The role of selective attention in the positivity offset: Evidence from event related potentials

Some research suggests that positive and negative valence stimuli may be processed differently. For example, negative material may capture and hold attention more readily than equally arousing positive material. This is called the negativity bias, and it has been observed as both behavioural and ele...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:PloS one 2021-11, Vol.16 (11), p.e0258640-e0258640
Hauptverfasser: Booy, Regard M, Carolan, Patrick L
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page e0258640
container_issue 11
container_start_page e0258640
container_title PloS one
container_volume 16
creator Booy, Regard M
Carolan, Patrick L
description Some research suggests that positive and negative valence stimuli may be processed differently. For example, negative material may capture and hold attention more readily than equally arousing positive material. This is called the negativity bias, and it has been observed as both behavioural and electroencephalographic (EEG) effects. Consequently, it has been attributed to both automatic and elaborative processes. However, at the lowest levels of arousal, faster reaction times and stronger EEG responses to positive material have been observed. This is called the positivity offset, and the underlying cognitive mechanism is less understood. To study the role of selective attention in the positivity offset, participants completed a negative affective priming (NAP) task modified to dissociate priming for positive and negative words. The task required participants to indicate the valence of a target word, while simultaneously ignoring a distractor. In experiment 1, a behavioural facilitation effect (faster response time) was observed for positive words, in stark contrast to the original NAP task. These results were congruent with a previously reported general categorization advantage for positive material. In experiment 2, participants performed the task while EEG was recorded. In additional to replicating the behavioural results from experiment 1, positive words elicited a larger Late Positive Potential (LPP) component on ignored repetition relative to control trials. Surprisingly, negative words elicited a larger LPP than positive words on control trials. These results suggest that the positivity offset may reflect a greater sensitivity to priming effects due to a more flexible attentional set.
doi_str_mv 10.1371/journal.pone.0258640
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>gale_plos_</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_plos_journals_2593006132</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><galeid>A681151539</galeid><doaj_id>oai_doaj_org_article_d0b4da3632264c6b942dd4de84d4617c</doaj_id><sourcerecordid>A681151539</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c692t-319f61943f430d6cb1f992d9f6fbcfacd5a33e1e4dc8812a246f90c092f240d43</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqNk99v0zAQxyMEYmPwHyCIhITgocW_4sY8IE3TgEqTJsHg1bj2ufXkxsV2Kvbf467Z1KA9oDw4On--38td7qrqJUZTTGf4w3XoY6f8dBM6mCLStJyhR9UxFpRMOEH08cH7UfUspWuEGtpy_rQ6omxGMUHsuPp1tYI6Bg91sHUCDzq7LdQqZ-iyC13tujoXZBOSKzcu3xTQJsgf6_OtM9BpqG0M6xq2RVBH8CqDKfitXvn0vHpiywEvhvOk-vH5_Ors6-Ti8sv87PRiorkgeUKxsBwLRi2jyHC9wFYIYkrQLrRV2jSKUsDAjG5bTBRh3AqkkSCWMGQYPale7303PiQ5NCdJ0giKEMeUFGK-J0xQ13IT3VrFGxmUk7eBEJdSxey0B2nQghlFOSWEM80XghFjmIGWGcbxTBevT0O2frEGo0uxUfmR6fimcyu5DFvZNryZEVEM3g0GMfzuIWW5dkmD96qD0O-_uxG8IbOCvvkHfbi6gVqqUoDrbCh59c5UnvIW4wY3dJd2-gBVHgNrp8skWVfiI8H7kaAwGf7kpepTkvPv3_6fvfw5Zt8esCtQPq9S8P1u5tIYZHtQx5BSBHvfZIzkbhHuuiF3iyCHRSiyV4c_6F50N_n0LyUPA2Q</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Website</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2593006132</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>The role of selective attention in the positivity offset: Evidence from event related potentials</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</source><source>Elektronische Zeitschriftenbibliothek - Frei zugängliche E-Journals</source><source>PubMed Central</source><source>Free Full-Text Journals in Chemistry</source><source>Public Library of Science (PLoS)</source><creator>Booy, Regard M ; Carolan, Patrick L</creator><creatorcontrib>Booy, Regard M ; Carolan, Patrick L</creatorcontrib><description>Some research suggests that positive and negative valence stimuli may be processed differently. For example, negative material may capture and hold attention more readily than equally arousing positive material. This is called the negativity bias, and it has been observed as both behavioural and electroencephalographic (EEG) effects. Consequently, it has been attributed to both automatic and elaborative processes. However, at the lowest levels of arousal, faster reaction times and stronger EEG responses to positive material have been observed. This is called the positivity offset, and the underlying cognitive mechanism is less understood. To study the role of selective attention in the positivity offset, participants completed a negative affective priming (NAP) task modified to dissociate priming for positive and negative words. The task required participants to indicate the valence of a target word, while simultaneously ignoring a distractor. In experiment 1, a behavioural facilitation effect (faster response time) was observed for positive words, in stark contrast to the original NAP task. These results were congruent with a previously reported general categorization advantage for positive material. In experiment 2, participants performed the task while EEG was recorded. In additional to replicating the behavioural results from experiment 1, positive words elicited a larger Late Positive Potential (LPP) component on ignored repetition relative to control trials. Surprisingly, negative words elicited a larger LPP than positive words on control trials. These results suggest that the positivity offset may reflect a greater sensitivity to priming effects due to a more flexible attentional set.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0258640</identifier><identifier>PMID: 34731204</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: Public Library of Science</publisher><subject>Adult ; Affect - physiology ; Analysis ; Arousal ; Arousal - physiology ; Attention - physiology ; Attentional bias ; Bias ; Biology and Life Sciences ; Cognition &amp; reasoning ; Cognition - physiology ; Cognitive ability ; EEG ; Electroencephalography ; Emotions ; Emotions - physiology ; Evaluation ; Event-related potentials ; Evoked Potentials - physiology ; Female ; Humans ; Medicine and Health Sciences ; Memory, Short-Term - physiology ; Photic Stimulation ; Priming ; Reaction Time - physiology ; Repetition Priming - physiology ; Research and Analysis Methods ; Response time ; Semantics ; Social Sciences ; Young Adult</subject><ispartof>PloS one, 2021-11, Vol.16 (11), p.e0258640-e0258640</ispartof><rights>COPYRIGHT 2021 Public Library of Science</rights><rights>2021 Booy, Carolan. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.</rights><rights>2021 Booy, Carolan 2021 Booy, Carolan</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c692t-319f61943f430d6cb1f992d9f6fbcfacd5a33e1e4dc8812a246f90c092f240d43</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c692t-319f61943f430d6cb1f992d9f6fbcfacd5a33e1e4dc8812a246f90c092f240d43</cites><orcidid>0000-0001-9150-3276</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8565729/pdf/$$EPDF$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$Hfree_for_read</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8565729/$$EHTML$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$Hfree_for_read</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,723,776,780,860,881,2096,2915,23845,27901,27902,53766,53768,79343,79344</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34731204$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Booy, Regard M</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Carolan, Patrick L</creatorcontrib><title>The role of selective attention in the positivity offset: Evidence from event related potentials</title><title>PloS one</title><addtitle>PLoS One</addtitle><description>Some research suggests that positive and negative valence stimuli may be processed differently. For example, negative material may capture and hold attention more readily than equally arousing positive material. This is called the negativity bias, and it has been observed as both behavioural and electroencephalographic (EEG) effects. Consequently, it has been attributed to both automatic and elaborative processes. However, at the lowest levels of arousal, faster reaction times and stronger EEG responses to positive material have been observed. This is called the positivity offset, and the underlying cognitive mechanism is less understood. To study the role of selective attention in the positivity offset, participants completed a negative affective priming (NAP) task modified to dissociate priming for positive and negative words. The task required participants to indicate the valence of a target word, while simultaneously ignoring a distractor. In experiment 1, a behavioural facilitation effect (faster response time) was observed for positive words, in stark contrast to the original NAP task. These results were congruent with a previously reported general categorization advantage for positive material. In experiment 2, participants performed the task while EEG was recorded. In additional to replicating the behavioural results from experiment 1, positive words elicited a larger Late Positive Potential (LPP) component on ignored repetition relative to control trials. Surprisingly, negative words elicited a larger LPP than positive words on control trials. These results suggest that the positivity offset may reflect a greater sensitivity to priming effects due to a more flexible attentional set.</description><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Affect - physiology</subject><subject>Analysis</subject><subject>Arousal</subject><subject>Arousal - physiology</subject><subject>Attention - physiology</subject><subject>Attentional bias</subject><subject>Bias</subject><subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject><subject>Cognition &amp; reasoning</subject><subject>Cognition - physiology</subject><subject>Cognitive ability</subject><subject>EEG</subject><subject>Electroencephalography</subject><subject>Emotions</subject><subject>Emotions - physiology</subject><subject>Evaluation</subject><subject>Event-related potentials</subject><subject>Evoked Potentials - physiology</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Medicine and Health Sciences</subject><subject>Memory, Short-Term - physiology</subject><subject>Photic Stimulation</subject><subject>Priming</subject><subject>Reaction Time - physiology</subject><subject>Repetition Priming - physiology</subject><subject>Research and Analysis Methods</subject><subject>Response time</subject><subject>Semantics</subject><subject>Social Sciences</subject><subject>Young Adult</subject><issn>1932-6203</issn><issn>1932-6203</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2021</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>DOA</sourceid><recordid>eNqNk99v0zAQxyMEYmPwHyCIhITgocW_4sY8IE3TgEqTJsHg1bj2ufXkxsV2Kvbf467Z1KA9oDw4On--38td7qrqJUZTTGf4w3XoY6f8dBM6mCLStJyhR9UxFpRMOEH08cH7UfUspWuEGtpy_rQ6omxGMUHsuPp1tYI6Bg91sHUCDzq7LdQqZ-iyC13tujoXZBOSKzcu3xTQJsgf6_OtM9BpqG0M6xq2RVBH8CqDKfitXvn0vHpiywEvhvOk-vH5_Ors6-Ti8sv87PRiorkgeUKxsBwLRi2jyHC9wFYIYkrQLrRV2jSKUsDAjG5bTBRh3AqkkSCWMGQYPale7303PiQ5NCdJ0giKEMeUFGK-J0xQ13IT3VrFGxmUk7eBEJdSxey0B2nQghlFOSWEM80XghFjmIGWGcbxTBevT0O2frEGo0uxUfmR6fimcyu5DFvZNryZEVEM3g0GMfzuIWW5dkmD96qD0O-_uxG8IbOCvvkHfbi6gVqqUoDrbCh59c5UnvIW4wY3dJd2-gBVHgNrp8skWVfiI8H7kaAwGf7kpepTkvPv3_6fvfw5Zt8esCtQPq9S8P1u5tIYZHtQx5BSBHvfZIzkbhHuuiF3iyCHRSiyV4c_6F50N_n0LyUPA2Q</recordid><startdate>20211103</startdate><enddate>20211103</enddate><creator>Booy, Regard M</creator><creator>Carolan, Patrick L</creator><general>Public Library of Science</general><general>Public Library of Science (PLoS)</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>IOV</scope><scope>ISR</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>7QL</scope><scope>7QO</scope><scope>7RV</scope><scope>7SN</scope><scope>7SS</scope><scope>7T5</scope><scope>7TG</scope><scope>7TM</scope><scope>7U9</scope><scope>7X2</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8C1</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FE</scope><scope>8FG</scope><scope>8FH</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABJCF</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AEUYN</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ARAPS</scope><scope>ATCPS</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BBNVY</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BGLVJ</scope><scope>BHPHI</scope><scope>C1K</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>D1I</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>H94</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>KB.</scope><scope>KB0</scope><scope>KL.</scope><scope>L6V</scope><scope>LK8</scope><scope>M0K</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M7N</scope><scope>M7P</scope><scope>M7S</scope><scope>NAPCQ</scope><scope>P5Z</scope><scope>P62</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PATMY</scope><scope>PDBOC</scope><scope>PIMPY</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PTHSS</scope><scope>PYCSY</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope><scope>DOA</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9150-3276</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>20211103</creationdate><title>The role of selective attention in the positivity offset: Evidence from event related potentials</title><author>Booy, Regard M ; Carolan, Patrick L</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c692t-319f61943f430d6cb1f992d9f6fbcfacd5a33e1e4dc8812a246f90c092f240d43</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2021</creationdate><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Affect - physiology</topic><topic>Analysis</topic><topic>Arousal</topic><topic>Arousal - physiology</topic><topic>Attention - physiology</topic><topic>Attentional bias</topic><topic>Bias</topic><topic>Biology and Life Sciences</topic><topic>Cognition &amp; reasoning</topic><topic>Cognition - physiology</topic><topic>Cognitive ability</topic><topic>EEG</topic><topic>Electroencephalography</topic><topic>Emotions</topic><topic>Emotions - physiology</topic><topic>Evaluation</topic><topic>Event-related potentials</topic><topic>Evoked Potentials - physiology</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Medicine and Health Sciences</topic><topic>Memory, Short-Term - physiology</topic><topic>Photic Stimulation</topic><topic>Priming</topic><topic>Reaction Time - physiology</topic><topic>Repetition Priming - physiology</topic><topic>Research and Analysis Methods</topic><topic>Response time</topic><topic>Semantics</topic><topic>Social Sciences</topic><topic>Young Adult</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Booy, Regard M</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Carolan, Patrick L</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Gale In Context: Opposing Viewpoints</collection><collection>Gale In Context: Science</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B)</collection><collection>Biotechnology Research Abstracts</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Database</collection><collection>Ecology Abstracts</collection><collection>Entomology Abstracts (Full archive)</collection><collection>Immunology Abstracts</collection><collection>Meteorological &amp; Geoastrophysical Abstracts</collection><collection>Nucleic Acids Abstracts</collection><collection>Virology and AIDS Abstracts</collection><collection>Agricultural Science Collection</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Public Health Database</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>ProQuest SciTech Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Technology Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Materials Science &amp; Engineering Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Sustainability</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies &amp; Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>Agricultural &amp; Environmental Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Technology Collection</collection><collection>Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Materials Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Health &amp; Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Materials Science Database</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Meteorological &amp; Geoastrophysical Abstracts - Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest Engineering Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>Agricultural Science Database</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C)</collection><collection>Biological Science Database</collection><collection>Engineering Database</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Premium</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies &amp; Aerospace Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Advanced Technologies &amp; Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>Environmental Science Database</collection><collection>Materials Science Collection</collection><collection>Publicly Available Content Database</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>Engineering Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Science Collection</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><collection>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</collection><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Booy, Regard M</au><au>Carolan, Patrick L</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>The role of selective attention in the positivity offset: Evidence from event related potentials</atitle><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle><addtitle>PLoS One</addtitle><date>2021-11-03</date><risdate>2021</risdate><volume>16</volume><issue>11</issue><spage>e0258640</spage><epage>e0258640</epage><pages>e0258640-e0258640</pages><issn>1932-6203</issn><eissn>1932-6203</eissn><abstract>Some research suggests that positive and negative valence stimuli may be processed differently. For example, negative material may capture and hold attention more readily than equally arousing positive material. This is called the negativity bias, and it has been observed as both behavioural and electroencephalographic (EEG) effects. Consequently, it has been attributed to both automatic and elaborative processes. However, at the lowest levels of arousal, faster reaction times and stronger EEG responses to positive material have been observed. This is called the positivity offset, and the underlying cognitive mechanism is less understood. To study the role of selective attention in the positivity offset, participants completed a negative affective priming (NAP) task modified to dissociate priming for positive and negative words. The task required participants to indicate the valence of a target word, while simultaneously ignoring a distractor. In experiment 1, a behavioural facilitation effect (faster response time) was observed for positive words, in stark contrast to the original NAP task. These results were congruent with a previously reported general categorization advantage for positive material. In experiment 2, participants performed the task while EEG was recorded. In additional to replicating the behavioural results from experiment 1, positive words elicited a larger Late Positive Potential (LPP) component on ignored repetition relative to control trials. Surprisingly, negative words elicited a larger LPP than positive words on control trials. These results suggest that the positivity offset may reflect a greater sensitivity to priming effects due to a more flexible attentional set.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>Public Library of Science</pub><pmid>34731204</pmid><doi>10.1371/journal.pone.0258640</doi><tpages>e0258640</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9150-3276</orcidid><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 1932-6203
ispartof PloS one, 2021-11, Vol.16 (11), p.e0258640-e0258640
issn 1932-6203
1932-6203
language eng
recordid cdi_plos_journals_2593006132
source MEDLINE; DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals; Elektronische Zeitschriftenbibliothek - Frei zugängliche E-Journals; PubMed Central; Free Full-Text Journals in Chemistry; Public Library of Science (PLoS)
subjects Adult
Affect - physiology
Analysis
Arousal
Arousal - physiology
Attention - physiology
Attentional bias
Bias
Biology and Life Sciences
Cognition & reasoning
Cognition - physiology
Cognitive ability
EEG
Electroencephalography
Emotions
Emotions - physiology
Evaluation
Event-related potentials
Evoked Potentials - physiology
Female
Humans
Medicine and Health Sciences
Memory, Short-Term - physiology
Photic Stimulation
Priming
Reaction Time - physiology
Repetition Priming - physiology
Research and Analysis Methods
Response time
Semantics
Social Sciences
Young Adult
title The role of selective attention in the positivity offset: Evidence from event related potentials
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-02T00%3A07%3A35IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-gale_plos_&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=The%20role%20of%20selective%20attention%20in%20the%20positivity%20offset:%20Evidence%20from%20event%20related%20potentials&rft.jtitle=PloS%20one&rft.au=Booy,%20Regard%20M&rft.date=2021-11-03&rft.volume=16&rft.issue=11&rft.spage=e0258640&rft.epage=e0258640&rft.pages=e0258640-e0258640&rft.issn=1932-6203&rft.eissn=1932-6203&rft_id=info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0258640&rft_dat=%3Cgale_plos_%3EA681151539%3C/gale_plos_%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2593006132&rft_id=info:pmid/34731204&rft_galeid=A681151539&rft_doaj_id=oai_doaj_org_article_d0b4da3632264c6b942dd4de84d4617c&rfr_iscdi=true