Observer Effects and Avian-Call-Count Survey Quality: Rare-Species Biases and Overconfidence

Wildlife monitoring surveys are prone to nondetection errors and false positives. To determine factors that affect the incidence of these errors, we built an Internet-based survey that simulated avian point counts, and measured error rates among volunteer observers. Using similar-sounding vocalizati...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:The Auk 2012-01, Vol.129 (1), p.76-86
Hauptverfasser: Farmer, Robert G, Leonard, Marty L, Horn, Andrew G
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 86
container_issue 1
container_start_page 76
container_title The Auk
container_volume 129
creator Farmer, Robert G
Leonard, Marty L
Horn, Andrew G
description Wildlife monitoring surveys are prone to nondetection errors and false positives. To determine factors that affect the incidence of these errors, we built an Internet-based survey that simulated avian point counts, and measured error rates among volunteer observers. Using similar-sounding vocalizations from paired rare and common bird species, we measured the effects of species rarity and observer skill, and the influence of a reward system that explicitly encouraged the detection of rare species. Higher self-reported skill levels and common species independently predicted fewer nondetections (probability range: 0.11 [experts, common species] to 0.54 [moderates, rare species]). Overall proportions of detections that were false positives increased significantly as skill level declined (range: 0.06 [experts, common species] to 0.22 [moderates, rare species]). Moderately skilled observers were significantly more likely to report false-positive records of common species than of rare species, whereas experts were significantly more likely to report false-positives of rare species than of common species. The reward for correctly detecting rare species did not significantly affect these patterns. Because false positives can also result from observers overestimating their own abilities (“overconfidence”), we lastly tested whether observers' beliefs that they had recorded error-free data (“confidence”) tended to be incorrect (“overconfident”), and whether this pattern varied with skill. Observer confidence increased significantly with observer skill, whereas overconfidence was uniformly high (overall mean proportion = 0.73). Our results emphasize the value of controlling for observer skill in data collection and modeling and do not support the use of opinion-based (i.e., subjective) indications of observer confidence.
doi_str_mv 10.1525/auk.2012.11129
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>jstor_proqu</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_journals_1017642461</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><jstor_id>10.1525/auk.2012.11129</jstor_id><sourcerecordid>10.1525/auk.2012.11129</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-b380t-13038acb4ffd5ab3244d1374aca60e61f22e60bfbacea14a633eaad4e1aa95ff3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkE1Lw0AQhhdRsFavngMedeN-JU281VA_oFC0ehPCZDMLqTFbd5NC_71b493TMPC87wwPIZecxTwRyS0Mn7FgXMScc5EfkQnPZUaVSNQxmTDGFM2YzE7JmfebsCYsyyfkY1V5dDt00cIY1L2PoKuj-a6BjhbQtrSwQ9dH6yEw--hlgLbp93fRKzik6y3qBn1034DHMbgKTdp2pqmx03hOTgy0Hi_-5pS8Pyzeiie6XD0-F_MlrWTGespl-At0pYypE6ikUKrmcqZAQ8ow5UYITFllKtAIXEEqJQLUCjlAnhgjp-Rq7N06-z2g78uNHVwXTpac8VmqhEp5oOKR0s5679CUW9d8gdsHqDwYLIPB8mCw_DUYAtdjYON76_6nb0a6aqzt8D_8B6nJgHo</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1017642461</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Observer Effects and Avian-Call-Count Survey Quality: Rare-Species Biases and Overconfidence</title><source>JSTOR Archive Collection A-Z Listing</source><source>EZB-FREE-00999 freely available EZB journals</source><creator>Farmer, Robert G ; Leonard, Marty L ; Horn, Andrew G</creator><creatorcontrib>Farmer, Robert G ; Leonard, Marty L ; Horn, Andrew G</creatorcontrib><description>Wildlife monitoring surveys are prone to nondetection errors and false positives. To determine factors that affect the incidence of these errors, we built an Internet-based survey that simulated avian point counts, and measured error rates among volunteer observers. Using similar-sounding vocalizations from paired rare and common bird species, we measured the effects of species rarity and observer skill, and the influence of a reward system that explicitly encouraged the detection of rare species. Higher self-reported skill levels and common species independently predicted fewer nondetections (probability range: 0.11 [experts, common species] to 0.54 [moderates, rare species]). Overall proportions of detections that were false positives increased significantly as skill level declined (range: 0.06 [experts, common species] to 0.22 [moderates, rare species]). Moderately skilled observers were significantly more likely to report false-positive records of common species than of rare species, whereas experts were significantly more likely to report false-positives of rare species than of common species. The reward for correctly detecting rare species did not significantly affect these patterns. Because false positives can also result from observers overestimating their own abilities (“overconfidence”), we lastly tested whether observers' beliefs that they had recorded error-free data (“confidence”) tended to be incorrect (“overconfident”), and whether this pattern varied with skill. Observer confidence increased significantly with observer skill, whereas overconfidence was uniformly high (overall mean proportion = 0.73). Our results emphasize the value of controlling for observer skill in data collection and modeling and do not support the use of opinion-based (i.e., subjective) indications of observer confidence.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0004-8038</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1938-4254</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 2732-4613</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1525/auk.2012.11129</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Waco: University of California Press</publisher><subject>acoustic survey ; Animal vocalization ; Aviculture ; bias ; Bird songs ; Birds ; Breeding ; call count surveys ; citizen science ; Data collection ; detection ; Error rates ; False positive errors ; Methods and Statistics ; nondetection ; Observational research ; observer effects ; Participant observation ; Rare species ; Studies ; Volunteerism ; Volunteers ; Wildlife</subject><ispartof>The Auk, 2012-01, Vol.129 (1), p.76-86</ispartof><rights>2012 by The American Ornithologists' Union. All rights reserved. Please direct all requests for permission to photocopy or reproduce article content through the University of California Press's Rights and Permissions website, http://www.ucpressjournals.com/reprintInfo.asp.</rights><rights>2012 by The American Ornithologists' Union</rights><rights>Copyright (c) 2012 by The American Ornithologists' Union</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-b380t-13038acb4ffd5ab3244d1374aca60e61f22e60bfbacea14a633eaad4e1aa95ff3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-b380t-13038acb4ffd5ab3244d1374aca60e61f22e60bfbacea14a633eaad4e1aa95ff3</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,803,27924,27925</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Farmer, Robert G</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Leonard, Marty L</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Horn, Andrew G</creatorcontrib><title>Observer Effects and Avian-Call-Count Survey Quality: Rare-Species Biases and Overconfidence</title><title>The Auk</title><description>Wildlife monitoring surveys are prone to nondetection errors and false positives. To determine factors that affect the incidence of these errors, we built an Internet-based survey that simulated avian point counts, and measured error rates among volunteer observers. Using similar-sounding vocalizations from paired rare and common bird species, we measured the effects of species rarity and observer skill, and the influence of a reward system that explicitly encouraged the detection of rare species. Higher self-reported skill levels and common species independently predicted fewer nondetections (probability range: 0.11 [experts, common species] to 0.54 [moderates, rare species]). Overall proportions of detections that were false positives increased significantly as skill level declined (range: 0.06 [experts, common species] to 0.22 [moderates, rare species]). Moderately skilled observers were significantly more likely to report false-positive records of common species than of rare species, whereas experts were significantly more likely to report false-positives of rare species than of common species. The reward for correctly detecting rare species did not significantly affect these patterns. Because false positives can also result from observers overestimating their own abilities (“overconfidence”), we lastly tested whether observers' beliefs that they had recorded error-free data (“confidence”) tended to be incorrect (“overconfident”), and whether this pattern varied with skill. Observer confidence increased significantly with observer skill, whereas overconfidence was uniformly high (overall mean proportion = 0.73). Our results emphasize the value of controlling for observer skill in data collection and modeling and do not support the use of opinion-based (i.e., subjective) indications of observer confidence.</description><subject>acoustic survey</subject><subject>Animal vocalization</subject><subject>Aviculture</subject><subject>bias</subject><subject>Bird songs</subject><subject>Birds</subject><subject>Breeding</subject><subject>call count surveys</subject><subject>citizen science</subject><subject>Data collection</subject><subject>detection</subject><subject>Error rates</subject><subject>False positive errors</subject><subject>Methods and Statistics</subject><subject>nondetection</subject><subject>Observational research</subject><subject>observer effects</subject><subject>Participant observation</subject><subject>Rare species</subject><subject>Studies</subject><subject>Volunteerism</subject><subject>Volunteers</subject><subject>Wildlife</subject><issn>0004-8038</issn><issn>1938-4254</issn><issn>2732-4613</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2012</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>8G5</sourceid><sourceid>ABUWG</sourceid><sourceid>AFKRA</sourceid><sourceid>AZQEC</sourceid><sourceid>BEC</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>CCPQU</sourceid><sourceid>DWQXO</sourceid><sourceid>GNUQQ</sourceid><sourceid>GUQSH</sourceid><sourceid>M2O</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkE1Lw0AQhhdRsFavngMedeN-JU281VA_oFC0ehPCZDMLqTFbd5NC_71b493TMPC87wwPIZecxTwRyS0Mn7FgXMScc5EfkQnPZUaVSNQxmTDGFM2YzE7JmfebsCYsyyfkY1V5dDt00cIY1L2PoKuj-a6BjhbQtrSwQ9dH6yEw--hlgLbp93fRKzik6y3qBn1034DHMbgKTdp2pqmx03hOTgy0Hi_-5pS8Pyzeiie6XD0-F_MlrWTGespl-At0pYypE6ikUKrmcqZAQ8ow5UYITFllKtAIXEEqJQLUCjlAnhgjp-Rq7N06-z2g78uNHVwXTpac8VmqhEp5oOKR0s5679CUW9d8gdsHqDwYLIPB8mCw_DUYAtdjYON76_6nb0a6aqzt8D_8B6nJgHo</recordid><startdate>201201</startdate><enddate>201201</enddate><creator>Farmer, Robert G</creator><creator>Leonard, Marty L</creator><creator>Horn, Andrew G</creator><general>University of California Press</general><general>American Ornithological Society</general><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>7SN</scope><scope>7TN</scope><scope>7U9</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88A</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>88I</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FE</scope><scope>8FH</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>8G5</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BBNVY</scope><scope>BEC</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BHPHI</scope><scope>C1K</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>F1W</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>GUQSH</scope><scope>H94</scope><scope>H95</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>L.G</scope><scope>LK8</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>M2O</scope><scope>M2P</scope><scope>M7P</scope><scope>MBDVC</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PADUT</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>S0X</scope></search><sort><creationdate>201201</creationdate><title>Observer Effects and Avian-Call-Count Survey Quality: Rare-Species Biases and Overconfidence</title><author>Farmer, Robert G ; Leonard, Marty L ; Horn, Andrew G</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-b380t-13038acb4ffd5ab3244d1374aca60e61f22e60bfbacea14a633eaad4e1aa95ff3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2012</creationdate><topic>acoustic survey</topic><topic>Animal vocalization</topic><topic>Aviculture</topic><topic>bias</topic><topic>Bird songs</topic><topic>Birds</topic><topic>Breeding</topic><topic>call count surveys</topic><topic>citizen science</topic><topic>Data collection</topic><topic>detection</topic><topic>Error rates</topic><topic>False positive errors</topic><topic>Methods and Statistics</topic><topic>nondetection</topic><topic>Observational research</topic><topic>observer effects</topic><topic>Participant observation</topic><topic>Rare species</topic><topic>Studies</topic><topic>Volunteerism</topic><topic>Volunteers</topic><topic>Wildlife</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Farmer, Robert G</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Leonard, Marty L</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Horn, Andrew G</creatorcontrib><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>Ecology Abstracts</collection><collection>Oceanic Abstracts</collection><collection>Virology and AIDS Abstracts</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Biology Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>Science Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>ProQuest SciTech Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Research Library (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>eLibrary</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>ASFA: Aquatic Sciences and Fisheries Abstracts</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>Research Library Prep</collection><collection>AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts</collection><collection>Aquatic Science &amp; Fisheries Abstracts (ASFA) 1: Biological Sciences &amp; Living Resources</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection</collection><collection>Aquatic Science &amp; Fisheries Abstracts (ASFA) Professional</collection><collection>ProQuest Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>Psychology Database</collection><collection>Research Library</collection><collection>Science Database</collection><collection>Biological Science Database</collection><collection>Research Library (Corporate)</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>Research Library China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>SIRS Editorial</collection><jtitle>The Auk</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Farmer, Robert G</au><au>Leonard, Marty L</au><au>Horn, Andrew G</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Observer Effects and Avian-Call-Count Survey Quality: Rare-Species Biases and Overconfidence</atitle><jtitle>The Auk</jtitle><date>2012-01</date><risdate>2012</risdate><volume>129</volume><issue>1</issue><spage>76</spage><epage>86</epage><pages>76-86</pages><issn>0004-8038</issn><eissn>1938-4254</eissn><eissn>2732-4613</eissn><abstract>Wildlife monitoring surveys are prone to nondetection errors and false positives. To determine factors that affect the incidence of these errors, we built an Internet-based survey that simulated avian point counts, and measured error rates among volunteer observers. Using similar-sounding vocalizations from paired rare and common bird species, we measured the effects of species rarity and observer skill, and the influence of a reward system that explicitly encouraged the detection of rare species. Higher self-reported skill levels and common species independently predicted fewer nondetections (probability range: 0.11 [experts, common species] to 0.54 [moderates, rare species]). Overall proportions of detections that were false positives increased significantly as skill level declined (range: 0.06 [experts, common species] to 0.22 [moderates, rare species]). Moderately skilled observers were significantly more likely to report false-positive records of common species than of rare species, whereas experts were significantly more likely to report false-positives of rare species than of common species. The reward for correctly detecting rare species did not significantly affect these patterns. Because false positives can also result from observers overestimating their own abilities (“overconfidence”), we lastly tested whether observers' beliefs that they had recorded error-free data (“confidence”) tended to be incorrect (“overconfident”), and whether this pattern varied with skill. Observer confidence increased significantly with observer skill, whereas overconfidence was uniformly high (overall mean proportion = 0.73). Our results emphasize the value of controlling for observer skill in data collection and modeling and do not support the use of opinion-based (i.e., subjective) indications of observer confidence.</abstract><cop>Waco</cop><pub>University of California Press</pub><doi>10.1525/auk.2012.11129</doi><tpages>11</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0004-8038
ispartof The Auk, 2012-01, Vol.129 (1), p.76-86
issn 0004-8038
1938-4254
2732-4613
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_journals_1017642461
source JSTOR Archive Collection A-Z Listing; EZB-FREE-00999 freely available EZB journals
subjects acoustic survey
Animal vocalization
Aviculture
bias
Bird songs
Birds
Breeding
call count surveys
citizen science
Data collection
detection
Error rates
False positive errors
Methods and Statistics
nondetection
Observational research
observer effects
Participant observation
Rare species
Studies
Volunteerism
Volunteers
Wildlife
title Observer Effects and Avian-Call-Count Survey Quality: Rare-Species Biases and Overconfidence
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-29T06%3A37%3A00IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-jstor_proqu&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Observer%20Effects%20and%20Avian-Call-Count%20Survey%20Quality:%20Rare-Species%20Biases%20and%20Overconfidence&rft.jtitle=The%20Auk&rft.au=Farmer,%20Robert%20G&rft.date=2012-01&rft.volume=129&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=76&rft.epage=86&rft.pages=76-86&rft.issn=0004-8038&rft.eissn=1938-4254&rft_id=info:doi/10.1525/auk.2012.11129&rft_dat=%3Cjstor_proqu%3E10.1525/auk.2012.11129%3C/jstor_proqu%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1017642461&rft_id=info:pmid/&rft_jstor_id=10.1525/auk.2012.11129&rfr_iscdi=true