Differential conditioning using covert stimuli

Differential conditioning of verbal operants using covert stimuli, based on Cautela's covert reinforcement and covert sensitization methods of self control therapy, was studied in the laboratory. Each of 22 subjects was trained to visualize a reinforcing and an aversive scene, based on reported...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Behavior therapy 1973, Vol.4 (1), p.96-99
Hauptverfasser: Epstein, Leonard H., Peterson, Gerald L.
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 99
container_issue 1
container_start_page 96
container_title Behavior therapy
container_volume 4
creator Epstein, Leonard H.
Peterson, Gerald L.
description Differential conditioning of verbal operants using covert stimuli, based on Cautela's covert reinforcement and covert sensitization methods of self control therapy, was studied in the laboratory. Each of 22 subjects was trained to visualize a reinforcing and an aversive scene, based on reported preferences from stimuli on the Reinforcement Survey Schedule. The operant was the emission of a number between 0 and 100. For one group the consequence of responding with a number ending in 1–3 was the self-presented reinforcing scene cued by the experimenter, with the aversive scene contingent upon a number ending in 7–9. The consequences were reversed for the other group. The results showed that contingent covert stimuli significantly changed the rates of the selected operants from baseline. This study supports Cautela's contention that covert stimuli can control behavior. The difficulty of measuring the covert stimulus was noted.
doi_str_mv 10.1016/S0005-7894(73)80078-4
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_journals_1300422366</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><els_id>S0005789473800784</els_id><sourcerecordid>1300422366</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c337t-e9d79f92e2db0517409444299c0962acadf6526ff5f97e9314151481039820913</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkM1LAzEQxYMoWKt_glDwooetk69NchKpn1DwoJ7Dmp1ISrupSbbgf--2Fa9eZubw3hvej5BzClMKtL5-BQBZKW3EpeJXGkDpShyQEdXDQbXWh2T0JzkmJzkvADhwKkdkehe8x4RdCc1y4mLXhhJiF7rPSZ-308UNpjLJJaz6ZTglR75ZZjz73WPy_nD_Nnuq5i-Pz7PbeeU4V6VC0yrjDUPWfoCkSoARQjBjHJiaNa5pfS1Z7b30RqHhVFBJhabAjWZgKB-Ti33uOsWvHnOxi9inbnhpKQcQjPG6HlRyr3Ip5pzQ23UKqyZ9Wwp2i8bu0Nhtb6u43aGxYvDd7H04VNgETDa7gJ3DNiR0xbYx_JPwA5ZkaO4</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1300422366</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Differential conditioning using covert stimuli</title><source>Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals</source><source>Periodicals Index Online</source><creator>Epstein, Leonard H. ; Peterson, Gerald L.</creator><creatorcontrib>Epstein, Leonard H. ; Peterson, Gerald L.</creatorcontrib><description>Differential conditioning of verbal operants using covert stimuli, based on Cautela's covert reinforcement and covert sensitization methods of self control therapy, was studied in the laboratory. Each of 22 subjects was trained to visualize a reinforcing and an aversive scene, based on reported preferences from stimuli on the Reinforcement Survey Schedule. The operant was the emission of a number between 0 and 100. For one group the consequence of responding with a number ending in 1–3 was the self-presented reinforcing scene cued by the experimenter, with the aversive scene contingent upon a number ending in 7–9. The consequences were reversed for the other group. The results showed that contingent covert stimuli significantly changed the rates of the selected operants from baseline. This study supports Cautela's contention that covert stimuli can control behavior. The difficulty of measuring the covert stimulus was noted.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0005-7894</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1878-1888</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/S0005-7894(73)80078-4</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>New York: Elsevier Ltd</publisher><ispartof>Behavior therapy, 1973, Vol.4 (1), p.96-99</ispartof><rights>1973 Academic Press, Inc.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c337t-e9d79f92e2db0517409444299c0962acadf6526ff5f97e9314151481039820913</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c337t-e9d79f92e2db0517409444299c0962acadf6526ff5f97e9314151481039820913</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0005789473800784$$EHTML$$P50$$Gelsevier$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,3537,4010,27846,27900,27901,27902,65306</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Epstein, Leonard H.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Peterson, Gerald L.</creatorcontrib><title>Differential conditioning using covert stimuli</title><title>Behavior therapy</title><description>Differential conditioning of verbal operants using covert stimuli, based on Cautela's covert reinforcement and covert sensitization methods of self control therapy, was studied in the laboratory. Each of 22 subjects was trained to visualize a reinforcing and an aversive scene, based on reported preferences from stimuli on the Reinforcement Survey Schedule. The operant was the emission of a number between 0 and 100. For one group the consequence of responding with a number ending in 1–3 was the self-presented reinforcing scene cued by the experimenter, with the aversive scene contingent upon a number ending in 7–9. The consequences were reversed for the other group. The results showed that contingent covert stimuli significantly changed the rates of the selected operants from baseline. This study supports Cautela's contention that covert stimuli can control behavior. The difficulty of measuring the covert stimulus was noted.</description><issn>0005-7894</issn><issn>1878-1888</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1973</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>K30</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkM1LAzEQxYMoWKt_glDwooetk69NchKpn1DwoJ7Dmp1ISrupSbbgf--2Fa9eZubw3hvej5BzClMKtL5-BQBZKW3EpeJXGkDpShyQEdXDQbXWh2T0JzkmJzkvADhwKkdkehe8x4RdCc1y4mLXhhJiF7rPSZ-308UNpjLJJaz6ZTglR75ZZjz73WPy_nD_Nnuq5i-Pz7PbeeU4V6VC0yrjDUPWfoCkSoARQjBjHJiaNa5pfS1Z7b30RqHhVFBJhabAjWZgKB-Ti33uOsWvHnOxi9inbnhpKQcQjPG6HlRyr3Ip5pzQ23UKqyZ9Wwp2i8bu0Nhtb6u43aGxYvDd7H04VNgETDa7gJ3DNiR0xbYx_JPwA5ZkaO4</recordid><startdate>1973</startdate><enddate>1973</enddate><creator>Epstein, Leonard H.</creator><creator>Peterson, Gerald L.</creator><general>Elsevier Ltd</general><general>Academic Press</general><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>HFXKP</scope><scope>IOIBA</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope></search><sort><creationdate>1973</creationdate><title>Differential conditioning using covert stimuli</title><author>Epstein, Leonard H. ; Peterson, Gerald L.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c337t-e9d79f92e2db0517409444299c0962acadf6526ff5f97e9314151481039820913</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1973</creationdate><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Epstein, Leonard H.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Peterson, Gerald L.</creatorcontrib><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 17</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 29</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><jtitle>Behavior therapy</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Epstein, Leonard H.</au><au>Peterson, Gerald L.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Differential conditioning using covert stimuli</atitle><jtitle>Behavior therapy</jtitle><date>1973</date><risdate>1973</risdate><volume>4</volume><issue>1</issue><spage>96</spage><epage>99</epage><pages>96-99</pages><issn>0005-7894</issn><eissn>1878-1888</eissn><abstract>Differential conditioning of verbal operants using covert stimuli, based on Cautela's covert reinforcement and covert sensitization methods of self control therapy, was studied in the laboratory. Each of 22 subjects was trained to visualize a reinforcing and an aversive scene, based on reported preferences from stimuli on the Reinforcement Survey Schedule. The operant was the emission of a number between 0 and 100. For one group the consequence of responding with a number ending in 1–3 was the self-presented reinforcing scene cued by the experimenter, with the aversive scene contingent upon a number ending in 7–9. The consequences were reversed for the other group. The results showed that contingent covert stimuli significantly changed the rates of the selected operants from baseline. This study supports Cautela's contention that covert stimuli can control behavior. The difficulty of measuring the covert stimulus was noted.</abstract><cop>New York</cop><pub>Elsevier Ltd</pub><doi>10.1016/S0005-7894(73)80078-4</doi><tpages>4</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0005-7894
ispartof Behavior therapy, 1973, Vol.4 (1), p.96-99
issn 0005-7894
1878-1888
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_journals_1300422366
source Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals; Periodicals Index Online
title Differential conditioning using covert stimuli
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-06T11%3A07%3A52IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Differential%20conditioning%20using%20covert%20stimuli&rft.jtitle=Behavior%20therapy&rft.au=Epstein,%20Leonard%20H.&rft.date=1973&rft.volume=4&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=96&rft.epage=99&rft.pages=96-99&rft.issn=0005-7894&rft.eissn=1878-1888&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/S0005-7894(73)80078-4&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E1300422366%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1300422366&rft_id=info:pmid/&rft_els_id=S0005789473800784&rfr_iscdi=true