Can Children Read Evolutionary Trees?

Representations of the “tree of life” such as cladograms show the history of lineages and their relationships. They are increasingly found in formal and informal learning settings. Unfortunately, there is evidence that these representations can be challenging to interpret correctly. This study explo...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 2013-04, Vol.59 (2), p.221-247
Hauptverfasser: Ainsworth, Shaaron, Saffer, Jessica
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 247
container_issue 2
container_start_page 221
container_title Merrill-Palmer Quarterly
container_volume 59
creator Ainsworth, Shaaron
Saffer, Jessica
description Representations of the “tree of life” such as cladograms show the history of lineages and their relationships. They are increasingly found in formal and informal learning settings. Unfortunately, there is evidence that these representations can be challenging to interpret correctly. This study explored the question of whether children aged 7–11 can read these trees and, if so, what factors influence their understanding. A total of 28 children were shown cladograms with both different content (species and features shown) and form (how branches rotated). Questions required these children to reason about different aspects of cladogram interpretation and to search varying depths of the tree. Overall, children did remarkably well: 56% of their answers were completely correct after only 15 minutes of instruction. The youngest quartile of children performed worse than other ages, but there were no further age differences. Children's performance was influenced by the content and the depth of tree searched but not by the rotation of the branches. Like adults, they found reasoning about the relatedness of species particularly difficult. Children's explanations revealed varied insights: from correct semantic interpretation to syntactic interpretation to a variety of misunderstandings. Demonstration of this basic competency provides a foundation from which to design a more extended curriculum for children that uses cladograms to support evolutionary understanding.
doi_str_mv 10.13110/merrpalmquar1982.59.2.0221
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>gale_proqu</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_journals_1353087908</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><galeid>A330803113</galeid><ericid>EJ1004007</ericid><jstor_id>10.13110/merrpalmquar1982.59.2.0221</jstor_id><sourcerecordid>A330803113</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c2568-4c3c86ea6398867137da8f099b7cbfe4b934e8bf3a3aee5c8777ddba9e17ecac3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqVkU1LAzEQhoMoWKs_QSgUj7tOkt1NggcppX4hCFLBW8hmZ-uW_WiTVvDfG11RlF5kDoGZ55nAvISMKcSUUwrnDTq3MnWz3hpHlWRxqmIWA2N0jwxoytMIWJbtkwEwwSLF4fmQHHm_BOBc0XRAzqamHU1fqrpw2I4e0RSj2WtXbzdV1xr3Npo7RH95TA5KU3s8-XqH5OlqNp_eRPcP17fTyX1kWZrJKLHcygxNxpWUmaBcFEaWoFQubF5ikiueoMxLbrhBTK0UQhRFbhRSgdZYPiTjfu_Kdest-o1edlvXhi815SkHKRTIH2phatRVW3YbZ2xTeasnPEAQTsMDFe2gFtiiM3XXYlmF9i8-3sGHKrCp7E7hohes67x3WOqVq5pwNE1Bf8aj_8ajU6WZ_ogn2Ke9ja6y3-bsjgIkACLMZT9f-k3n_rX6HcIEoH0</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1353087908</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Can Children Read Evolutionary Trees?</title><source>Jstor Complete Legacy</source><source>Education Source</source><creator>Ainsworth, Shaaron ; Saffer, Jessica</creator><creatorcontrib>Ainsworth, Shaaron ; Saffer, Jessica</creatorcontrib><description>Representations of the “tree of life” such as cladograms show the history of lineages and their relationships. They are increasingly found in formal and informal learning settings. Unfortunately, there is evidence that these representations can be challenging to interpret correctly. This study explored the question of whether children aged 7–11 can read these trees and, if so, what factors influence their understanding. A total of 28 children were shown cladograms with both different content (species and features shown) and form (how branches rotated). Questions required these children to reason about different aspects of cladogram interpretation and to search varying depths of the tree. Overall, children did remarkably well: 56% of their answers were completely correct after only 15 minutes of instruction. The youngest quartile of children performed worse than other ages, but there were no further age differences. Children's performance was influenced by the content and the depth of tree searched but not by the rotation of the branches. Like adults, they found reasoning about the relatedness of species particularly difficult. Children's explanations revealed varied insights: from correct semantic interpretation to syntactic interpretation to a variety of misunderstandings. Demonstration of this basic competency provides a foundation from which to design a more extended curriculum for children that uses cladograms to support evolutionary understanding.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0272-930X</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1535-0266</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.13110/merrpalmquar1982.59.2.0221</identifier><identifier>CODEN: MPQUA5</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Detroit: Wayne State University Press</publisher><subject>Age Differences ; Animals ; Biological evolution ; Child development ; Children ; Comprehension ; Curriculum development ; Descendants ; Endangered &amp; extinct species ; England (Nottingham) ; Evidence ; Evolution ; Foreign Countries ; High schools ; Humans ; Inferences ; Influences ; Informal Education ; Intuition ; Learning ; Logical Thinking ; Personality ; Phylogenetic trees ; Reading ; Reasoning ; Science education ; Semantics ; Students ; Study and teaching ; Syntax ; Syntax semantics relationship ; Training ; Trees ; Visual Aids</subject><ispartof>Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 2013-04, Vol.59 (2), p.221-247</ispartof><rights>Copyright 2013 Wayne State University Press</rights><rights>COPYRIGHT 2013 Wayne State University Press</rights><rights>Copyright Wayne State University Press Apr 2013</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,778,782,801,27907,27908</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ1004007$$DView record in ERIC$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Ainsworth, Shaaron</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Saffer, Jessica</creatorcontrib><title>Can Children Read Evolutionary Trees?</title><title>Merrill-Palmer Quarterly</title><description>Representations of the “tree of life” such as cladograms show the history of lineages and their relationships. They are increasingly found in formal and informal learning settings. Unfortunately, there is evidence that these representations can be challenging to interpret correctly. This study explored the question of whether children aged 7–11 can read these trees and, if so, what factors influence their understanding. A total of 28 children were shown cladograms with both different content (species and features shown) and form (how branches rotated). Questions required these children to reason about different aspects of cladogram interpretation and to search varying depths of the tree. Overall, children did remarkably well: 56% of their answers were completely correct after only 15 minutes of instruction. The youngest quartile of children performed worse than other ages, but there were no further age differences. Children's performance was influenced by the content and the depth of tree searched but not by the rotation of the branches. Like adults, they found reasoning about the relatedness of species particularly difficult. Children's explanations revealed varied insights: from correct semantic interpretation to syntactic interpretation to a variety of misunderstandings. Demonstration of this basic competency provides a foundation from which to design a more extended curriculum for children that uses cladograms to support evolutionary understanding.</description><subject>Age Differences</subject><subject>Animals</subject><subject>Biological evolution</subject><subject>Child development</subject><subject>Children</subject><subject>Comprehension</subject><subject>Curriculum development</subject><subject>Descendants</subject><subject>Endangered &amp; extinct species</subject><subject>England (Nottingham)</subject><subject>Evidence</subject><subject>Evolution</subject><subject>Foreign Countries</subject><subject>High schools</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Inferences</subject><subject>Influences</subject><subject>Informal Education</subject><subject>Intuition</subject><subject>Learning</subject><subject>Logical Thinking</subject><subject>Personality</subject><subject>Phylogenetic trees</subject><subject>Reading</subject><subject>Reasoning</subject><subject>Science education</subject><subject>Semantics</subject><subject>Students</subject><subject>Study and teaching</subject><subject>Syntax</subject><subject>Syntax semantics relationship</subject><subject>Training</subject><subject>Trees</subject><subject>Visual Aids</subject><issn>0272-930X</issn><issn>1535-0266</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2013</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>ABUWG</sourceid><sourceid>AFKRA</sourceid><sourceid>AIMQZ</sourceid><sourceid>AZQEC</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>CCPQU</sourceid><sourceid>DWQXO</sourceid><sourceid>GNUQQ</sourceid><recordid>eNqVkU1LAzEQhoMoWKs_QSgUj7tOkt1NggcppX4hCFLBW8hmZ-uW_WiTVvDfG11RlF5kDoGZ55nAvISMKcSUUwrnDTq3MnWz3hpHlWRxqmIWA2N0jwxoytMIWJbtkwEwwSLF4fmQHHm_BOBc0XRAzqamHU1fqrpw2I4e0RSj2WtXbzdV1xr3Npo7RH95TA5KU3s8-XqH5OlqNp_eRPcP17fTyX1kWZrJKLHcygxNxpWUmaBcFEaWoFQubF5ikiueoMxLbrhBTK0UQhRFbhRSgdZYPiTjfu_Kdest-o1edlvXhi815SkHKRTIH2phatRVW3YbZ2xTeasnPEAQTsMDFe2gFtiiM3XXYlmF9i8-3sGHKrCp7E7hohes67x3WOqVq5pwNE1Bf8aj_8ajU6WZ_ogn2Ke9ja6y3-bsjgIkACLMZT9f-k3n_rX6HcIEoH0</recordid><startdate>20130401</startdate><enddate>20130401</enddate><creator>Ainsworth, Shaaron</creator><creator>Saffer, Jessica</creator><general>Wayne State University Press</general><scope>7SW</scope><scope>BJH</scope><scope>BNH</scope><scope>BNI</scope><scope>BNJ</scope><scope>BNO</scope><scope>ERI</scope><scope>PET</scope><scope>REK</scope><scope>WWN</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>0-V</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>4T-</scope><scope>4U-</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88B</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>8A4</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>AIMQZ</scope><scope>ALSLI</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>CJNVE</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>LIQON</scope><scope>M0P</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>PQEDU</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>S0X</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20130401</creationdate><title>Can Children Read Evolutionary Trees?</title><author>Ainsworth, Shaaron ; Saffer, Jessica</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c2568-4c3c86ea6398867137da8f099b7cbfe4b934e8bf3a3aee5c8777ddba9e17ecac3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2013</creationdate><topic>Age Differences</topic><topic>Animals</topic><topic>Biological evolution</topic><topic>Child development</topic><topic>Children</topic><topic>Comprehension</topic><topic>Curriculum development</topic><topic>Descendants</topic><topic>Endangered &amp; extinct species</topic><topic>England (Nottingham)</topic><topic>Evidence</topic><topic>Evolution</topic><topic>Foreign Countries</topic><topic>High schools</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Inferences</topic><topic>Influences</topic><topic>Informal Education</topic><topic>Intuition</topic><topic>Learning</topic><topic>Logical Thinking</topic><topic>Personality</topic><topic>Phylogenetic trees</topic><topic>Reading</topic><topic>Reasoning</topic><topic>Science education</topic><topic>Semantics</topic><topic>Students</topic><topic>Study and teaching</topic><topic>Syntax</topic><topic>Syntax semantics relationship</topic><topic>Training</topic><topic>Trees</topic><topic>Visual Aids</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Ainsworth, Shaaron</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Saffer, Jessica</creatorcontrib><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Ovid)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>ERIC( SilverPlatter )</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>Education Resources Information Center (ERIC)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Docstoc</collection><collection>University Readers</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Education Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>Education Periodicals</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>ProQuest One Literature</collection><collection>Social Science Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>Education Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>ProQuest One Literature - U.S. Customers Only</collection><collection>Education Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest One Education</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>SIRS Editorial</collection><jtitle>Merrill-Palmer Quarterly</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Ainsworth, Shaaron</au><au>Saffer, Jessica</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><ericid>EJ1004007</ericid><atitle>Can Children Read Evolutionary Trees?</atitle><jtitle>Merrill-Palmer Quarterly</jtitle><date>2013-04-01</date><risdate>2013</risdate><volume>59</volume><issue>2</issue><spage>221</spage><epage>247</epage><pages>221-247</pages><issn>0272-930X</issn><eissn>1535-0266</eissn><coden>MPQUA5</coden><abstract>Representations of the “tree of life” such as cladograms show the history of lineages and their relationships. They are increasingly found in formal and informal learning settings. Unfortunately, there is evidence that these representations can be challenging to interpret correctly. This study explored the question of whether children aged 7–11 can read these trees and, if so, what factors influence their understanding. A total of 28 children were shown cladograms with both different content (species and features shown) and form (how branches rotated). Questions required these children to reason about different aspects of cladogram interpretation and to search varying depths of the tree. Overall, children did remarkably well: 56% of their answers were completely correct after only 15 minutes of instruction. The youngest quartile of children performed worse than other ages, but there were no further age differences. Children's performance was influenced by the content and the depth of tree searched but not by the rotation of the branches. Like adults, they found reasoning about the relatedness of species particularly difficult. Children's explanations revealed varied insights: from correct semantic interpretation to syntactic interpretation to a variety of misunderstandings. Demonstration of this basic competency provides a foundation from which to design a more extended curriculum for children that uses cladograms to support evolutionary understanding.</abstract><cop>Detroit</cop><pub>Wayne State University Press</pub><doi>10.13110/merrpalmquar1982.59.2.0221</doi><tpages>27</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0272-930X
ispartof Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 2013-04, Vol.59 (2), p.221-247
issn 0272-930X
1535-0266
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_journals_1353087908
source Jstor Complete Legacy; Education Source
subjects Age Differences
Animals
Biological evolution
Child development
Children
Comprehension
Curriculum development
Descendants
Endangered & extinct species
England (Nottingham)
Evidence
Evolution
Foreign Countries
High schools
Humans
Inferences
Influences
Informal Education
Intuition
Learning
Logical Thinking
Personality
Phylogenetic trees
Reading
Reasoning
Science education
Semantics
Students
Study and teaching
Syntax
Syntax semantics relationship
Training
Trees
Visual Aids
title Can Children Read Evolutionary Trees?
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-17T03%3A25%3A52IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-gale_proqu&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Can%20Children%20Read%20Evolutionary%20Trees?&rft.jtitle=Merrill-Palmer%20Quarterly&rft.au=Ainsworth,%20Shaaron&rft.date=2013-04-01&rft.volume=59&rft.issue=2&rft.spage=221&rft.epage=247&rft.pages=221-247&rft.issn=0272-930X&rft.eissn=1535-0266&rft.coden=MPQUA5&rft_id=info:doi/10.13110/merrpalmquar1982.59.2.0221&rft_dat=%3Cgale_proqu%3EA330803113%3C/gale_proqu%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1353087908&rft_id=info:pmid/&rft_galeid=A330803113&rft_ericid=EJ1004007&rft_jstor_id=10.13110/merrpalmquar1982.59.2.0221&rfr_iscdi=true