Contextual cues in selective listening
Two messages were presented dichotically and subjects were asked to "shadow" whatever they heard on one ear. Somewhere in the middle the two passages were switched to the opposite ears. Subjects occasionally repeated one or two words, at the break, from the wrong ear, but never transferred...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Quarterly journal of experimental psychology 1960-10, Vol.12 (4), p.242-248 |
---|---|
1. Verfasser: | |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 248 |
---|---|
container_issue | 4 |
container_start_page | 242 |
container_title | Quarterly journal of experimental psychology |
container_volume | 12 |
creator | Treisman, Anne M. |
description | Two messages were presented dichotically and subjects were asked to "shadow" whatever they heard on one ear. Somewhere in the middle the two passages were switched to the opposite ears. Subjects occasionally repeated one or two words, at the break, from the wrong ear, but never transferred to it for longer than this. The higher the transition probabilities in the passage the more likely they were to do this. One explanation might be that the "selective filter" (Broadbent, 1958) acts by selectively raising thresholds for signals from the rejected sources rather than acting as an all-or-none barrier. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1080/17470216008416732 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_journals_1301819348</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sage_id>10.1080_17470216008416732</sage_id><sourcerecordid>1301819348</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c361t-42fd0595d858190500743d6aaa005c3b63f0eaeb72459b5b875b1b8eb4b5d1273</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9kEtLAzEUhYMoWKs_wN2A4G7qzWuSghspvqDgRsFdSGYyJWWa1CSj9t87ZVwIoqvLvZzv3MNB6BzDDIOEKyyYAIIrAMlwJSg5QJP9rQRCqkM0AaC05Jy_HqOTlNbDijmTE3S5CD7bz9zrrqh7mwrni2Q7W2f3bovOpWy986tTdNTqLtmz7zlFL3e3z4uHcvl0_7i4WZY1rXAuGWkb4HPeSC7xHDiAYLSptNYAvKamoi1YbY0gjM8NN1Jwg420hhneYCLoFF2MvtsY3oY4Wa1DH_3wUmEKeDClTA4qPKrqGFKKtlXb6DY67hQGta9D_apjYGYjk_TK_nD9B7geAefbEDf6I8SuUVnvuhDbqH3tkqJ_41-gZHAF</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1301819348</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Contextual cues in selective listening</title><source>Periodicals Index Online</source><source>Taylor & Francis Journals Complete</source><creator>Treisman, Anne M.</creator><creatorcontrib>Treisman, Anne M.</creatorcontrib><description>Two messages were presented dichotically and subjects were asked to "shadow" whatever they heard on one ear. Somewhere in the middle the two passages were switched to the opposite ears. Subjects occasionally repeated one or two words, at the break, from the wrong ear, but never transferred to it for longer than this. The higher the transition probabilities in the passage the more likely they were to do this. One explanation might be that the "selective filter" (Broadbent, 1958) acts by selectively raising thresholds for signals from the rejected sources rather than acting as an all-or-none barrier.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0033-555X</identifier><identifier>ISSN: 1747-0218</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1747-0226</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1080/17470216008416732</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>London, England: Taylor & Francis Group</publisher><ispartof>Quarterly journal of experimental psychology, 1960-10, Vol.12 (4), p.242-248</ispartof><rights>Copyright Taylor & Francis Group, LLC 1960</rights><rights>1960 Experimental Pscyhology Society</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c361t-42fd0595d858190500743d6aaa005c3b63f0eaeb72459b5b875b1b8eb4b5d1273</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c361t-42fd0595d858190500743d6aaa005c3b63f0eaeb72459b5b875b1b8eb4b5d1273</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.tandfonline.com/doi/pdf/10.1080/17470216008416732$$EPDF$$P50$$Ginformaworld$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/17470216008416732$$EHTML$$P50$$Ginformaworld$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,27869,27924,27925,59647,60436</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Treisman, Anne M.</creatorcontrib><title>Contextual cues in selective listening</title><title>Quarterly journal of experimental psychology</title><description>Two messages were presented dichotically and subjects were asked to "shadow" whatever they heard on one ear. Somewhere in the middle the two passages were switched to the opposite ears. Subjects occasionally repeated one or two words, at the break, from the wrong ear, but never transferred to it for longer than this. The higher the transition probabilities in the passage the more likely they were to do this. One explanation might be that the "selective filter" (Broadbent, 1958) acts by selectively raising thresholds for signals from the rejected sources rather than acting as an all-or-none barrier.</description><issn>0033-555X</issn><issn>1747-0218</issn><issn>1747-0226</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1960</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>K30</sourceid><recordid>eNp9kEtLAzEUhYMoWKs_wN2A4G7qzWuSghspvqDgRsFdSGYyJWWa1CSj9t87ZVwIoqvLvZzv3MNB6BzDDIOEKyyYAIIrAMlwJSg5QJP9rQRCqkM0AaC05Jy_HqOTlNbDijmTE3S5CD7bz9zrrqh7mwrni2Q7W2f3bovOpWy986tTdNTqLtmz7zlFL3e3z4uHcvl0_7i4WZY1rXAuGWkb4HPeSC7xHDiAYLSptNYAvKamoi1YbY0gjM8NN1Jwg420hhneYCLoFF2MvtsY3oY4Wa1DH_3wUmEKeDClTA4qPKrqGFKKtlXb6DY67hQGta9D_apjYGYjk_TK_nD9B7geAefbEDf6I8SuUVnvuhDbqH3tkqJ_41-gZHAF</recordid><startdate>196010</startdate><enddate>196010</enddate><creator>Treisman, Anne M.</creator><general>Taylor & Francis Group</general><general>SAGE Publications</general><general>Academic Press</general><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>FYSDU</scope><scope>GPCCI</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope></search><sort><creationdate>196010</creationdate><title>Contextual cues in selective listening</title><author>Treisman, Anne M.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c361t-42fd0595d858190500743d6aaa005c3b63f0eaeb72459b5b875b1b8eb4b5d1273</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1960</creationdate><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Treisman, Anne M.</creatorcontrib><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 07</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 10</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><jtitle>Quarterly journal of experimental psychology</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Treisman, Anne M.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Contextual cues in selective listening</atitle><jtitle>Quarterly journal of experimental psychology</jtitle><date>1960-10</date><risdate>1960</risdate><volume>12</volume><issue>4</issue><spage>242</spage><epage>248</epage><pages>242-248</pages><issn>0033-555X</issn><issn>1747-0218</issn><eissn>1747-0226</eissn><abstract>Two messages were presented dichotically and subjects were asked to "shadow" whatever they heard on one ear. Somewhere in the middle the two passages were switched to the opposite ears. Subjects occasionally repeated one or two words, at the break, from the wrong ear, but never transferred to it for longer than this. The higher the transition probabilities in the passage the more likely they were to do this. One explanation might be that the "selective filter" (Broadbent, 1958) acts by selectively raising thresholds for signals from the rejected sources rather than acting as an all-or-none barrier.</abstract><cop>London, England</cop><pub>Taylor & Francis Group</pub><doi>10.1080/17470216008416732</doi><tpages>7</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0033-555X |
ispartof | Quarterly journal of experimental psychology, 1960-10, Vol.12 (4), p.242-248 |
issn | 0033-555X 1747-0218 1747-0226 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_journals_1301819348 |
source | Periodicals Index Online; Taylor & Francis Journals Complete |
title | Contextual cues in selective listening |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-02T04%3A04%3A59IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Contextual%20cues%20in%20selective%20listening&rft.jtitle=Quarterly%20journal%20of%20experimental%20psychology&rft.au=Treisman,%20Anne%20M.&rft.date=1960-10&rft.volume=12&rft.issue=4&rft.spage=242&rft.epage=248&rft.pages=242-248&rft.issn=0033-555X&rft.eissn=1747-0226&rft_id=info:doi/10.1080/17470216008416732&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E1301819348%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1301819348&rft_id=info:pmid/&rft_sage_id=10.1080_17470216008416732&rfr_iscdi=true |