Young children's use of models and photographs as spatial representations
The ability of 2.5- and 3-year-old children to use a scale model and photographs as representations of a space is examined here, as a replication and extension of DeLoache (1987, 1989a, 1989b, 1990, 1991). Like DeLoache, the results of the present study indicate that 2.5-year-olds have great difficu...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Cognitive development 1992-07, Vol.7 (3), p.351-363 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 363 |
---|---|
container_issue | 3 |
container_start_page | 351 |
container_title | Cognitive development |
container_volume | 7 |
creator | Dow, Gina Annunziato Pick, Herbert L. |
description | The ability of 2.5- and 3-year-old children to use a scale model and photographs as representations of a space is examined here, as a replication and extension of
DeLoache (1987, 1989a, 1989b, 1990, 1991). Like DeLoache, the results of the present study indicate that 2.5-year-olds have great difficulty when asked to use information from a scale model of one place; they do not have this trouble with photographs of the region. However, they have similar difficulties with using photographs when the space depicted by the photograph is characterized as belonging to another agent. This combination of results suggest that both the three-dimensionality and “ownership” may interfere with appreciating the “dual orientation” of some representations as both real things in themselves and representations of something else, as suggested by DeLoache. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1016/0885-2014(92)90021-I |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_journals_1297659404</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><els_id>088520149290021I</els_id><sourcerecordid>1297659404</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c334t-5466fa8776b21bc7f8b91c14a38a301254e9fb9d084ca5a1a2212c6338b9664b3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9kE1LxDAQhoMouK7-Aw8BD-qhmknTtLkIsvhRWPCioKeQptPdLrWpSSv478264tHTMPC87zAPIafAroCBvGZFkSWcgbhQ_FIxxiEp98gMilwlnMvXfTL7Qw7JUQgbFmMqgxkp39zUr6hdt13tsT8PdApIXUPfXY1doKav6bB2o1t5M6zjHmgYzNiajnocPAbsx7i6PhyTg8Z0AU9-55y83N89Lx6T5dNDubhdJjZNxZhkQsrGFHkuKw6VzZuiUmBBmLQwKQOeCVRNpWpWCGsyA4Zz4FamaeSkFFU6J2e73sG7jwnDqDdu8n08qYGrXGZKMBEpsaOsdyF4bPTg23fjvzQwvZWmt0b01ohWXP9I02WM3exi8Xf8bNHrYFvsLdatRzvq2rX_F3wDqYdyzg</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1297659404</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Young children's use of models and photographs as spatial representations</title><source>Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals Complete</source><source>Periodicals Index Online</source><creator>Dow, Gina Annunziato ; Pick, Herbert L.</creator><creatorcontrib>Dow, Gina Annunziato ; Pick, Herbert L.</creatorcontrib><description>The ability of 2.5- and 3-year-old children to use a scale model and photographs as representations of a space is examined here, as a replication and extension of
DeLoache (1987, 1989a, 1989b, 1990, 1991). Like DeLoache, the results of the present study indicate that 2.5-year-olds have great difficulty when asked to use information from a scale model of one place; they do not have this trouble with photographs of the region. However, they have similar difficulties with using photographs when the space depicted by the photograph is characterized as belonging to another agent. This combination of results suggest that both the three-dimensionality and “ownership” may interfere with appreciating the “dual orientation” of some representations as both real things in themselves and representations of something else, as suggested by DeLoache.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0885-2014</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1879-226X</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/0885-2014(92)90021-I</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Norwood, N.J: Elsevier Inc</publisher><ispartof>Cognitive development, 1992-07, Vol.7 (3), p.351-363</ispartof><rights>1992</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c334t-5466fa8776b21bc7f8b91c14a38a301254e9fb9d084ca5a1a2212c6338b9664b3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c334t-5466fa8776b21bc7f8b91c14a38a301254e9fb9d084ca5a1a2212c6338b9664b3</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0885-2014(92)90021-I$$EHTML$$P50$$Gelsevier$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,3550,27869,27924,27925,45995</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Dow, Gina Annunziato</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Pick, Herbert L.</creatorcontrib><title>Young children's use of models and photographs as spatial representations</title><title>Cognitive development</title><description>The ability of 2.5- and 3-year-old children to use a scale model and photographs as representations of a space is examined here, as a replication and extension of
DeLoache (1987, 1989a, 1989b, 1990, 1991). Like DeLoache, the results of the present study indicate that 2.5-year-olds have great difficulty when asked to use information from a scale model of one place; they do not have this trouble with photographs of the region. However, they have similar difficulties with using photographs when the space depicted by the photograph is characterized as belonging to another agent. This combination of results suggest that both the three-dimensionality and “ownership” may interfere with appreciating the “dual orientation” of some representations as both real things in themselves and representations of something else, as suggested by DeLoache.</description><issn>0885-2014</issn><issn>1879-226X</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1992</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>K30</sourceid><recordid>eNp9kE1LxDAQhoMouK7-Aw8BD-qhmknTtLkIsvhRWPCioKeQptPdLrWpSSv478264tHTMPC87zAPIafAroCBvGZFkSWcgbhQ_FIxxiEp98gMilwlnMvXfTL7Qw7JUQgbFmMqgxkp39zUr6hdt13tsT8PdApIXUPfXY1doKav6bB2o1t5M6zjHmgYzNiajnocPAbsx7i6PhyTg8Z0AU9-55y83N89Lx6T5dNDubhdJjZNxZhkQsrGFHkuKw6VzZuiUmBBmLQwKQOeCVRNpWpWCGsyA4Zz4FamaeSkFFU6J2e73sG7jwnDqDdu8n08qYGrXGZKMBEpsaOsdyF4bPTg23fjvzQwvZWmt0b01ohWXP9I02WM3exi8Xf8bNHrYFvsLdatRzvq2rX_F3wDqYdyzg</recordid><startdate>19920701</startdate><enddate>19920701</enddate><creator>Dow, Gina Annunziato</creator><creator>Pick, Herbert L.</creator><general>Elsevier Inc</general><general>Ablex Pub. Corp</general><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope><scope>SAAPM</scope></search><sort><creationdate>19920701</creationdate><title>Young children's use of models and photographs as spatial representations</title><author>Dow, Gina Annunziato ; Pick, Herbert L.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c334t-5466fa8776b21bc7f8b91c14a38a301254e9fb9d084ca5a1a2212c6338b9664b3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1992</creationdate><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Dow, Gina Annunziato</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Pick, Herbert L.</creatorcontrib><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 42</collection><jtitle>Cognitive development</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Dow, Gina Annunziato</au><au>Pick, Herbert L.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Young children's use of models and photographs as spatial representations</atitle><jtitle>Cognitive development</jtitle><date>1992-07-01</date><risdate>1992</risdate><volume>7</volume><issue>3</issue><spage>351</spage><epage>363</epage><pages>351-363</pages><issn>0885-2014</issn><eissn>1879-226X</eissn><abstract>The ability of 2.5- and 3-year-old children to use a scale model and photographs as representations of a space is examined here, as a replication and extension of
DeLoache (1987, 1989a, 1989b, 1990, 1991). Like DeLoache, the results of the present study indicate that 2.5-year-olds have great difficulty when asked to use information from a scale model of one place; they do not have this trouble with photographs of the region. However, they have similar difficulties with using photographs when the space depicted by the photograph is characterized as belonging to another agent. This combination of results suggest that both the three-dimensionality and “ownership” may interfere with appreciating the “dual orientation” of some representations as both real things in themselves and representations of something else, as suggested by DeLoache.</abstract><cop>Norwood, N.J</cop><pub>Elsevier Inc</pub><doi>10.1016/0885-2014(92)90021-I</doi><tpages>13</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0885-2014 |
ispartof | Cognitive development, 1992-07, Vol.7 (3), p.351-363 |
issn | 0885-2014 1879-226X |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_journals_1297659404 |
source | Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals Complete; Periodicals Index Online |
title | Young children's use of models and photographs as spatial representations |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-26T02%3A39%3A14IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Young%20children's%20use%20of%20models%20and%20photographs%20as%20spatial%20representations&rft.jtitle=Cognitive%20development&rft.au=Dow,%20Gina%20Annunziato&rft.date=1992-07-01&rft.volume=7&rft.issue=3&rft.spage=351&rft.epage=363&rft.pages=351-363&rft.issn=0885-2014&rft.eissn=1879-226X&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/0885-2014(92)90021-I&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E1297659404%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1297659404&rft_id=info:pmid/&rft_els_id=088520149290021I&rfr_iscdi=true |