Visual form of ASL verb signs predicts non-signer judgment of transitivity

Longstanding cross-linguistic work on event representations in spoken languages have argued for a robust mapping between an event's underlying representation and its syntactic encoding, such that-for example-the agent of an event is most frequently mapped to subject position. In the same vein,...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:PloS one 2022-02, Vol.17 (2), p.e0262098
Hauptverfasser: Bradley, Chuck, Malaia, Evie A, Siskind, Jeffrey Mark, Wilbur, Ronnie B
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page
container_issue 2
container_start_page e0262098
container_title PloS one
container_volume 17
creator Bradley, Chuck
Malaia, Evie A
Siskind, Jeffrey Mark
Wilbur, Ronnie B
description Longstanding cross-linguistic work on event representations in spoken languages have argued for a robust mapping between an event's underlying representation and its syntactic encoding, such that-for example-the agent of an event is most frequently mapped to subject position. In the same vein, sign languages have long been claimed to construct signs that visually represent their meaning, i.e., signs that are iconic. Experimental research on linguistic parameters such as plurality and aspect has recently shown some of them to be visually universal in sign, i.e. recognized by non-signers as well as signers, and have identified specific visual cues that achieve this mapping. However, little is known about what makes action representations in sign language iconic, or whether and how the mapping of underlying event representations to syntactic encoding is visually apparent in the form of a verb sign. To this end, we asked what visual cues non-signers may use in evaluating transitivity (i.e., the number of entities involved in an action). To do this, we correlated non-signer judgments about transitivity of verb signs from American Sign Language (ASL) with phonological characteristics of these signs. We found that non-signers did not accurately guess the transitivity of the signs, but that non-signer transitivity judgments can nevertheless be predicted from the signs' visual characteristics. Further, non-signers cue in on just those features that code event representations across sign languages, despite interpreting them differently. This suggests the existence of visual biases that underlie detection of linguistic categories, such as transitivity, which may uncouple from underlying conceptual representations over time in mature sign languages due to lexicalization processes.
doi_str_mv 10.1371/journal.pone.0262098
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>gale_plos_</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_plos_journals_2633248579</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><galeid>A694957278</galeid><doaj_id>oai_doaj_org_article_6f9d0157749d4aa3b7c0aba1fab2fb84</doaj_id><sourcerecordid>A694957278</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c692t-63352412b0feedefd106f6741895e8af677f12ad45416a4c15ebf3010988ee193</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqNkl2L1DAUhoso7rr6D0QLgujFjPlqm9wIw-LHyMCCq3sb0vakk6FtxiQd3H9v6nSXqeyF9KLp6XPe0_f0TZKXGC0xLfCHnR1cr9rl3vawRCQnSPBHyTkWlCziA318cj5Lnnm_QyijPM-fJmc0I5hmGT9Pvt0YP6g21dZ1qdXp6nqTHsCVqTdN79O9g9pUwae97RdjCVy6G-qmgz6MeHCq9yaYgwm3z5MnWrUeXkz3i-Tn508_Lr8uNldf1perzaLKBQmLnMbpDJMSaYAadI1RrvOCYS4y4CoeC42JqlnGcK5YhTMoNUU42uMA0dJF8vqou2-tl9MavCRRmDCeFSOxPhK1VTu5d6ZT7lZaZeTfgnWNVC6YqgWZa1EjnBUFEzVTipZFhVSpsFYl0SVnUevjNG0oO6iraNypdiY6f9ObrWzsQXLOkUA0CrybBJz9NYAPsjO-grZVPdjh-N2cCcFwRN_8gz7sbqIaFQ2YXts4txpF5SoXTGQFKXiklg9Q8aqhM1XMjDaxPmt4P2uITIDfoVGD93J9_f3_2aubOfv2hN2CasPW23YIxvZ-DrIjWDnrvQN9v2SM5Bj5u23IMfJyinxse3X6g-6b7jJO_wDJhftE</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Website</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2633248579</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Visual form of ASL verb signs predicts non-signer judgment of transitivity</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</source><source>Elektronische Zeitschriftenbibliothek - Frei zugängliche E-Journals</source><source>PubMed Central</source><source>Free Full-Text Journals in Chemistry</source><source>Public Library of Science (PLoS)</source><creator>Bradley, Chuck ; Malaia, Evie A ; Siskind, Jeffrey Mark ; Wilbur, Ronnie B</creator><contributor>Perlman, Marcus</contributor><creatorcontrib>Bradley, Chuck ; Malaia, Evie A ; Siskind, Jeffrey Mark ; Wilbur, Ronnie B ; Perlman, Marcus</creatorcontrib><description>Longstanding cross-linguistic work on event representations in spoken languages have argued for a robust mapping between an event's underlying representation and its syntactic encoding, such that-for example-the agent of an event is most frequently mapped to subject position. In the same vein, sign languages have long been claimed to construct signs that visually represent their meaning, i.e., signs that are iconic. Experimental research on linguistic parameters such as plurality and aspect has recently shown some of them to be visually universal in sign, i.e. recognized by non-signers as well as signers, and have identified specific visual cues that achieve this mapping. However, little is known about what makes action representations in sign language iconic, or whether and how the mapping of underlying event representations to syntactic encoding is visually apparent in the form of a verb sign. To this end, we asked what visual cues non-signers may use in evaluating transitivity (i.e., the number of entities involved in an action). To do this, we correlated non-signer judgments about transitivity of verb signs from American Sign Language (ASL) with phonological characteristics of these signs. We found that non-signers did not accurately guess the transitivity of the signs, but that non-signer transitivity judgments can nevertheless be predicted from the signs' visual characteristics. Further, non-signers cue in on just those features that code event representations across sign languages, despite interpreting them differently. This suggests the existence of visual biases that underlie detection of linguistic categories, such as transitivity, which may uncouple from underlying conceptual representations over time in mature sign languages due to lexicalization processes.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0262098</identifier><identifier>PMID: 35213558</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: Public Library of Science</publisher><subject>American Sign Language ; Analysis ; Biology and Life Sciences ; Cues ; Deafness - physiopathology ; Deafness - prevention &amp; control ; Experimental research ; Female ; Fingers - physiology ; Hand - physiology ; Humans ; Interpreting ; Judgment ; Language ; Languages ; Lexicalization ; Linguistics ; Linguistics - trends ; Male ; Mapping ; Medicine and Health Sciences ; Morphology ; Phonology ; Physiological aspects ; Representations ; Sentence structure ; Sign Language ; Social Sciences ; Syntactic processing ; Thumb - physiology ; Valence ; Verbs ; Vision, Ocular - physiology ; Visual stimuli ; Visualization ; Word meaning</subject><ispartof>PloS one, 2022-02, Vol.17 (2), p.e0262098</ispartof><rights>COPYRIGHT 2022 Public Library of Science</rights><rights>2022 Bradley et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.</rights><rights>2022 Bradley et al 2022 Bradley et al</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c692t-63352412b0feedefd106f6741895e8af677f12ad45416a4c15ebf3010988ee193</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c692t-63352412b0feedefd106f6741895e8af677f12ad45416a4c15ebf3010988ee193</cites><orcidid>0000-0002-9695-1024</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8880903/pdf/$$EPDF$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$Hfree_for_read</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8880903/$$EHTML$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$Hfree_for_read</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,723,776,780,860,881,2096,2915,23845,27901,27902,53766,53768,79343,79344</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35213558$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><contributor>Perlman, Marcus</contributor><creatorcontrib>Bradley, Chuck</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Malaia, Evie A</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Siskind, Jeffrey Mark</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Wilbur, Ronnie B</creatorcontrib><title>Visual form of ASL verb signs predicts non-signer judgment of transitivity</title><title>PloS one</title><addtitle>PLoS One</addtitle><description>Longstanding cross-linguistic work on event representations in spoken languages have argued for a robust mapping between an event's underlying representation and its syntactic encoding, such that-for example-the agent of an event is most frequently mapped to subject position. In the same vein, sign languages have long been claimed to construct signs that visually represent their meaning, i.e., signs that are iconic. Experimental research on linguistic parameters such as plurality and aspect has recently shown some of them to be visually universal in sign, i.e. recognized by non-signers as well as signers, and have identified specific visual cues that achieve this mapping. However, little is known about what makes action representations in sign language iconic, or whether and how the mapping of underlying event representations to syntactic encoding is visually apparent in the form of a verb sign. To this end, we asked what visual cues non-signers may use in evaluating transitivity (i.e., the number of entities involved in an action). To do this, we correlated non-signer judgments about transitivity of verb signs from American Sign Language (ASL) with phonological characteristics of these signs. We found that non-signers did not accurately guess the transitivity of the signs, but that non-signer transitivity judgments can nevertheless be predicted from the signs' visual characteristics. Further, non-signers cue in on just those features that code event representations across sign languages, despite interpreting them differently. This suggests the existence of visual biases that underlie detection of linguistic categories, such as transitivity, which may uncouple from underlying conceptual representations over time in mature sign languages due to lexicalization processes.</description><subject>American Sign Language</subject><subject>Analysis</subject><subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject><subject>Cues</subject><subject>Deafness - physiopathology</subject><subject>Deafness - prevention &amp; control</subject><subject>Experimental research</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Fingers - physiology</subject><subject>Hand - physiology</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Interpreting</subject><subject>Judgment</subject><subject>Language</subject><subject>Languages</subject><subject>Lexicalization</subject><subject>Linguistics</subject><subject>Linguistics - trends</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Mapping</subject><subject>Medicine and Health Sciences</subject><subject>Morphology</subject><subject>Phonology</subject><subject>Physiological aspects</subject><subject>Representations</subject><subject>Sentence structure</subject><subject>Sign Language</subject><subject>Social Sciences</subject><subject>Syntactic processing</subject><subject>Thumb - physiology</subject><subject>Valence</subject><subject>Verbs</subject><subject>Vision, Ocular - physiology</subject><subject>Visual stimuli</subject><subject>Visualization</subject><subject>Word meaning</subject><issn>1932-6203</issn><issn>1932-6203</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2022</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>DOA</sourceid><recordid>eNqNkl2L1DAUhoso7rr6D0QLgujFjPlqm9wIw-LHyMCCq3sb0vakk6FtxiQd3H9v6nSXqeyF9KLp6XPe0_f0TZKXGC0xLfCHnR1cr9rl3vawRCQnSPBHyTkWlCziA318cj5Lnnm_QyijPM-fJmc0I5hmGT9Pvt0YP6g21dZ1qdXp6nqTHsCVqTdN79O9g9pUwae97RdjCVy6G-qmgz6MeHCq9yaYgwm3z5MnWrUeXkz3i-Tn508_Lr8uNldf1perzaLKBQmLnMbpDJMSaYAadI1RrvOCYS4y4CoeC42JqlnGcK5YhTMoNUU42uMA0dJF8vqou2-tl9MavCRRmDCeFSOxPhK1VTu5d6ZT7lZaZeTfgnWNVC6YqgWZa1EjnBUFEzVTipZFhVSpsFYl0SVnUevjNG0oO6iraNypdiY6f9ObrWzsQXLOkUA0CrybBJz9NYAPsjO-grZVPdjh-N2cCcFwRN_8gz7sbqIaFQ2YXts4txpF5SoXTGQFKXiklg9Q8aqhM1XMjDaxPmt4P2uITIDfoVGD93J9_f3_2aubOfv2hN2CasPW23YIxvZ-DrIjWDnrvQN9v2SM5Bj5u23IMfJyinxse3X6g-6b7jJO_wDJhftE</recordid><startdate>20220225</startdate><enddate>20220225</enddate><creator>Bradley, Chuck</creator><creator>Malaia, Evie A</creator><creator>Siskind, Jeffrey Mark</creator><creator>Wilbur, Ronnie B</creator><general>Public Library of Science</general><general>Public Library of Science (PLoS)</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>IOV</scope><scope>ISR</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>7QL</scope><scope>7QO</scope><scope>7RV</scope><scope>7SN</scope><scope>7SS</scope><scope>7T5</scope><scope>7T9</scope><scope>7TG</scope><scope>7TM</scope><scope>7U9</scope><scope>7X2</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8C1</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FE</scope><scope>8FG</scope><scope>8FH</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABJCF</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AEUYN</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ARAPS</scope><scope>ATCPS</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BBNVY</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BGLVJ</scope><scope>BHPHI</scope><scope>C1K</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>D1I</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>H94</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>KB.</scope><scope>KB0</scope><scope>KL.</scope><scope>L6V</scope><scope>LK8</scope><scope>M0K</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M7N</scope><scope>M7P</scope><scope>M7S</scope><scope>NAPCQ</scope><scope>P5Z</scope><scope>P62</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PATMY</scope><scope>PDBOC</scope><scope>PIMPY</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PTHSS</scope><scope>PYCSY</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope><scope>DOA</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-9695-1024</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>20220225</creationdate><title>Visual form of ASL verb signs predicts non-signer judgment of transitivity</title><author>Bradley, Chuck ; Malaia, Evie A ; Siskind, Jeffrey Mark ; Wilbur, Ronnie B</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c692t-63352412b0feedefd106f6741895e8af677f12ad45416a4c15ebf3010988ee193</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2022</creationdate><topic>American Sign Language</topic><topic>Analysis</topic><topic>Biology and Life Sciences</topic><topic>Cues</topic><topic>Deafness - physiopathology</topic><topic>Deafness - prevention &amp; control</topic><topic>Experimental research</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Fingers - physiology</topic><topic>Hand - physiology</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Interpreting</topic><topic>Judgment</topic><topic>Language</topic><topic>Languages</topic><topic>Lexicalization</topic><topic>Linguistics</topic><topic>Linguistics - trends</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Mapping</topic><topic>Medicine and Health Sciences</topic><topic>Morphology</topic><topic>Phonology</topic><topic>Physiological aspects</topic><topic>Representations</topic><topic>Sentence structure</topic><topic>Sign Language</topic><topic>Social Sciences</topic><topic>Syntactic processing</topic><topic>Thumb - physiology</topic><topic>Valence</topic><topic>Verbs</topic><topic>Vision, Ocular - physiology</topic><topic>Visual stimuli</topic><topic>Visualization</topic><topic>Word meaning</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Bradley, Chuck</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Malaia, Evie A</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Siskind, Jeffrey Mark</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Wilbur, Ronnie B</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Gale In Context: Opposing Viewpoints</collection><collection>Gale In Context: Science</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B)</collection><collection>Biotechnology Research Abstracts</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Database</collection><collection>Ecology Abstracts</collection><collection>Entomology Abstracts (Full archive)</collection><collection>Immunology Abstracts</collection><collection>Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)</collection><collection>Meteorological &amp; Geoastrophysical Abstracts</collection><collection>Nucleic Acids Abstracts</collection><collection>Virology and AIDS Abstracts</collection><collection>Agricultural Science Collection</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Public Health Database</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>ProQuest SciTech Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Technology Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Materials Science &amp; Engineering Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Sustainability</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies &amp; Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>Agricultural &amp; Environmental Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Technology Collection (ProQuest)</collection><collection>Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Materials Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Health &amp; Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Materials Science Database</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Meteorological &amp; Geoastrophysical Abstracts - Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest Engineering Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>Agricultural Science Database</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C)</collection><collection>Biological Science Database</collection><collection>Engineering Database</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Premium</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies &amp; Aerospace Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Advanced Technologies &amp; Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>Environmental Science Database</collection><collection>Materials Science Collection</collection><collection>Publicly Available Content Database</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>Engineering Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Science Collection</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><collection>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</collection><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Bradley, Chuck</au><au>Malaia, Evie A</au><au>Siskind, Jeffrey Mark</au><au>Wilbur, Ronnie B</au><au>Perlman, Marcus</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Visual form of ASL verb signs predicts non-signer judgment of transitivity</atitle><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle><addtitle>PLoS One</addtitle><date>2022-02-25</date><risdate>2022</risdate><volume>17</volume><issue>2</issue><spage>e0262098</spage><pages>e0262098-</pages><issn>1932-6203</issn><eissn>1932-6203</eissn><abstract>Longstanding cross-linguistic work on event representations in spoken languages have argued for a robust mapping between an event's underlying representation and its syntactic encoding, such that-for example-the agent of an event is most frequently mapped to subject position. In the same vein, sign languages have long been claimed to construct signs that visually represent their meaning, i.e., signs that are iconic. Experimental research on linguistic parameters such as plurality and aspect has recently shown some of them to be visually universal in sign, i.e. recognized by non-signers as well as signers, and have identified specific visual cues that achieve this mapping. However, little is known about what makes action representations in sign language iconic, or whether and how the mapping of underlying event representations to syntactic encoding is visually apparent in the form of a verb sign. To this end, we asked what visual cues non-signers may use in evaluating transitivity (i.e., the number of entities involved in an action). To do this, we correlated non-signer judgments about transitivity of verb signs from American Sign Language (ASL) with phonological characteristics of these signs. We found that non-signers did not accurately guess the transitivity of the signs, but that non-signer transitivity judgments can nevertheless be predicted from the signs' visual characteristics. Further, non-signers cue in on just those features that code event representations across sign languages, despite interpreting them differently. This suggests the existence of visual biases that underlie detection of linguistic categories, such as transitivity, which may uncouple from underlying conceptual representations over time in mature sign languages due to lexicalization processes.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>Public Library of Science</pub><pmid>35213558</pmid><doi>10.1371/journal.pone.0262098</doi><tpages>e0262098</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-9695-1024</orcidid><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 1932-6203
ispartof PloS one, 2022-02, Vol.17 (2), p.e0262098
issn 1932-6203
1932-6203
language eng
recordid cdi_plos_journals_2633248579
source MEDLINE; DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals; Elektronische Zeitschriftenbibliothek - Frei zugängliche E-Journals; PubMed Central; Free Full-Text Journals in Chemistry; Public Library of Science (PLoS)
subjects American Sign Language
Analysis
Biology and Life Sciences
Cues
Deafness - physiopathology
Deafness - prevention & control
Experimental research
Female
Fingers - physiology
Hand - physiology
Humans
Interpreting
Judgment
Language
Languages
Lexicalization
Linguistics
Linguistics - trends
Male
Mapping
Medicine and Health Sciences
Morphology
Phonology
Physiological aspects
Representations
Sentence structure
Sign Language
Social Sciences
Syntactic processing
Thumb - physiology
Valence
Verbs
Vision, Ocular - physiology
Visual stimuli
Visualization
Word meaning
title Visual form of ASL verb signs predicts non-signer judgment of transitivity
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-31T18%3A01%3A47IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-gale_plos_&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Visual%20form%20of%20ASL%20verb%20signs%20predicts%20non-signer%20judgment%20of%20transitivity&rft.jtitle=PloS%20one&rft.au=Bradley,%20Chuck&rft.date=2022-02-25&rft.volume=17&rft.issue=2&rft.spage=e0262098&rft.pages=e0262098-&rft.issn=1932-6203&rft.eissn=1932-6203&rft_id=info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0262098&rft_dat=%3Cgale_plos_%3EA694957278%3C/gale_plos_%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2633248579&rft_id=info:pmid/35213558&rft_galeid=A694957278&rft_doaj_id=oai_doaj_org_article_6f9d0157749d4aa3b7c0aba1fab2fb84&rfr_iscdi=true