Source Credibility and the Language of Expert Testimony1

In an examination of the language used by expert witnesses during actual courtroom testimony, it was expected that experts who exhibited content themes related to their credentials or experience (expertise) and to objectivity (trustworthiness) would be perceived as being more credible. Forty‐three s...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Journal of applied social psychology 1992-12, Vol.22 (24), p.1909-1939
Hauptverfasser: Hurwitz, Steven D., Miron, Murray S., Johnson, Blair T.
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 1939
container_issue 24
container_start_page 1909
container_title Journal of applied social psychology
container_volume 22
creator Hurwitz, Steven D.
Miron, Murray S.
Johnson, Blair T.
description In an examination of the language used by expert witnesses during actual courtroom testimony, it was expected that experts who exhibited content themes related to their credentials or experience (expertise) and to objectivity (trustworthiness) would be perceived as being more credible. Forty‐three segments of expert testimony were taken from actual court transcripts and content analyzed. Two‐factor analytically derived factors predicted expert witness membership into low‐ and high‐credibility groups, defined a priori by credibility judgments of undergraduate raters (n = 348). These factors were (a) the use of passive voice and (b) the witnesses' background and qualifications. Further analyses revealed that perceptions of expert witness credibility were also a function of the usage of words that connote power (an expert's official status, degree of prominence and/or recognition) or negative (suffering or damage). Results are discussed in terms of dimensions of source credibility and their parallels to past research in persuasion.
doi_str_mv 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1992.tb01530.x
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_wiley</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_journals_1873338661</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>4317850451</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-p850-209dabe3caf653db9a873a428cff9de863bbf0bb73e33efd775b1b805c8e2d4e3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNo9kNFLwzAQxoMoOKf_Q9Dn1lyztMmbY8ypDBS295A0l9mytbXtcP3vTdnwXu6O-_i-40fII7AYQj2XMQihIpCQxqBUEveWgeAsPl2Ryf_pmkwYSyBSLFG35K7ryrAqweSEyE19bHOkixZdYYt90Q_UVI7230jXptodzQ5p7eny1GDb0y12fXGoqwHuyY03-w4fLn1Ktq_L7eItWn-u3hfzddRIwaKEKWcs8tz4VHBnlZEZN7NE5t4rhzLl1npmbcaRc_Quy4QFK5nIJSZuhnxKns62TVv_HEO6LsPDVUjUEKw4l2kKQfVyVv0Wexx00xYH0w4amB4p6VKPKPSIQo-U9IWSPumP-eZrHPkfI41epg</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1873338661</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Source Credibility and the Language of Expert Testimony1</title><source>Sociological Abstracts</source><source>Access via Wiley Online Library</source><creator>Hurwitz, Steven D. ; Miron, Murray S. ; Johnson, Blair T.</creator><creatorcontrib>Hurwitz, Steven D. ; Miron, Murray S. ; Johnson, Blair T.</creatorcontrib><description>In an examination of the language used by expert witnesses during actual courtroom testimony, it was expected that experts who exhibited content themes related to their credentials or experience (expertise) and to objectivity (trustworthiness) would be perceived as being more credible. Forty‐three segments of expert testimony were taken from actual court transcripts and content analyzed. Two‐factor analytically derived factors predicted expert witness membership into low‐ and high‐credibility groups, defined a priori by credibility judgments of undergraduate raters (n = 348). These factors were (a) the use of passive voice and (b) the witnesses' background and qualifications. Further analyses revealed that perceptions of expert witness credibility were also a function of the usage of words that connote power (an expert's official status, degree of prominence and/or recognition) or negative (suffering or damage). Results are discussed in terms of dimensions of source credibility and their parallels to past research in persuasion.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0021-9029</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1559-1816</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1992.tb01530.x</identifier><identifier>CODEN: JASPBX</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishing Ltd</publisher><subject>Credibility ; Expert witness testimony</subject><ispartof>Journal of applied social psychology, 1992-12, Vol.22 (24), p.1909-1939</ispartof><rights>1992 V. H. Winston &amp; Sons, Inc.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/pdf/10.1111%2Fj.1559-1816.1992.tb01530.x$$EPDF$$P50$$Gwiley$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1111%2Fj.1559-1816.1992.tb01530.x$$EHTML$$P50$$Gwiley$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,1417,27924,27925,33774,45574,45575</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Hurwitz, Steven D.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Miron, Murray S.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Johnson, Blair T.</creatorcontrib><title>Source Credibility and the Language of Expert Testimony1</title><title>Journal of applied social psychology</title><description>In an examination of the language used by expert witnesses during actual courtroom testimony, it was expected that experts who exhibited content themes related to their credentials or experience (expertise) and to objectivity (trustworthiness) would be perceived as being more credible. Forty‐three segments of expert testimony were taken from actual court transcripts and content analyzed. Two‐factor analytically derived factors predicted expert witness membership into low‐ and high‐credibility groups, defined a priori by credibility judgments of undergraduate raters (n = 348). These factors were (a) the use of passive voice and (b) the witnesses' background and qualifications. Further analyses revealed that perceptions of expert witness credibility were also a function of the usage of words that connote power (an expert's official status, degree of prominence and/or recognition) or negative (suffering or damage). Results are discussed in terms of dimensions of source credibility and their parallels to past research in persuasion.</description><subject>Credibility</subject><subject>Expert witness testimony</subject><issn>0021-9029</issn><issn>1559-1816</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1992</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>BHHNA</sourceid><recordid>eNo9kNFLwzAQxoMoOKf_Q9Dn1lyztMmbY8ypDBS295A0l9mytbXtcP3vTdnwXu6O-_i-40fII7AYQj2XMQihIpCQxqBUEveWgeAsPl2Ryf_pmkwYSyBSLFG35K7ryrAqweSEyE19bHOkixZdYYt90Q_UVI7230jXptodzQ5p7eny1GDb0y12fXGoqwHuyY03-w4fLn1Ktq_L7eItWn-u3hfzddRIwaKEKWcs8tz4VHBnlZEZN7NE5t4rhzLl1npmbcaRc_Quy4QFK5nIJSZuhnxKns62TVv_HEO6LsPDVUjUEKw4l2kKQfVyVv0Wexx00xYH0w4amB4p6VKPKPSIQo-U9IWSPumP-eZrHPkfI41epg</recordid><startdate>199212</startdate><enddate>199212</enddate><creator>Hurwitz, Steven D.</creator><creator>Miron, Murray S.</creator><creator>Johnson, Blair T.</creator><general>Blackwell Publishing Ltd</general><general>Wiley Subscription Services, Inc</general><scope>7U4</scope><scope>8BJ</scope><scope>BHHNA</scope><scope>DWI</scope><scope>FQK</scope><scope>JBE</scope><scope>WZK</scope></search><sort><creationdate>199212</creationdate><title>Source Credibility and the Language of Expert Testimony1</title><author>Hurwitz, Steven D. ; Miron, Murray S. ; Johnson, Blair T.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-p850-209dabe3caf653db9a873a428cff9de863bbf0bb73e33efd775b1b805c8e2d4e3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1992</creationdate><topic>Credibility</topic><topic>Expert witness testimony</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Hurwitz, Steven D.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Miron, Murray S.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Johnson, Blair T.</creatorcontrib><collection>Sociological Abstracts (pre-2017)</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS)</collection><collection>Sociological Abstracts</collection><collection>Sociological Abstracts</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences</collection><collection>Sociological Abstracts (Ovid)</collection><jtitle>Journal of applied social psychology</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Hurwitz, Steven D.</au><au>Miron, Murray S.</au><au>Johnson, Blair T.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Source Credibility and the Language of Expert Testimony1</atitle><jtitle>Journal of applied social psychology</jtitle><date>1992-12</date><risdate>1992</risdate><volume>22</volume><issue>24</issue><spage>1909</spage><epage>1939</epage><pages>1909-1939</pages><issn>0021-9029</issn><eissn>1559-1816</eissn><coden>JASPBX</coden><abstract>In an examination of the language used by expert witnesses during actual courtroom testimony, it was expected that experts who exhibited content themes related to their credentials or experience (expertise) and to objectivity (trustworthiness) would be perceived as being more credible. Forty‐three segments of expert testimony were taken from actual court transcripts and content analyzed. Two‐factor analytically derived factors predicted expert witness membership into low‐ and high‐credibility groups, defined a priori by credibility judgments of undergraduate raters (n = 348). These factors were (a) the use of passive voice and (b) the witnesses' background and qualifications. Further analyses revealed that perceptions of expert witness credibility were also a function of the usage of words that connote power (an expert's official status, degree of prominence and/or recognition) or negative (suffering or damage). Results are discussed in terms of dimensions of source credibility and their parallels to past research in persuasion.</abstract><cop>Oxford, UK</cop><pub>Blackwell Publishing Ltd</pub><doi>10.1111/j.1559-1816.1992.tb01530.x</doi><tpages>31</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0021-9029
ispartof Journal of applied social psychology, 1992-12, Vol.22 (24), p.1909-1939
issn 0021-9029
1559-1816
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_journals_1873338661
source Sociological Abstracts; Access via Wiley Online Library
subjects Credibility
Expert witness testimony
title Source Credibility and the Language of Expert Testimony1
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-25T01%3A58%3A24IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_wiley&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Source%20Credibility%20and%20the%20Language%20of%20Expert%20Testimony1&rft.jtitle=Journal%20of%20applied%20social%20psychology&rft.au=Hurwitz,%20Steven%20D.&rft.date=1992-12&rft.volume=22&rft.issue=24&rft.spage=1909&rft.epage=1939&rft.pages=1909-1939&rft.issn=0021-9029&rft.eissn=1559-1816&rft.coden=JASPBX&rft_id=info:doi/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1992.tb01530.x&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_wiley%3E4317850451%3C/proquest_wiley%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1873338661&rft_id=info:pmid/&rfr_iscdi=true