Reflexivity in Meadow Mari: Binding and Agree
According to the Canonical Binding Theory (Chomsky 1981), anaphors must be bound in their local domain and pronominals must be free. The discovery of “long‐distance anaphors” (e.g. Thrainsson 1976, Giorgi 1984), which violate the locality condition, induced the search for independent criteria. Giorg...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Studia linguistica 2017-04, Vol.71 (1-2), p.178-204 |
---|---|
1. Verfasser: | |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 204 |
---|---|
container_issue | 1-2 |
container_start_page | 178 |
container_title | Studia linguistica |
container_volume | 71 |
creator | Volkova, Anna |
description | According to the Canonical Binding Theory (Chomsky 1981), anaphors must be bound in their local domain and pronominals must be free. The discovery of “long‐distance anaphors” (e.g. Thrainsson 1976, Giorgi 1984), which violate the locality condition, induced the search for independent criteria. Giorgi (1984:310) proposed a widely adopted criterion: “pronouns can have split antecedents and anaphors cannot”. Recent minimalist binding theories derive this property of anaphors from the way a dependency on the antecedent is established which makes it intrinsic to binding. However, this leads to an important problem, since some languages have elements that i) may be locally bound and, hence, look like anaphors; yet ii) allow split antecedents which is a property of pronouns (e.g. Japanese and Korean, Katada 1991, Kasai 2000). In this paper I analyze the data of another such language, namely Meadow Mari (Uralic), and show that such facts require a modular approach to binding (see Reuland 2011). I further argue that here the left periphery contains the relevant factor. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1111/stul.12071 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_journals_1879955684</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>1879955684</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c3371-cad0dbc0235184b4066a2ee2d55ee20df7501c795b2961db07c9492b9aacd4683</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9kN9LwzAQx4MoWKcv_gUF34TOXNIkjW9zzB-wIej2HNIkHRm1nUnn3H9vZ332Hu74wufu4IPQNeAx9HUXu109BoIFnKAEci4yyQpyihKMqcwoSHqOLmLc4D7zgicoe3NV7b79l-8OqW_ShdO23acLHfx9-uAb65t1qhubTtbBuUt0Vuk6uqu_OUKrx9ly-pzNX59eppN5ZigVkBltsS0NJpRBkZc55lwT54hlrO_YVoJhMEKykkgOtsTCyFySUmptbM4LOkI3w91taD93LnZq0-5C079UUAgpGeNF3lO3A2VCG2NwldoG_6HDQQFWRx3qqEP96uhhGOC9r93hH1K9L1fzYecHpWtgeQ</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1879955684</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Reflexivity in Meadow Mari: Binding and Agree</title><source>Wiley Online Library Journals</source><creator>Volkova, Anna</creator><creatorcontrib>Volkova, Anna</creatorcontrib><description>According to the Canonical Binding Theory (Chomsky 1981), anaphors must be bound in their local domain and pronominals must be free. The discovery of “long‐distance anaphors” (e.g. Thrainsson 1976, Giorgi 1984), which violate the locality condition, induced the search for independent criteria. Giorgi (1984:310) proposed a widely adopted criterion: “pronouns can have split antecedents and anaphors cannot”. Recent minimalist binding theories derive this property of anaphors from the way a dependency on the antecedent is established which makes it intrinsic to binding. However, this leads to an important problem, since some languages have elements that i) may be locally bound and, hence, look like anaphors; yet ii) allow split antecedents which is a property of pronouns (e.g. Japanese and Korean, Katada 1991, Kasai 2000). In this paper I analyze the data of another such language, namely Meadow Mari (Uralic), and show that such facts require a modular approach to binding (see Reuland 2011). I further argue that here the left periphery contains the relevant factor.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0039-3193</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1467-9582</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1111/stul.12071</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Oxford: Wiley Subscription Services, Inc</publisher><subject>Anaphora ; Antecedents ; Chomsky, Noam ; Government and binding theory ; Grammatical agreement ; Japanese language ; Korean language ; Mari ; Minimalist program ; Pronouns ; Reflexivity ; Uralic languages</subject><ispartof>Studia linguistica, 2017-04, Vol.71 (1-2), p.178-204</ispartof><rights>2017 The Editorial Board of</rights><rights>Copyright © 2017 The Editorial Board of Studia Linguistica</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c3371-cad0dbc0235184b4066a2ee2d55ee20df7501c795b2961db07c9492b9aacd4683</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c3371-cad0dbc0235184b4066a2ee2d55ee20df7501c795b2961db07c9492b9aacd4683</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/pdf/10.1111%2Fstul.12071$$EPDF$$P50$$Gwiley$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1111%2Fstul.12071$$EHTML$$P50$$Gwiley$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,1411,27901,27902,45550,45551</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Volkova, Anna</creatorcontrib><title>Reflexivity in Meadow Mari: Binding and Agree</title><title>Studia linguistica</title><description>According to the Canonical Binding Theory (Chomsky 1981), anaphors must be bound in their local domain and pronominals must be free. The discovery of “long‐distance anaphors” (e.g. Thrainsson 1976, Giorgi 1984), which violate the locality condition, induced the search for independent criteria. Giorgi (1984:310) proposed a widely adopted criterion: “pronouns can have split antecedents and anaphors cannot”. Recent minimalist binding theories derive this property of anaphors from the way a dependency on the antecedent is established which makes it intrinsic to binding. However, this leads to an important problem, since some languages have elements that i) may be locally bound and, hence, look like anaphors; yet ii) allow split antecedents which is a property of pronouns (e.g. Japanese and Korean, Katada 1991, Kasai 2000). In this paper I analyze the data of another such language, namely Meadow Mari (Uralic), and show that such facts require a modular approach to binding (see Reuland 2011). I further argue that here the left periphery contains the relevant factor.</description><subject>Anaphora</subject><subject>Antecedents</subject><subject>Chomsky, Noam</subject><subject>Government and binding theory</subject><subject>Grammatical agreement</subject><subject>Japanese language</subject><subject>Korean language</subject><subject>Mari</subject><subject>Minimalist program</subject><subject>Pronouns</subject><subject>Reflexivity</subject><subject>Uralic languages</subject><issn>0039-3193</issn><issn>1467-9582</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2017</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><recordid>eNp9kN9LwzAQx4MoWKcv_gUF34TOXNIkjW9zzB-wIej2HNIkHRm1nUnn3H9vZ332Hu74wufu4IPQNeAx9HUXu109BoIFnKAEci4yyQpyihKMqcwoSHqOLmLc4D7zgicoe3NV7b79l-8OqW_ShdO23acLHfx9-uAb65t1qhubTtbBuUt0Vuk6uqu_OUKrx9ly-pzNX59eppN5ZigVkBltsS0NJpRBkZc55lwT54hlrO_YVoJhMEKykkgOtsTCyFySUmptbM4LOkI3w91taD93LnZq0-5C079UUAgpGeNF3lO3A2VCG2NwldoG_6HDQQFWRx3qqEP96uhhGOC9r93hH1K9L1fzYecHpWtgeQ</recordid><startdate>201704</startdate><enddate>201704</enddate><creator>Volkova, Anna</creator><general>Wiley Subscription Services, Inc</general><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7T9</scope><scope>8BM</scope></search><sort><creationdate>201704</creationdate><title>Reflexivity in Meadow Mari: Binding and Agree</title><author>Volkova, Anna</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c3371-cad0dbc0235184b4066a2ee2d55ee20df7501c795b2961db07c9492b9aacd4683</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2017</creationdate><topic>Anaphora</topic><topic>Antecedents</topic><topic>Chomsky, Noam</topic><topic>Government and binding theory</topic><topic>Grammatical agreement</topic><topic>Japanese language</topic><topic>Korean language</topic><topic>Mari</topic><topic>Minimalist program</topic><topic>Pronouns</topic><topic>Reflexivity</topic><topic>Uralic languages</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Volkova, Anna</creatorcontrib><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)</collection><collection>ComDisDome</collection><jtitle>Studia linguistica</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Volkova, Anna</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Reflexivity in Meadow Mari: Binding and Agree</atitle><jtitle>Studia linguistica</jtitle><date>2017-04</date><risdate>2017</risdate><volume>71</volume><issue>1-2</issue><spage>178</spage><epage>204</epage><pages>178-204</pages><issn>0039-3193</issn><eissn>1467-9582</eissn><abstract>According to the Canonical Binding Theory (Chomsky 1981), anaphors must be bound in their local domain and pronominals must be free. The discovery of “long‐distance anaphors” (e.g. Thrainsson 1976, Giorgi 1984), which violate the locality condition, induced the search for independent criteria. Giorgi (1984:310) proposed a widely adopted criterion: “pronouns can have split antecedents and anaphors cannot”. Recent minimalist binding theories derive this property of anaphors from the way a dependency on the antecedent is established which makes it intrinsic to binding. However, this leads to an important problem, since some languages have elements that i) may be locally bound and, hence, look like anaphors; yet ii) allow split antecedents which is a property of pronouns (e.g. Japanese and Korean, Katada 1991, Kasai 2000). In this paper I analyze the data of another such language, namely Meadow Mari (Uralic), and show that such facts require a modular approach to binding (see Reuland 2011). I further argue that here the left periphery contains the relevant factor.</abstract><cop>Oxford</cop><pub>Wiley Subscription Services, Inc</pub><doi>10.1111/stul.12071</doi><tpages>27</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0039-3193 |
ispartof | Studia linguistica, 2017-04, Vol.71 (1-2), p.178-204 |
issn | 0039-3193 1467-9582 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_journals_1879955684 |
source | Wiley Online Library Journals |
subjects | Anaphora Antecedents Chomsky, Noam Government and binding theory Grammatical agreement Japanese language Korean language Mari Minimalist program Pronouns Reflexivity Uralic languages |
title | Reflexivity in Meadow Mari: Binding and Agree |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-04T17%3A35%3A19IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Reflexivity%20in%20Meadow%20Mari:%20Binding%20and%20Agree&rft.jtitle=Studia%20linguistica&rft.au=Volkova,%20Anna&rft.date=2017-04&rft.volume=71&rft.issue=1-2&rft.spage=178&rft.epage=204&rft.pages=178-204&rft.issn=0039-3193&rft.eissn=1467-9582&rft_id=info:doi/10.1111/stul.12071&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E1879955684%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1879955684&rft_id=info:pmid/&rfr_iscdi=true |