Working memory capacity, short-term memory capacity, and the continued influence effect: A latent-variable analysis

Misinformation often affects inferences and judgments even after it has been retracted and discredited. This is known as the continued influence effect. Memory processes have been theorized to contribute to the continued influence effect, and much previous research has focussed on the role of long-t...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Intelligence (Norwood) 2018-07, Vol.69, p.117-122
Hauptverfasser: Brydges, Christopher R., Gignac, Gilles E., Ecker, Ullrich K.H.
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 122
container_issue
container_start_page 117
container_title Intelligence (Norwood)
container_volume 69
creator Brydges, Christopher R.
Gignac, Gilles E.
Ecker, Ullrich K.H.
description Misinformation often affects inferences and judgments even after it has been retracted and discredited. This is known as the continued influence effect. Memory processes have been theorized to contribute to the continued influence effect, and much previous research has focussed on the role of long-term memory processes at the time misinformation is retrieved during inferential reasoning and judgments. Recently, however, experimental research has focussed upon the role of working memory (WM) processes engaged in the updating and integration of information, when the retraction is encoded. From an individual differences perspective, susceptibility to continued influence effects should be predicted by a person's WM abilities, if continued reliance on misinformation is influenced, at least in part, by insufficient integration of the initial misinformation and its subsequent retraction. Consequently, we hypothesized that WM capacity would predict susceptibility to continued influence effects uniquely and more substantially than short-term memory (STM) capacity. Participants (N = 216) completed a continued-influence task, as well as a battery of WM and STM capacity tasks. Based on a latent variable model, our hypothesis was supported (WM capacity: β = −0.36, p = .013; STM capacity: β = 0.22, p = .187). Consequently, we suggest that low WM capacity is a measurable “risk factor” for continued reliance on misinformation. •Misinformation often affects judgments even after it has been retracted.•Memory processes are theorized to contribute to the continued influence effect.•A WM capacity latent-variable was associated with continued influence effects.•A STM capacity latent-variable was not associated with continued influence effects.
doi_str_mv 10.1016/j.intell.2018.03.009
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_journals_2110458822</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><els_id>S0160289617303641</els_id><sourcerecordid>2110458822</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c380t-86e5db698ca2a9387c287d2cbc67c9f0eff92d2e80fd22d91701c231cbb84a143</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9kM1LxDAQxYMouK7-Bx4CXm2dpF-pB2ERv2DBi-IxpMnUzdpt1yS70P_eLPUmeJrDvPdm3o-QSwYpA1berFPbB-y6lAMTKWQpQH1EZkxUWVLVWXFMZlEGCRd1eUrOvF8DQMFyMSP-Y3Bftv-kG9wMbqRabZW2YbymfjW4kAR0m7871RsaVkj10Afb79BQ27fdDnuNFNsWdbilC9qpgH1I9spZ1XQYXaobvfXn5KRVnceL3zkn748Pb_fPyfL16eV-sUx0JiAkosTCNGUttOKqzkSluagM140uK123EA_V3HAU0BrOTc0qYJpnTDeNyBXLszm5mnK3bvjeoQ9yPexcfMJLzhjkhRCcR1U-qbQbvHfYyq2zG-VGyUAe8Mq1nPDKA14JmYx4o-1usmFssLfopNf2AMBYF_tLM9j_A34A-n2HUw</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2110458822</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Working memory capacity, short-term memory capacity, and the continued influence effect: A latent-variable analysis</title><source>Applied Social Sciences Index &amp; Abstracts (ASSIA)</source><source>Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals</source><creator>Brydges, Christopher R. ; Gignac, Gilles E. ; Ecker, Ullrich K.H.</creator><creatorcontrib>Brydges, Christopher R. ; Gignac, Gilles E. ; Ecker, Ullrich K.H.</creatorcontrib><description>Misinformation often affects inferences and judgments even after it has been retracted and discredited. This is known as the continued influence effect. Memory processes have been theorized to contribute to the continued influence effect, and much previous research has focussed on the role of long-term memory processes at the time misinformation is retrieved during inferential reasoning and judgments. Recently, however, experimental research has focussed upon the role of working memory (WM) processes engaged in the updating and integration of information, when the retraction is encoded. From an individual differences perspective, susceptibility to continued influence effects should be predicted by a person's WM abilities, if continued reliance on misinformation is influenced, at least in part, by insufficient integration of the initial misinformation and its subsequent retraction. Consequently, we hypothesized that WM capacity would predict susceptibility to continued influence effects uniquely and more substantially than short-term memory (STM) capacity. Participants (N = 216) completed a continued-influence task, as well as a battery of WM and STM capacity tasks. Based on a latent variable model, our hypothesis was supported (WM capacity: β = −0.36, p = .013; STM capacity: β = 0.22, p = .187). Consequently, we suggest that low WM capacity is a measurable “risk factor” for continued reliance on misinformation. •Misinformation often affects judgments even after it has been retracted.•Memory processes are theorized to contribute to the continued influence effect.•A WM capacity latent-variable was associated with continued influence effects.•A STM capacity latent-variable was not associated with continued influence effects.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0160-2896</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1873-7935</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2018.03.009</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>New York: Elsevier Inc</publisher><subject>Cognition &amp; reasoning ; Cognitive ability ; Continued influence effect ; Discredited ; Experimental research ; Individual differences ; Integration ; Intelligence ; Long term memory ; Misinformation ; Reliance ; Short term memory ; Susceptibility ; Working memory</subject><ispartof>Intelligence (Norwood), 2018-07, Vol.69, p.117-122</ispartof><rights>2018</rights><rights>Copyright Ablex Publishing Corporation Jul/Aug 2018</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c380t-86e5db698ca2a9387c287d2cbc67c9f0eff92d2e80fd22d91701c231cbb84a143</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c380t-86e5db698ca2a9387c287d2cbc67c9f0eff92d2e80fd22d91701c231cbb84a143</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0160289617303641$$EHTML$$P50$$Gelsevier$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,3537,27901,27902,30976,65306</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Brydges, Christopher R.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Gignac, Gilles E.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Ecker, Ullrich K.H.</creatorcontrib><title>Working memory capacity, short-term memory capacity, and the continued influence effect: A latent-variable analysis</title><title>Intelligence (Norwood)</title><description>Misinformation often affects inferences and judgments even after it has been retracted and discredited. This is known as the continued influence effect. Memory processes have been theorized to contribute to the continued influence effect, and much previous research has focussed on the role of long-term memory processes at the time misinformation is retrieved during inferential reasoning and judgments. Recently, however, experimental research has focussed upon the role of working memory (WM) processes engaged in the updating and integration of information, when the retraction is encoded. From an individual differences perspective, susceptibility to continued influence effects should be predicted by a person's WM abilities, if continued reliance on misinformation is influenced, at least in part, by insufficient integration of the initial misinformation and its subsequent retraction. Consequently, we hypothesized that WM capacity would predict susceptibility to continued influence effects uniquely and more substantially than short-term memory (STM) capacity. Participants (N = 216) completed a continued-influence task, as well as a battery of WM and STM capacity tasks. Based on a latent variable model, our hypothesis was supported (WM capacity: β = −0.36, p = .013; STM capacity: β = 0.22, p = .187). Consequently, we suggest that low WM capacity is a measurable “risk factor” for continued reliance on misinformation. •Misinformation often affects judgments even after it has been retracted.•Memory processes are theorized to contribute to the continued influence effect.•A WM capacity latent-variable was associated with continued influence effects.•A STM capacity latent-variable was not associated with continued influence effects.</description><subject>Cognition &amp; reasoning</subject><subject>Cognitive ability</subject><subject>Continued influence effect</subject><subject>Discredited</subject><subject>Experimental research</subject><subject>Individual differences</subject><subject>Integration</subject><subject>Intelligence</subject><subject>Long term memory</subject><subject>Misinformation</subject><subject>Reliance</subject><subject>Short term memory</subject><subject>Susceptibility</subject><subject>Working memory</subject><issn>0160-2896</issn><issn>1873-7935</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2018</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>7QJ</sourceid><recordid>eNp9kM1LxDAQxYMouK7-Bx4CXm2dpF-pB2ERv2DBi-IxpMnUzdpt1yS70P_eLPUmeJrDvPdm3o-QSwYpA1berFPbB-y6lAMTKWQpQH1EZkxUWVLVWXFMZlEGCRd1eUrOvF8DQMFyMSP-Y3Bftv-kG9wMbqRabZW2YbymfjW4kAR0m7871RsaVkj10Afb79BQ27fdDnuNFNsWdbilC9qpgH1I9spZ1XQYXaobvfXn5KRVnceL3zkn748Pb_fPyfL16eV-sUx0JiAkosTCNGUttOKqzkSluagM140uK123EA_V3HAU0BrOTc0qYJpnTDeNyBXLszm5mnK3bvjeoQ9yPexcfMJLzhjkhRCcR1U-qbQbvHfYyq2zG-VGyUAe8Mq1nPDKA14JmYx4o-1usmFssLfopNf2AMBYF_tLM9j_A34A-n2HUw</recordid><startdate>201807</startdate><enddate>201807</enddate><creator>Brydges, Christopher R.</creator><creator>Gignac, Gilles E.</creator><creator>Ecker, Ullrich K.H.</creator><general>Elsevier Inc</general><general>Elsevier Science Ltd</general><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7QJ</scope><scope>7TK</scope></search><sort><creationdate>201807</creationdate><title>Working memory capacity, short-term memory capacity, and the continued influence effect: A latent-variable analysis</title><author>Brydges, Christopher R. ; Gignac, Gilles E. ; Ecker, Ullrich K.H.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c380t-86e5db698ca2a9387c287d2cbc67c9f0eff92d2e80fd22d91701c231cbb84a143</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2018</creationdate><topic>Cognition &amp; reasoning</topic><topic>Cognitive ability</topic><topic>Continued influence effect</topic><topic>Discredited</topic><topic>Experimental research</topic><topic>Individual differences</topic><topic>Integration</topic><topic>Intelligence</topic><topic>Long term memory</topic><topic>Misinformation</topic><topic>Reliance</topic><topic>Short term memory</topic><topic>Susceptibility</topic><topic>Working memory</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Brydges, Christopher R.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Gignac, Gilles E.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Ecker, Ullrich K.H.</creatorcontrib><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Applied Social Sciences Index &amp; Abstracts (ASSIA)</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><jtitle>Intelligence (Norwood)</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Brydges, Christopher R.</au><au>Gignac, Gilles E.</au><au>Ecker, Ullrich K.H.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Working memory capacity, short-term memory capacity, and the continued influence effect: A latent-variable analysis</atitle><jtitle>Intelligence (Norwood)</jtitle><date>2018-07</date><risdate>2018</risdate><volume>69</volume><spage>117</spage><epage>122</epage><pages>117-122</pages><issn>0160-2896</issn><eissn>1873-7935</eissn><abstract>Misinformation often affects inferences and judgments even after it has been retracted and discredited. This is known as the continued influence effect. Memory processes have been theorized to contribute to the continued influence effect, and much previous research has focussed on the role of long-term memory processes at the time misinformation is retrieved during inferential reasoning and judgments. Recently, however, experimental research has focussed upon the role of working memory (WM) processes engaged in the updating and integration of information, when the retraction is encoded. From an individual differences perspective, susceptibility to continued influence effects should be predicted by a person's WM abilities, if continued reliance on misinformation is influenced, at least in part, by insufficient integration of the initial misinformation and its subsequent retraction. Consequently, we hypothesized that WM capacity would predict susceptibility to continued influence effects uniquely and more substantially than short-term memory (STM) capacity. Participants (N = 216) completed a continued-influence task, as well as a battery of WM and STM capacity tasks. Based on a latent variable model, our hypothesis was supported (WM capacity: β = −0.36, p = .013; STM capacity: β = 0.22, p = .187). Consequently, we suggest that low WM capacity is a measurable “risk factor” for continued reliance on misinformation. •Misinformation often affects judgments even after it has been retracted.•Memory processes are theorized to contribute to the continued influence effect.•A WM capacity latent-variable was associated with continued influence effects.•A STM capacity latent-variable was not associated with continued influence effects.</abstract><cop>New York</cop><pub>Elsevier Inc</pub><doi>10.1016/j.intell.2018.03.009</doi><tpages>6</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0160-2896
ispartof Intelligence (Norwood), 2018-07, Vol.69, p.117-122
issn 0160-2896
1873-7935
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_journals_2110458822
source Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA); Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals
subjects Cognition & reasoning
Cognitive ability
Continued influence effect
Discredited
Experimental research
Individual differences
Integration
Intelligence
Long term memory
Misinformation
Reliance
Short term memory
Susceptibility
Working memory
title Working memory capacity, short-term memory capacity, and the continued influence effect: A latent-variable analysis
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-04T16%3A51%3A44IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Working%20memory%20capacity,%20short-term%20memory%20capacity,%20and%20the%20continued%20influence%20effect:%20A%20latent-variable%20analysis&rft.jtitle=Intelligence%20(Norwood)&rft.au=Brydges,%20Christopher%20R.&rft.date=2018-07&rft.volume=69&rft.spage=117&rft.epage=122&rft.pages=117-122&rft.issn=0160-2896&rft.eissn=1873-7935&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/j.intell.2018.03.009&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E2110458822%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2110458822&rft_id=info:pmid/&rft_els_id=S0160289617303641&rfr_iscdi=true