Cultural variation in young children's social motivation for peer collaboration and its relation to the ontogeny of Theory of Mind

Children seek and like to engage in collaborative activities with their peers. This social motivation is hypothesized to facilitate their emerging social-cognitive skills and vice versa. Current evidence on the ontogeny of social motivation and its' links to social cognition, however, is subjec...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:PloS one 2020-11, Vol.15 (11), p.e0242071-e0242071
Hauptverfasser: Stengelin, Roman, Hepach, Robert, Haun, Daniel B M
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page e0242071
container_issue 11
container_start_page e0242071
container_title PloS one
container_volume 15
creator Stengelin, Roman
Hepach, Robert
Haun, Daniel B M
description Children seek and like to engage in collaborative activities with their peers. This social motivation is hypothesized to facilitate their emerging social-cognitive skills and vice versa. Current evidence on the ontogeny of social motivation and its' links to social cognition, however, is subject to a sampling bias toward participants from urban Western populations. Here, we show both cross-cultural variation and homogeneity in three- to eight-year-old children's expressed positive emotions during and explicit preferences for peer collaboration across three diverse populations (urban German, rural Hai||om/Namibia, rural Ovambo/Namibia; n = 240). Children expressed more positive emotions during collaboration as compared to individual activity, but the extent varied across populations. Children's preferences for collaboration differed markedly between populations and across ages: While German children across all ages sought collaboration, Hai||om children preferred to act individually throughout childhood. Ovambo children preferred individual play increasingly with age. Across populations, positive emotions expressed selectively during collaboration, predicted children's social-cognitive skills. These findings provide evidence that culture shapes young children's social motivation for dyadic peer collaboration. At the same time, the positive relation of social motivation and social cognition in early ontogeny appears cross-culturally constant.
doi_str_mv 10.1371/journal.pone.0242071
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>gale_plos_</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_plos_journals_2462406370</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><galeid>A642270439</galeid><doaj_id>oai_doaj_org_article_6e0aa5fbfb7b4777b127efda23c0109e</doaj_id><sourcerecordid>A642270439</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c692t-24f49b84ad44feac925722db51872266662494bc4a33fe448e163d7dc150ab283</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqNk8uO0zAUhiMEYoaBN0BgCYnLosW3xskGaVRxqTRoJBjYWo5zkrhK7WI7Fd3y5LiXGTVoFtgLW8ff_9s-9smy5wRPCRPk_dIN3qp-unYWpphyigV5kJ2TktFJTjF7eDI_y56EsMR4xoo8f5ydMUYJEYScZ3_mQx8Hr3q0Ud6oaJxFxqKtG2yLdGf62oN9E1Bw2iRo5aLZHKjGebQG8Ei7vleV84ewsjUyMSAP_SEQHYodIGeja8FukWvQTQfO72dfja2fZo8a1Qd4dhwvsh-fPt7Mv0yurj8v5pdXE52XNE4ob3hZFVzVnDegdElngtK6mpEijXlqlJe80lwx1gDnBZCc1aLWZIZVRQt2kb08-K57F-QxfUFSnoQ4ZwInYnEgaqeWcu3NSvmtdMrIfcD5Viofje5B5oCVmjVVU4mKCyEqQgU0taJMY4JLSF4fjrsN1QpqDTamLI9MxyvWdLJ1Gyny1MnuMG-PBt79GiBEuTJBQ8q1BTfsz80IxgWZJfTVP-j9tztSrUoXMLZxaV-9M5WXOadUYM7KRE3voVKvYWV0-muNSfGR4N1IkJgIv2OrhhDk4vu3_2evf47Z1ydsB6qPXXD9sPtUYQzyA6i9C8FDc5dkguWuVG6zIXelIo-lkmQvTh_oTnRbG-wvxEkPgQ</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Website</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2462406370</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Cultural variation in young children's social motivation for peer collaboration and its relation to the ontogeny of Theory of Mind</title><source>Public Library of Science (PLoS) Journals Open Access</source><source>MEDLINE</source><source>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</source><source>EZB-FREE-00999 freely available EZB journals</source><source>PubMed Central</source><source>Free Full-Text Journals in Chemistry</source><source>Sociological Abstracts</source><creator>Stengelin, Roman ; Hepach, Robert ; Haun, Daniel B M</creator><contributor>Capraro, Valerio</contributor><creatorcontrib>Stengelin, Roman ; Hepach, Robert ; Haun, Daniel B M ; Capraro, Valerio</creatorcontrib><description>Children seek and like to engage in collaborative activities with their peers. This social motivation is hypothesized to facilitate their emerging social-cognitive skills and vice versa. Current evidence on the ontogeny of social motivation and its' links to social cognition, however, is subject to a sampling bias toward participants from urban Western populations. Here, we show both cross-cultural variation and homogeneity in three- to eight-year-old children's expressed positive emotions during and explicit preferences for peer collaboration across three diverse populations (urban German, rural Hai||om/Namibia, rural Ovambo/Namibia; n = 240). Children expressed more positive emotions during collaboration as compared to individual activity, but the extent varied across populations. Children's preferences for collaboration differed markedly between populations and across ages: While German children across all ages sought collaboration, Hai||om children preferred to act individually throughout childhood. Ovambo children preferred individual play increasingly with age. Across populations, positive emotions expressed selectively during collaboration, predicted children's social-cognitive skills. These findings provide evidence that culture shapes young children's social motivation for dyadic peer collaboration. At the same time, the positive relation of social motivation and social cognition in early ontogeny appears cross-culturally constant.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0242071</identifier><identifier>PMID: 33211711</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: Public Library of Science</publisher><subject>Adults ; Biology and Life Sciences ; Caregivers ; Child ; Child Development ; Child, Preschool ; Childhood ; Children ; Children &amp; youth ; Children's social skills ; Cognition ; Cognitive ability ; Collaboration ; Cultural Characteristics ; Cultural differences ; Culture ; Emotions ; Female ; Germany ; Homogeneity ; Humans ; Hunter-gatherers ; Male ; Motivation ; Motivation (Psychology) ; Multiculturalism &amp; pluralism ; Namibia ; Ontogeny ; Parents &amp; parenting ; Peer Group ; Peers ; People and Places ; Populations ; Psychological aspects ; Psychological research ; Psychology ; Rural Population ; Rural populations ; Skills ; Social aspects ; Social Behavior ; Social change ; Social interactions ; Social Sciences ; Socialization ; Theory of Mind ; Urban Population ; Urban populations</subject><ispartof>PloS one, 2020-11, Vol.15 (11), p.e0242071-e0242071</ispartof><rights>COPYRIGHT 2020 Public Library of Science</rights><rights>2020 Stengelin et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.</rights><rights>2020 Stengelin et al 2020 Stengelin et al</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c692t-24f49b84ad44feac925722db51872266662494bc4a33fe448e163d7dc150ab283</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c692t-24f49b84ad44feac925722db51872266662494bc4a33fe448e163d7dc150ab283</cites><orcidid>0000-0003-2212-4613</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7676710/pdf/$$EPDF$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$Hfree_for_read</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7676710/$$EHTML$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$Hfree_for_read</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,723,776,780,860,881,2096,2915,23847,27323,27903,27904,33753,53769,53771,79346,79347</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33211711$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><contributor>Capraro, Valerio</contributor><creatorcontrib>Stengelin, Roman</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Hepach, Robert</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Haun, Daniel B M</creatorcontrib><title>Cultural variation in young children's social motivation for peer collaboration and its relation to the ontogeny of Theory of Mind</title><title>PloS one</title><addtitle>PLoS One</addtitle><description>Children seek and like to engage in collaborative activities with their peers. This social motivation is hypothesized to facilitate their emerging social-cognitive skills and vice versa. Current evidence on the ontogeny of social motivation and its' links to social cognition, however, is subject to a sampling bias toward participants from urban Western populations. Here, we show both cross-cultural variation and homogeneity in three- to eight-year-old children's expressed positive emotions during and explicit preferences for peer collaboration across three diverse populations (urban German, rural Hai||om/Namibia, rural Ovambo/Namibia; n = 240). Children expressed more positive emotions during collaboration as compared to individual activity, but the extent varied across populations. Children's preferences for collaboration differed markedly between populations and across ages: While German children across all ages sought collaboration, Hai||om children preferred to act individually throughout childhood. Ovambo children preferred individual play increasingly with age. Across populations, positive emotions expressed selectively during collaboration, predicted children's social-cognitive skills. These findings provide evidence that culture shapes young children's social motivation for dyadic peer collaboration. At the same time, the positive relation of social motivation and social cognition in early ontogeny appears cross-culturally constant.</description><subject>Adults</subject><subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject><subject>Caregivers</subject><subject>Child</subject><subject>Child Development</subject><subject>Child, Preschool</subject><subject>Childhood</subject><subject>Children</subject><subject>Children &amp; youth</subject><subject>Children's social skills</subject><subject>Cognition</subject><subject>Cognitive ability</subject><subject>Collaboration</subject><subject>Cultural Characteristics</subject><subject>Cultural differences</subject><subject>Culture</subject><subject>Emotions</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Germany</subject><subject>Homogeneity</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Hunter-gatherers</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Motivation</subject><subject>Motivation (Psychology)</subject><subject>Multiculturalism &amp; pluralism</subject><subject>Namibia</subject><subject>Ontogeny</subject><subject>Parents &amp; parenting</subject><subject>Peer Group</subject><subject>Peers</subject><subject>People and Places</subject><subject>Populations</subject><subject>Psychological aspects</subject><subject>Psychological research</subject><subject>Psychology</subject><subject>Rural Population</subject><subject>Rural populations</subject><subject>Skills</subject><subject>Social aspects</subject><subject>Social Behavior</subject><subject>Social change</subject><subject>Social interactions</subject><subject>Social Sciences</subject><subject>Socialization</subject><subject>Theory of Mind</subject><subject>Urban Population</subject><subject>Urban populations</subject><issn>1932-6203</issn><issn>1932-6203</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2020</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>BHHNA</sourceid><sourceid>DOA</sourceid><recordid>eNqNk8uO0zAUhiMEYoaBN0BgCYnLosW3xskGaVRxqTRoJBjYWo5zkrhK7WI7Fd3y5LiXGTVoFtgLW8ff_9s-9smy5wRPCRPk_dIN3qp-unYWpphyigV5kJ2TktFJTjF7eDI_y56EsMR4xoo8f5ydMUYJEYScZ3_mQx8Hr3q0Ud6oaJxFxqKtG2yLdGf62oN9E1Bw2iRo5aLZHKjGebQG8Ei7vleV84ewsjUyMSAP_SEQHYodIGeja8FukWvQTQfO72dfja2fZo8a1Qd4dhwvsh-fPt7Mv0yurj8v5pdXE52XNE4ob3hZFVzVnDegdElngtK6mpEijXlqlJe80lwx1gDnBZCc1aLWZIZVRQt2kb08-K57F-QxfUFSnoQ4ZwInYnEgaqeWcu3NSvmtdMrIfcD5Viofje5B5oCVmjVVU4mKCyEqQgU0taJMY4JLSF4fjrsN1QpqDTamLI9MxyvWdLJ1Gyny1MnuMG-PBt79GiBEuTJBQ8q1BTfsz80IxgWZJfTVP-j9tztSrUoXMLZxaV-9M5WXOadUYM7KRE3voVKvYWV0-muNSfGR4N1IkJgIv2OrhhDk4vu3_2evf47Z1ydsB6qPXXD9sPtUYQzyA6i9C8FDc5dkguWuVG6zIXelIo-lkmQvTh_oTnRbG-wvxEkPgQ</recordid><startdate>20201119</startdate><enddate>20201119</enddate><creator>Stengelin, Roman</creator><creator>Hepach, Robert</creator><creator>Haun, Daniel B M</creator><general>Public Library of Science</general><general>Public Library of Science (PLoS)</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>IOV</scope><scope>ISR</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>7QL</scope><scope>7QO</scope><scope>7RV</scope><scope>7SN</scope><scope>7SS</scope><scope>7T5</scope><scope>7TG</scope><scope>7TM</scope><scope>7U4</scope><scope>7U9</scope><scope>7X2</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8C1</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FE</scope><scope>8FG</scope><scope>8FH</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABJCF</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AEUYN</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ARAPS</scope><scope>ATCPS</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BBNVY</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BGLVJ</scope><scope>BHHNA</scope><scope>BHPHI</scope><scope>C1K</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>D1I</scope><scope>DWI</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>H94</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>KB.</scope><scope>KB0</scope><scope>KL.</scope><scope>L6V</scope><scope>LK8</scope><scope>M0K</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M7N</scope><scope>M7P</scope><scope>M7S</scope><scope>NAPCQ</scope><scope>P5Z</scope><scope>P62</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PATMY</scope><scope>PDBOC</scope><scope>PIMPY</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PTHSS</scope><scope>PYCSY</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>WZK</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope><scope>DOA</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-2212-4613</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>20201119</creationdate><title>Cultural variation in young children's social motivation for peer collaboration and its relation to the ontogeny of Theory of Mind</title><author>Stengelin, Roman ; Hepach, Robert ; Haun, Daniel B M</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c692t-24f49b84ad44feac925722db51872266662494bc4a33fe448e163d7dc150ab283</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2020</creationdate><topic>Adults</topic><topic>Biology and Life Sciences</topic><topic>Caregivers</topic><topic>Child</topic><topic>Child Development</topic><topic>Child, Preschool</topic><topic>Childhood</topic><topic>Children</topic><topic>Children &amp; youth</topic><topic>Children's social skills</topic><topic>Cognition</topic><topic>Cognitive ability</topic><topic>Collaboration</topic><topic>Cultural Characteristics</topic><topic>Cultural differences</topic><topic>Culture</topic><topic>Emotions</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Germany</topic><topic>Homogeneity</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Hunter-gatherers</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Motivation</topic><topic>Motivation (Psychology)</topic><topic>Multiculturalism &amp; pluralism</topic><topic>Namibia</topic><topic>Ontogeny</topic><topic>Parents &amp; parenting</topic><topic>Peer Group</topic><topic>Peers</topic><topic>People and Places</topic><topic>Populations</topic><topic>Psychological aspects</topic><topic>Psychological research</topic><topic>Psychology</topic><topic>Rural Population</topic><topic>Rural populations</topic><topic>Skills</topic><topic>Social aspects</topic><topic>Social Behavior</topic><topic>Social change</topic><topic>Social interactions</topic><topic>Social Sciences</topic><topic>Socialization</topic><topic>Theory of Mind</topic><topic>Urban Population</topic><topic>Urban populations</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Stengelin, Roman</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Hepach, Robert</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Haun, Daniel B M</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Gale In Context: Opposing Viewpoints</collection><collection>Gale In Context: Science</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B)</collection><collection>Biotechnology Research Abstracts</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Database</collection><collection>Ecology Abstracts</collection><collection>Entomology Abstracts (Full archive)</collection><collection>Immunology Abstracts</collection><collection>Meteorological &amp; Geoastrophysical Abstracts</collection><collection>Nucleic Acids Abstracts</collection><collection>Sociological Abstracts (pre-2017)</collection><collection>Virology and AIDS Abstracts</collection><collection>Agricultural Science Collection</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Public Health Database</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>ProQuest SciTech Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Technology Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Materials Science &amp; Engineering Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Sustainability</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies &amp; Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>Agricultural &amp; Environmental Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Technology Collection</collection><collection>Sociological Abstracts</collection><collection>Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Materials Science Collection</collection><collection>Sociological Abstracts</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Health &amp; Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Materials Science Database</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Meteorological &amp; Geoastrophysical Abstracts - Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest Engineering Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>Agricultural Science Database</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C)</collection><collection>Biological Science Database</collection><collection>Engineering Database</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Premium</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies &amp; Aerospace Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Advanced Technologies &amp; Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>Environmental Science Database</collection><collection>Materials Science Collection</collection><collection>Publicly Available Content Database</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>Engineering Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Science Collection</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>Sociological Abstracts (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><collection>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</collection><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Stengelin, Roman</au><au>Hepach, Robert</au><au>Haun, Daniel B M</au><au>Capraro, Valerio</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Cultural variation in young children's social motivation for peer collaboration and its relation to the ontogeny of Theory of Mind</atitle><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle><addtitle>PLoS One</addtitle><date>2020-11-19</date><risdate>2020</risdate><volume>15</volume><issue>11</issue><spage>e0242071</spage><epage>e0242071</epage><pages>e0242071-e0242071</pages><issn>1932-6203</issn><eissn>1932-6203</eissn><abstract>Children seek and like to engage in collaborative activities with their peers. This social motivation is hypothesized to facilitate their emerging social-cognitive skills and vice versa. Current evidence on the ontogeny of social motivation and its' links to social cognition, however, is subject to a sampling bias toward participants from urban Western populations. Here, we show both cross-cultural variation and homogeneity in three- to eight-year-old children's expressed positive emotions during and explicit preferences for peer collaboration across three diverse populations (urban German, rural Hai||om/Namibia, rural Ovambo/Namibia; n = 240). Children expressed more positive emotions during collaboration as compared to individual activity, but the extent varied across populations. Children's preferences for collaboration differed markedly between populations and across ages: While German children across all ages sought collaboration, Hai||om children preferred to act individually throughout childhood. Ovambo children preferred individual play increasingly with age. Across populations, positive emotions expressed selectively during collaboration, predicted children's social-cognitive skills. These findings provide evidence that culture shapes young children's social motivation for dyadic peer collaboration. At the same time, the positive relation of social motivation and social cognition in early ontogeny appears cross-culturally constant.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>Public Library of Science</pub><pmid>33211711</pmid><doi>10.1371/journal.pone.0242071</doi><tpages>e0242071</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-2212-4613</orcidid><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 1932-6203
ispartof PloS one, 2020-11, Vol.15 (11), p.e0242071-e0242071
issn 1932-6203
1932-6203
language eng
recordid cdi_plos_journals_2462406370
source Public Library of Science (PLoS) Journals Open Access; MEDLINE; DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals; EZB-FREE-00999 freely available EZB journals; PubMed Central; Free Full-Text Journals in Chemistry; Sociological Abstracts
subjects Adults
Biology and Life Sciences
Caregivers
Child
Child Development
Child, Preschool
Childhood
Children
Children & youth
Children's social skills
Cognition
Cognitive ability
Collaboration
Cultural Characteristics
Cultural differences
Culture
Emotions
Female
Germany
Homogeneity
Humans
Hunter-gatherers
Male
Motivation
Motivation (Psychology)
Multiculturalism & pluralism
Namibia
Ontogeny
Parents & parenting
Peer Group
Peers
People and Places
Populations
Psychological aspects
Psychological research
Psychology
Rural Population
Rural populations
Skills
Social aspects
Social Behavior
Social change
Social interactions
Social Sciences
Socialization
Theory of Mind
Urban Population
Urban populations
title Cultural variation in young children's social motivation for peer collaboration and its relation to the ontogeny of Theory of Mind
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-27T10%3A57%3A25IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-gale_plos_&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Cultural%20variation%20in%20young%20children's%20social%20motivation%20for%20peer%20collaboration%20and%20its%20relation%20to%20the%20ontogeny%20of%20Theory%20of%20Mind&rft.jtitle=PloS%20one&rft.au=Stengelin,%20Roman&rft.date=2020-11-19&rft.volume=15&rft.issue=11&rft.spage=e0242071&rft.epage=e0242071&rft.pages=e0242071-e0242071&rft.issn=1932-6203&rft.eissn=1932-6203&rft_id=info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0242071&rft_dat=%3Cgale_plos_%3EA642270439%3C/gale_plos_%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2462406370&rft_id=info:pmid/33211711&rft_galeid=A642270439&rft_doaj_id=oai_doaj_org_article_6e0aa5fbfb7b4777b127efda23c0109e&rfr_iscdi=true