Matthias R.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Estimating the Prevalence of Gender-Biased Language in Undergraduates’ Everyday Speech</atitle><jtitle>Sex roles</jtitle><stitle>Sex Roles</stitle><date>2020</date><risdate>2020</risdate><volume>82</volume><issue>1-2</issue><spage>81</spage><epage>93</epage><pages>81-93</pages><issn>0360-0025</issn><eissn>1573-2762</eissn><abstract>Research has shown that language can be gender-biased; however, little research has investigated the prevalence of this bias in everyday speech. Using recordings sampled from undergraduates’ daily conversations, we investigated two forms of gender bias: paternalism through use of the infantilizing label
girl
to refer to women and androcentrism through a tendency to use more masculine (e.g.,
man, guy
) than feminine (e.g.,
girl, woman
) labels in everyday speech. U.S. participants (
n
= 175) wore the Electronically Activated Recorder (EAR), a device that recorded sound samples from their environments for 30 s every 12.5 min, for up to 4 days. Verbatim transcripts were then analyzed for instances of commonly used labels for females and males (e.g.,
girl, woman, boy, man
). Results indicated that the label
girl
surpassed all other labels for women, as well as
boy
labels for men. We also found evidence of a masculine-label bias: Participants used masculine labels more frequently than feminine labels overall. These findings indicate the need for future research to investigate the potential consequences of infantilizing and androcentric language as well as the need for teachers, professors, clinicians, and practitioners of all types to be mindful of how their speech may include, exclude, or infantilize people based on gender.</abstract><cop>New York</cop><pub>Springer US</pub><doi>10.1007/s11199-019-01033-z</doi><tpages>13</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-6178-2008</orcidid><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0360-0025 |
ispartof | Sex roles, 2020, Vol.82 (1-2), p.81-93 |
issn | 0360-0025 1573-2762 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_journals_2194975723 |
source | Education Source (EBSCOhost); Sociological Abstracts; SpringerLink Journals - AutoHoldings |
subjects | Behavioral Science and Psychology Bias College students Females Gender Gender Studies Masculinity Medicine/Public Health Original Article Paternalism Psychology Sociology Speech Teachers Undergraduate Students Women |
title | Estimating the Prevalence of Gender-Biased Language in Undergraduates’ Everyday Speech |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-29T13%3A55%3A43IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Estimating%20the%20Prevalence%20of%20Gender-Biased%20Language%20in%20Undergraduates%E2%80%99%20Everyday%20Speech&rft.jtitle=Sex%20roles&rft.au=MacArthur,%20Heather%20J.&rft.date=2020&rft.volume=82&rft.issue=1-2&rft.spage=81&rft.epage=93&rft.pages=81-93&rft.issn=0360-0025&rft.eissn=1573-2762&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007/s11199-019-01033-z&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E2194975723%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2194975723&rft_id=info:pmid/&rfr_iscdi=true |