Observer Effects and Avian-Call-Count Survey Quality: Rare-Species Biases and Overconfidence
Wildlife monitoring surveys are prone to nondetection errors and false positives. To determine factors that affect the incidence of these errors, we built an Internet-based survey that simulated avian point counts, and measured error rates among volunteer observers. Using similar-sounding vocalizati...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | The Auk 2012-01, Vol.129 (1), p.76-86 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 86 |
---|---|
container_issue | 1 |
container_start_page | 76 |
container_title | The Auk |
container_volume | 129 |
creator | Farmer, Robert G Leonard, Marty L Horn, Andrew G |
description | Wildlife monitoring surveys are prone to nondetection errors and false positives. To determine factors that affect the incidence of these errors, we built an Internet-based survey that simulated avian point counts, and measured error rates among volunteer observers. Using similar-sounding vocalizations from paired rare and common bird species, we measured the effects of species rarity and observer skill, and the influence of a reward system that explicitly encouraged the detection of rare species. Higher self-reported skill levels and common species independently predicted fewer nondetections (probability range: 0.11 [experts, common species] to 0.54 [moderates, rare species]). Overall proportions of detections that were false positives increased significantly as skill level declined (range: 0.06 [experts, common species] to 0.22 [moderates, rare species]). Moderately skilled observers were significantly more likely to report false-positive records of common species than of rare species, whereas experts were significantly more likely to report false-positives of rare species than of common species. The reward for correctly detecting rare species did not significantly affect these patterns. Because false positives can also result from observers overestimating their own abilities (“overconfidence”), we lastly tested whether observers' beliefs that they had recorded error-free data (“confidence”) tended to be incorrect (“overconfident”), and whether this pattern varied with skill. Observer confidence increased significantly with observer skill, whereas overconfidence was uniformly high (overall mean proportion = 0.73). Our results emphasize the value of controlling for observer skill in data collection and modeling and do not support the use of opinion-based (i.e., subjective) indications of observer confidence. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1525/auk.2012.11129 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>jstor_proqu</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_journals_1017642461</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><jstor_id>10.1525/auk.2012.11129</jstor_id><sourcerecordid>10.1525/auk.2012.11129</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-b380t-13038acb4ffd5ab3244d1374aca60e61f22e60bfbacea14a633eaad4e1aa95ff3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkE1Lw0AQhhdRsFavngMedeN-JU281VA_oFC0ehPCZDMLqTFbd5NC_71b493TMPC87wwPIZecxTwRyS0Mn7FgXMScc5EfkQnPZUaVSNQxmTDGFM2YzE7JmfebsCYsyyfkY1V5dDt00cIY1L2PoKuj-a6BjhbQtrSwQ9dH6yEw--hlgLbp93fRKzik6y3qBn1034DHMbgKTdp2pqmx03hOTgy0Hi_-5pS8Pyzeiie6XD0-F_MlrWTGespl-At0pYypE6ikUKrmcqZAQ8ow5UYITFllKtAIXEEqJQLUCjlAnhgjp-Rq7N06-z2g78uNHVwXTpac8VmqhEp5oOKR0s5679CUW9d8gdsHqDwYLIPB8mCw_DUYAtdjYON76_6nb0a6aqzt8D_8B6nJgHo</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1017642461</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Observer Effects and Avian-Call-Count Survey Quality: Rare-Species Biases and Overconfidence</title><source>JSTOR Archive Collection A-Z Listing</source><source>EZB-FREE-00999 freely available EZB journals</source><creator>Farmer, Robert G ; Leonard, Marty L ; Horn, Andrew G</creator><creatorcontrib>Farmer, Robert G ; Leonard, Marty L ; Horn, Andrew G</creatorcontrib><description>Wildlife monitoring surveys are prone to nondetection errors and false positives. To determine factors that affect the incidence of these errors, we built an Internet-based survey that simulated avian point counts, and measured error rates among volunteer observers. Using similar-sounding vocalizations from paired rare and common bird species, we measured the effects of species rarity and observer skill, and the influence of a reward system that explicitly encouraged the detection of rare species. Higher self-reported skill levels and common species independently predicted fewer nondetections (probability range: 0.11 [experts, common species] to 0.54 [moderates, rare species]). Overall proportions of detections that were false positives increased significantly as skill level declined (range: 0.06 [experts, common species] to 0.22 [moderates, rare species]). Moderately skilled observers were significantly more likely to report false-positive records of common species than of rare species, whereas experts were significantly more likely to report false-positives of rare species than of common species. The reward for correctly detecting rare species did not significantly affect these patterns. Because false positives can also result from observers overestimating their own abilities (“overconfidence”), we lastly tested whether observers' beliefs that they had recorded error-free data (“confidence”) tended to be incorrect (“overconfident”), and whether this pattern varied with skill. Observer confidence increased significantly with observer skill, whereas overconfidence was uniformly high (overall mean proportion = 0.73). Our results emphasize the value of controlling for observer skill in data collection and modeling and do not support the use of opinion-based (i.e., subjective) indications of observer confidence.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0004-8038</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1938-4254</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 2732-4613</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1525/auk.2012.11129</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Waco: University of California Press</publisher><subject>acoustic survey ; Animal vocalization ; Aviculture ; bias ; Bird songs ; Birds ; Breeding ; call count surveys ; citizen science ; Data collection ; detection ; Error rates ; False positive errors ; Methods and Statistics ; nondetection ; Observational research ; observer effects ; Participant observation ; Rare species ; Studies ; Volunteerism ; Volunteers ; Wildlife</subject><ispartof>The Auk, 2012-01, Vol.129 (1), p.76-86</ispartof><rights>2012 by The American Ornithologists' Union. All rights reserved. Please direct all requests for permission to photocopy or reproduce article content through the University of California Press's Rights and Permissions website, http://www.ucpressjournals.com/reprintInfo.asp.</rights><rights>2012 by The American Ornithologists' Union</rights><rights>Copyright (c) 2012 by The American Ornithologists' Union</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-b380t-13038acb4ffd5ab3244d1374aca60e61f22e60bfbacea14a633eaad4e1aa95ff3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-b380t-13038acb4ffd5ab3244d1374aca60e61f22e60bfbacea14a633eaad4e1aa95ff3</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,803,27924,27925</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Farmer, Robert G</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Leonard, Marty L</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Horn, Andrew G</creatorcontrib><title>Observer Effects and Avian-Call-Count Survey Quality: Rare-Species Biases and Overconfidence</title><title>The Auk</title><description>Wildlife monitoring surveys are prone to nondetection errors and false positives. To determine factors that affect the incidence of these errors, we built an Internet-based survey that simulated avian point counts, and measured error rates among volunteer observers. Using similar-sounding vocalizations from paired rare and common bird species, we measured the effects of species rarity and observer skill, and the influence of a reward system that explicitly encouraged the detection of rare species. Higher self-reported skill levels and common species independently predicted fewer nondetections (probability range: 0.11 [experts, common species] to 0.54 [moderates, rare species]). Overall proportions of detections that were false positives increased significantly as skill level declined (range: 0.06 [experts, common species] to 0.22 [moderates, rare species]). Moderately skilled observers were significantly more likely to report false-positive records of common species than of rare species, whereas experts were significantly more likely to report false-positives of rare species than of common species. The reward for correctly detecting rare species did not significantly affect these patterns. Because false positives can also result from observers overestimating their own abilities (“overconfidence”), we lastly tested whether observers' beliefs that they had recorded error-free data (“confidence”) tended to be incorrect (“overconfident”), and whether this pattern varied with skill. Observer confidence increased significantly with observer skill, whereas overconfidence was uniformly high (overall mean proportion = 0.73). Our results emphasize the value of controlling for observer skill in data collection and modeling and do not support the use of opinion-based (i.e., subjective) indications of observer confidence.</description><subject>acoustic survey</subject><subject>Animal vocalization</subject><subject>Aviculture</subject><subject>bias</subject><subject>Bird songs</subject><subject>Birds</subject><subject>Breeding</subject><subject>call count surveys</subject><subject>citizen science</subject><subject>Data collection</subject><subject>detection</subject><subject>Error rates</subject><subject>False positive errors</subject><subject>Methods and Statistics</subject><subject>nondetection</subject><subject>Observational research</subject><subject>observer effects</subject><subject>Participant observation</subject><subject>Rare species</subject><subject>Studies</subject><subject>Volunteerism</subject><subject>Volunteers</subject><subject>Wildlife</subject><issn>0004-8038</issn><issn>1938-4254</issn><issn>2732-4613</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2012</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>8G5</sourceid><sourceid>ABUWG</sourceid><sourceid>AFKRA</sourceid><sourceid>AZQEC</sourceid><sourceid>BEC</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>CCPQU</sourceid><sourceid>DWQXO</sourceid><sourceid>GNUQQ</sourceid><sourceid>GUQSH</sourceid><sourceid>M2O</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkE1Lw0AQhhdRsFavngMedeN-JU281VA_oFC0ehPCZDMLqTFbd5NC_71b493TMPC87wwPIZecxTwRyS0Mn7FgXMScc5EfkQnPZUaVSNQxmTDGFM2YzE7JmfebsCYsyyfkY1V5dDt00cIY1L2PoKuj-a6BjhbQtrSwQ9dH6yEw--hlgLbp93fRKzik6y3qBn1034DHMbgKTdp2pqmx03hOTgy0Hi_-5pS8Pyzeiie6XD0-F_MlrWTGespl-At0pYypE6ikUKrmcqZAQ8ow5UYITFllKtAIXEEqJQLUCjlAnhgjp-Rq7N06-z2g78uNHVwXTpac8VmqhEp5oOKR0s5679CUW9d8gdsHqDwYLIPB8mCw_DUYAtdjYON76_6nb0a6aqzt8D_8B6nJgHo</recordid><startdate>201201</startdate><enddate>201201</enddate><creator>Farmer, Robert G</creator><creator>Leonard, Marty L</creator><creator>Horn, Andrew G</creator><general>University of California Press</general><general>American Ornithological Society</general><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>7SN</scope><scope>7TN</scope><scope>7U9</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88A</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>88I</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FE</scope><scope>8FH</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>8G5</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BBNVY</scope><scope>BEC</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BHPHI</scope><scope>C1K</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>F1W</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>GUQSH</scope><scope>H94</scope><scope>H95</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>L.G</scope><scope>LK8</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>M2O</scope><scope>M2P</scope><scope>M7P</scope><scope>MBDVC</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PADUT</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>S0X</scope></search><sort><creationdate>201201</creationdate><title>Observer Effects and Avian-Call-Count Survey Quality: Rare-Species Biases and Overconfidence</title><author>Farmer, Robert G ; Leonard, Marty L ; Horn, Andrew G</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-b380t-13038acb4ffd5ab3244d1374aca60e61f22e60bfbacea14a633eaad4e1aa95ff3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2012</creationdate><topic>acoustic survey</topic><topic>Animal vocalization</topic><topic>Aviculture</topic><topic>bias</topic><topic>Bird songs</topic><topic>Birds</topic><topic>Breeding</topic><topic>call count surveys</topic><topic>citizen science</topic><topic>Data collection</topic><topic>detection</topic><topic>Error rates</topic><topic>False positive errors</topic><topic>Methods and Statistics</topic><topic>nondetection</topic><topic>Observational research</topic><topic>observer effects</topic><topic>Participant observation</topic><topic>Rare species</topic><topic>Studies</topic><topic>Volunteerism</topic><topic>Volunteers</topic><topic>Wildlife</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Farmer, Robert G</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Leonard, Marty L</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Horn, Andrew G</creatorcontrib><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>Ecology Abstracts</collection><collection>Oceanic Abstracts</collection><collection>Virology and AIDS Abstracts</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Biology Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>Science Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>ProQuest SciTech Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Research Library (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>eLibrary</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>ASFA: Aquatic Sciences and Fisheries Abstracts</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>Research Library Prep</collection><collection>AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts</collection><collection>Aquatic Science & Fisheries Abstracts (ASFA) 1: Biological Sciences & Living Resources</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection</collection><collection>Aquatic Science & Fisheries Abstracts (ASFA) Professional</collection><collection>ProQuest Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>Psychology Database</collection><collection>Research Library</collection><collection>Science Database</collection><collection>Biological Science Database</collection><collection>Research Library (Corporate)</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>Research Library China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>SIRS Editorial</collection><jtitle>The Auk</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Farmer, Robert G</au><au>Leonard, Marty L</au><au>Horn, Andrew G</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Observer Effects and Avian-Call-Count Survey Quality: Rare-Species Biases and Overconfidence</atitle><jtitle>The Auk</jtitle><date>2012-01</date><risdate>2012</risdate><volume>129</volume><issue>1</issue><spage>76</spage><epage>86</epage><pages>76-86</pages><issn>0004-8038</issn><eissn>1938-4254</eissn><eissn>2732-4613</eissn><abstract>Wildlife monitoring surveys are prone to nondetection errors and false positives. To determine factors that affect the incidence of these errors, we built an Internet-based survey that simulated avian point counts, and measured error rates among volunteer observers. Using similar-sounding vocalizations from paired rare and common bird species, we measured the effects of species rarity and observer skill, and the influence of a reward system that explicitly encouraged the detection of rare species. Higher self-reported skill levels and common species independently predicted fewer nondetections (probability range: 0.11 [experts, common species] to 0.54 [moderates, rare species]). Overall proportions of detections that were false positives increased significantly as skill level declined (range: 0.06 [experts, common species] to 0.22 [moderates, rare species]). Moderately skilled observers were significantly more likely to report false-positive records of common species than of rare species, whereas experts were significantly more likely to report false-positives of rare species than of common species. The reward for correctly detecting rare species did not significantly affect these patterns. Because false positives can also result from observers overestimating their own abilities (“overconfidence”), we lastly tested whether observers' beliefs that they had recorded error-free data (“confidence”) tended to be incorrect (“overconfident”), and whether this pattern varied with skill. Observer confidence increased significantly with observer skill, whereas overconfidence was uniformly high (overall mean proportion = 0.73). Our results emphasize the value of controlling for observer skill in data collection and modeling and do not support the use of opinion-based (i.e., subjective) indications of observer confidence.</abstract><cop>Waco</cop><pub>University of California Press</pub><doi>10.1525/auk.2012.11129</doi><tpages>11</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0004-8038 |
ispartof | The Auk, 2012-01, Vol.129 (1), p.76-86 |
issn | 0004-8038 1938-4254 2732-4613 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_journals_1017642461 |
source | JSTOR Archive Collection A-Z Listing; EZB-FREE-00999 freely available EZB journals |
subjects | acoustic survey Animal vocalization Aviculture bias Bird songs Birds Breeding call count surveys citizen science Data collection detection Error rates False positive errors Methods and Statistics nondetection Observational research observer effects Participant observation Rare species Studies Volunteerism Volunteers Wildlife |
title | Observer Effects and Avian-Call-Count Survey Quality: Rare-Species Biases and Overconfidence |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-29T06%3A37%3A00IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-jstor_proqu&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Observer%20Effects%20and%20Avian-Call-Count%20Survey%20Quality:%20Rare-Species%20Biases%20and%20Overconfidence&rft.jtitle=The%20Auk&rft.au=Farmer,%20Robert%20G&rft.date=2012-01&rft.volume=129&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=76&rft.epage=86&rft.pages=76-86&rft.issn=0004-8038&rft.eissn=1938-4254&rft_id=info:doi/10.1525/auk.2012.11129&rft_dat=%3Cjstor_proqu%3E10.1525/auk.2012.11129%3C/jstor_proqu%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1017642461&rft_id=info:pmid/&rft_jstor_id=10.1525/auk.2012.11129&rfr_iscdi=true |