The effect of language on prosocial behaviors in preschool children
The present study investigated how linguistic group membership influences prosocial behaviors, namely helpfulness and cooperation, in preschool children. Whilst research indicates that children preferentially direct their prosocial behavior towards members of their own groups, the influence of perce...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | PloS one 2020-10, Vol.15 (10), p.e0240028-e0240028 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | e0240028 |
---|---|
container_issue | 10 |
container_start_page | e0240028 |
container_title | PloS one |
container_volume | 15 |
creator | Somogyi, Eszter Tran, Thuy Tuong Uyen Guellai, Bahia Király, Ildikó Esseily, Rana |
description | The present study investigated how linguistic group membership influences prosocial behaviors, namely helpfulness and cooperation, in preschool children. Whilst research indicates that children preferentially direct their prosocial behavior towards members of their own groups, the influence of perceived linguistic group membership on actual helpfulness and cooperation has not been investigated. We presented an experimenter to 4- and 5-year-olds either as a foreigner, who did not speak the local language or as a native person. Children were then given the opportunity to help or cooperate with this experimenter in a series of nonverbal playful tasks. Whilst 4-year-olds helped and cooperated equally with the foreign and the native experimenter, 5-year-olds required significantly more cues and prompts in order to help or cooperate in the foreign condition. We also found that children were overall more reluctant to respond prosocially in the cooperation tasks than in the helping tasks. We tested children in two European countries (France and Hungary) and found the same pattern of responses in the two locations, suggesting that our findings are not specific to the local culture. Our results extend the findings of earlier research that showed selectivity according to the language spoken by the partner for sharing and imitation. Studies that looked at helpfulness or cooperation used the minimal group paradigm to induce group membership (based on arbitrary cues) and used indirect measures of prosociality, such as different forms of reasoning about the partner. In our study, we used language, a natural cue for group membership (versus arbitrary cues or cues based on social conventions) and directly observed children's helpful and cooperative behaviors toward the experimenter. Our results also confirm previous results indicating that with age, children become selective in their prosocial behaviors as they acquire new means of social evaluation and categorization. We conclude that the language associated with a potential social partner is not only a cue for affiliation and shared knowledge but also a cue mediating children's prosocial acts. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1371/journal.pone.0240028 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>gale_plos_</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_plos_journals_2448835115</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><galeid>A637613600</galeid><doaj_id>oai_doaj_org_article_65cdd9f7eb674b86a6fbdc657d74c9fb</doaj_id><sourcerecordid>A637613600</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c726t-5d5b88e85376ef34dfe1471cd6f5484db4734593bf3598e607c61c8d28dfe9043</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqNk11r2zAUhs3YWLtu_2BshsFYL5JJlizJN4MQtjUQKGzdboUsHcUKjpVadtj-_eTGLXHpxbDA4ug5r86HTpK8xWiOCceft75vG1XP976BOcooQpl4lpzjgmQzliHy_GR_lrwKYYtQTgRjL5MzQlCWxXWeLG8qSMFa0F3qbVqrZtOrDaS-SfetD147VaclVOrgfBtSN5gh6Mr7OtWVq00LzevkhVV1gDfj_yL59e3rzfJqtr7-vlou1jPNM9bNcpOXQoDICWdgCTUWMOVYG2ZzKqgpKSc0L0hpSV4IYIhrhrUwmYhkgSi5SN4fdfe1D3LMP8iMUiFIjnEeidWRMF5t5b51O9X-lV45eWfw7UaqtnO6BslybUxhOZSM01IwxWxpNMu54VQXtoxaX8bb-nIHRkPTtaqeiE5PGlfJjT9IHhMUYgjm8ihQPXK7WqzlYEMkxsxpccCR_TRe1vrbHkIndy5oqGM_wPd3ORaY84KIiH54hD5diZHaqJisa6yPMepBVC5YbAAmDKFIzZ-g4mdg53R8WNZF-8ThcuIQmQ7-dBvVhyBXP3_8P3v9e8p-PGErUHVXBV_3nfNNmIL0COr4OkML9qGyGMlhLu6rIYe5kONcRLd3p818cLofBPIPyiUGEQ</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Website</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2448835115</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>The effect of language on prosocial behaviors in preschool children</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</source><source>Elektronische Zeitschriftenbibliothek - Frei zugängliche E-Journals</source><source>Public Library of Science (PLoS)</source><source>PubMed Central</source><source>Free Full-Text Journals in Chemistry</source><creator>Somogyi, Eszter ; Tran, Thuy Tuong Uyen ; Guellai, Bahia ; Király, Ildikó ; Esseily, Rana</creator><contributor>Capraro, Valerio</contributor><creatorcontrib>Somogyi, Eszter ; Tran, Thuy Tuong Uyen ; Guellai, Bahia ; Király, Ildikó ; Esseily, Rana ; Capraro, Valerio</creatorcontrib><description>The present study investigated how linguistic group membership influences prosocial behaviors, namely helpfulness and cooperation, in preschool children. Whilst research indicates that children preferentially direct their prosocial behavior towards members of their own groups, the influence of perceived linguistic group membership on actual helpfulness and cooperation has not been investigated. We presented an experimenter to 4- and 5-year-olds either as a foreigner, who did not speak the local language or as a native person. Children were then given the opportunity to help or cooperate with this experimenter in a series of nonverbal playful tasks. Whilst 4-year-olds helped and cooperated equally with the foreign and the native experimenter, 5-year-olds required significantly more cues and prompts in order to help or cooperate in the foreign condition. We also found that children were overall more reluctant to respond prosocially in the cooperation tasks than in the helping tasks. We tested children in two European countries (France and Hungary) and found the same pattern of responses in the two locations, suggesting that our findings are not specific to the local culture. Our results extend the findings of earlier research that showed selectivity according to the language spoken by the partner for sharing and imitation. Studies that looked at helpfulness or cooperation used the minimal group paradigm to induce group membership (based on arbitrary cues) and used indirect measures of prosociality, such as different forms of reasoning about the partner. In our study, we used language, a natural cue for group membership (versus arbitrary cues or cues based on social conventions) and directly observed children's helpful and cooperative behaviors toward the experimenter. Our results also confirm previous results indicating that with age, children become selective in their prosocial behaviors as they acquire new means of social evaluation and categorization. We conclude that the language associated with a potential social partner is not only a cue for affiliation and shared knowledge but also a cue mediating children's prosocial acts.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0240028</identifier><identifier>PMID: 33022022</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: Public Library of Science</publisher><subject>Behavior ; Biology and Life Sciences ; Child Behavior ; Child, Preschool ; Children ; Cooperation ; Cooperative Behavior ; Female ; France ; Gender ; Helping behavior ; Humanities and Social Sciences ; Humans ; Hungary ; Language ; Language and languages ; Learning ; Linguistic research ; Local culture ; Male ; Peers ; People and Places ; Preschool children ; Psycholinguistics ; Psychological aspects ; Selectivity ; Social aspects ; Social Behavior ; Social Sciences ; Task Performance and Analysis</subject><ispartof>PloS one, 2020-10, Vol.15 (10), p.e0240028-e0240028</ispartof><rights>COPYRIGHT 2020 Public Library of Science</rights><rights>2020 Somogyi et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.</rights><rights>Distributed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License</rights><rights>2020 Somogyi et al 2020 Somogyi et al</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c726t-5d5b88e85376ef34dfe1471cd6f5484db4734593bf3598e607c61c8d28dfe9043</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c726t-5d5b88e85376ef34dfe1471cd6f5484db4734593bf3598e607c61c8d28dfe9043</cites><orcidid>0000-0002-3935-3933 ; 0000-0002-1817-6202</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7537885/pdf/$$EPDF$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$Hfree_for_read</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7537885/$$EHTML$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$Hfree_for_read</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,727,780,784,864,885,2100,2926,23865,27923,27924,53790,53792,79371,79372</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33022022$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://hal.parisnanterre.fr/hal-03115749$$DView record in HAL$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><contributor>Capraro, Valerio</contributor><creatorcontrib>Somogyi, Eszter</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Tran, Thuy Tuong Uyen</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Guellai, Bahia</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Király, Ildikó</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Esseily, Rana</creatorcontrib><title>The effect of language on prosocial behaviors in preschool children</title><title>PloS one</title><addtitle>PLoS One</addtitle><description>The present study investigated how linguistic group membership influences prosocial behaviors, namely helpfulness and cooperation, in preschool children. Whilst research indicates that children preferentially direct their prosocial behavior towards members of their own groups, the influence of perceived linguistic group membership on actual helpfulness and cooperation has not been investigated. We presented an experimenter to 4- and 5-year-olds either as a foreigner, who did not speak the local language or as a native person. Children were then given the opportunity to help or cooperate with this experimenter in a series of nonverbal playful tasks. Whilst 4-year-olds helped and cooperated equally with the foreign and the native experimenter, 5-year-olds required significantly more cues and prompts in order to help or cooperate in the foreign condition. We also found that children were overall more reluctant to respond prosocially in the cooperation tasks than in the helping tasks. We tested children in two European countries (France and Hungary) and found the same pattern of responses in the two locations, suggesting that our findings are not specific to the local culture. Our results extend the findings of earlier research that showed selectivity according to the language spoken by the partner for sharing and imitation. Studies that looked at helpfulness or cooperation used the minimal group paradigm to induce group membership (based on arbitrary cues) and used indirect measures of prosociality, such as different forms of reasoning about the partner. In our study, we used language, a natural cue for group membership (versus arbitrary cues or cues based on social conventions) and directly observed children's helpful and cooperative behaviors toward the experimenter. Our results also confirm previous results indicating that with age, children become selective in their prosocial behaviors as they acquire new means of social evaluation and categorization. We conclude that the language associated with a potential social partner is not only a cue for affiliation and shared knowledge but also a cue mediating children's prosocial acts.</description><subject>Behavior</subject><subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject><subject>Child Behavior</subject><subject>Child, Preschool</subject><subject>Children</subject><subject>Cooperation</subject><subject>Cooperative Behavior</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>France</subject><subject>Gender</subject><subject>Helping behavior</subject><subject>Humanities and Social Sciences</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Hungary</subject><subject>Language</subject><subject>Language and languages</subject><subject>Learning</subject><subject>Linguistic research</subject><subject>Local culture</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Peers</subject><subject>People and Places</subject><subject>Preschool children</subject><subject>Psycholinguistics</subject><subject>Psychological aspects</subject><subject>Selectivity</subject><subject>Social aspects</subject><subject>Social Behavior</subject><subject>Social Sciences</subject><subject>Task Performance and Analysis</subject><issn>1932-6203</issn><issn>1932-6203</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2020</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>ABUWG</sourceid><sourceid>AFKRA</sourceid><sourceid>AZQEC</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>CCPQU</sourceid><sourceid>DWQXO</sourceid><sourceid>GNUQQ</sourceid><sourceid>DOA</sourceid><recordid>eNqNk11r2zAUhs3YWLtu_2BshsFYL5JJlizJN4MQtjUQKGzdboUsHcUKjpVadtj-_eTGLXHpxbDA4ug5r86HTpK8xWiOCceft75vG1XP976BOcooQpl4lpzjgmQzliHy_GR_lrwKYYtQTgRjL5MzQlCWxXWeLG8qSMFa0F3qbVqrZtOrDaS-SfetD147VaclVOrgfBtSN5gh6Mr7OtWVq00LzevkhVV1gDfj_yL59e3rzfJqtr7-vlou1jPNM9bNcpOXQoDICWdgCTUWMOVYG2ZzKqgpKSc0L0hpSV4IYIhrhrUwmYhkgSi5SN4fdfe1D3LMP8iMUiFIjnEeidWRMF5t5b51O9X-lV45eWfw7UaqtnO6BslybUxhOZSM01IwxWxpNMu54VQXtoxaX8bb-nIHRkPTtaqeiE5PGlfJjT9IHhMUYgjm8ihQPXK7WqzlYEMkxsxpccCR_TRe1vrbHkIndy5oqGM_wPd3ORaY84KIiH54hD5diZHaqJisa6yPMepBVC5YbAAmDKFIzZ-g4mdg53R8WNZF-8ThcuIQmQ7-dBvVhyBXP3_8P3v9e8p-PGErUHVXBV_3nfNNmIL0COr4OkML9qGyGMlhLu6rIYe5kONcRLd3p818cLofBPIPyiUGEQ</recordid><startdate>20201006</startdate><enddate>20201006</enddate><creator>Somogyi, Eszter</creator><creator>Tran, Thuy Tuong Uyen</creator><creator>Guellai, Bahia</creator><creator>Király, Ildikó</creator><creator>Esseily, Rana</creator><general>Public Library of Science</general><general>Public Library of Science (PLoS)</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>IOV</scope><scope>ISR</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>7QL</scope><scope>7QO</scope><scope>7RV</scope><scope>7SN</scope><scope>7SS</scope><scope>7T5</scope><scope>7TG</scope><scope>7TM</scope><scope>7U9</scope><scope>7X2</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8C1</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FE</scope><scope>8FG</scope><scope>8FH</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABJCF</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AEUYN</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ARAPS</scope><scope>ATCPS</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BBNVY</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BGLVJ</scope><scope>BHPHI</scope><scope>C1K</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>D1I</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>H94</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>KB.</scope><scope>KB0</scope><scope>KL.</scope><scope>L6V</scope><scope>LK8</scope><scope>M0K</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M7N</scope><scope>M7P</scope><scope>M7S</scope><scope>NAPCQ</scope><scope>P5Z</scope><scope>P62</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PATMY</scope><scope>PDBOC</scope><scope>PIMPY</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PTHSS</scope><scope>PYCSY</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>1XC</scope><scope>BXJBU</scope><scope>IHQJB</scope><scope>VOOES</scope><scope>5PM</scope><scope>DOA</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-3935-3933</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1817-6202</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>20201006</creationdate><title>The effect of language on prosocial behaviors in preschool children</title><author>Somogyi, Eszter ; Tran, Thuy Tuong Uyen ; Guellai, Bahia ; Király, Ildikó ; Esseily, Rana</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c726t-5d5b88e85376ef34dfe1471cd6f5484db4734593bf3598e607c61c8d28dfe9043</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2020</creationdate><topic>Behavior</topic><topic>Biology and Life Sciences</topic><topic>Child Behavior</topic><topic>Child, Preschool</topic><topic>Children</topic><topic>Cooperation</topic><topic>Cooperative Behavior</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>France</topic><topic>Gender</topic><topic>Helping behavior</topic><topic>Humanities and Social Sciences</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Hungary</topic><topic>Language</topic><topic>Language and languages</topic><topic>Learning</topic><topic>Linguistic research</topic><topic>Local culture</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Peers</topic><topic>People and Places</topic><topic>Preschool children</topic><topic>Psycholinguistics</topic><topic>Psychological aspects</topic><topic>Selectivity</topic><topic>Social aspects</topic><topic>Social Behavior</topic><topic>Social Sciences</topic><topic>Task Performance and Analysis</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Somogyi, Eszter</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Tran, Thuy Tuong Uyen</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Guellai, Bahia</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Király, Ildikó</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Esseily, Rana</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Gale In Context: Opposing Viewpoints</collection><collection>Gale In Context: Science</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B)</collection><collection>Biotechnology Research Abstracts</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Database</collection><collection>Ecology Abstracts</collection><collection>Entomology Abstracts (Full archive)</collection><collection>Immunology Abstracts</collection><collection>Meteorological & Geoastrophysical Abstracts</collection><collection>Nucleic Acids Abstracts</collection><collection>Virology and AIDS Abstracts</collection><collection>Agricultural Science Collection</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Public Health Database</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>ProQuest SciTech Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Technology Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Materials Science & Engineering Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Sustainability</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>Agricultural & Environmental Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Technology Collection</collection><collection>Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Materials Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Materials Science Database</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Meteorological & Geoastrophysical Abstracts - Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest Engineering Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>Agricultural Science Database</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C)</collection><collection>Biological Science Database</collection><collection>Engineering Database</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Premium</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>Environmental Science Database</collection><collection>Materials Science Collection</collection><collection>Publicly Available Content Database</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>Engineering Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Science Collection</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>Hyper Article en Ligne (HAL)</collection><collection>HAL-SHS: Archive ouverte en Sciences de l'Homme et de la Société</collection><collection>HAL-SHS: Archive ouverte en Sciences de l'Homme et de la Société (Open Access)</collection><collection>Hyper Article en Ligne (HAL) (Open Access)</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><collection>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</collection><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Somogyi, Eszter</au><au>Tran, Thuy Tuong Uyen</au><au>Guellai, Bahia</au><au>Király, Ildikó</au><au>Esseily, Rana</au><au>Capraro, Valerio</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>The effect of language on prosocial behaviors in preschool children</atitle><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle><addtitle>PLoS One</addtitle><date>2020-10-06</date><risdate>2020</risdate><volume>15</volume><issue>10</issue><spage>e0240028</spage><epage>e0240028</epage><pages>e0240028-e0240028</pages><issn>1932-6203</issn><eissn>1932-6203</eissn><abstract>The present study investigated how linguistic group membership influences prosocial behaviors, namely helpfulness and cooperation, in preschool children. Whilst research indicates that children preferentially direct their prosocial behavior towards members of their own groups, the influence of perceived linguistic group membership on actual helpfulness and cooperation has not been investigated. We presented an experimenter to 4- and 5-year-olds either as a foreigner, who did not speak the local language or as a native person. Children were then given the opportunity to help or cooperate with this experimenter in a series of nonverbal playful tasks. Whilst 4-year-olds helped and cooperated equally with the foreign and the native experimenter, 5-year-olds required significantly more cues and prompts in order to help or cooperate in the foreign condition. We also found that children were overall more reluctant to respond prosocially in the cooperation tasks than in the helping tasks. We tested children in two European countries (France and Hungary) and found the same pattern of responses in the two locations, suggesting that our findings are not specific to the local culture. Our results extend the findings of earlier research that showed selectivity according to the language spoken by the partner for sharing and imitation. Studies that looked at helpfulness or cooperation used the minimal group paradigm to induce group membership (based on arbitrary cues) and used indirect measures of prosociality, such as different forms of reasoning about the partner. In our study, we used language, a natural cue for group membership (versus arbitrary cues or cues based on social conventions) and directly observed children's helpful and cooperative behaviors toward the experimenter. Our results also confirm previous results indicating that with age, children become selective in their prosocial behaviors as they acquire new means of social evaluation and categorization. We conclude that the language associated with a potential social partner is not only a cue for affiliation and shared knowledge but also a cue mediating children's prosocial acts.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>Public Library of Science</pub><pmid>33022022</pmid><doi>10.1371/journal.pone.0240028</doi><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-3935-3933</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1817-6202</orcidid><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 1932-6203 |
ispartof | PloS one, 2020-10, Vol.15 (10), p.e0240028-e0240028 |
issn | 1932-6203 1932-6203 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_plos_journals_2448835115 |
source | MEDLINE; DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals; Elektronische Zeitschriftenbibliothek - Frei zugängliche E-Journals; Public Library of Science (PLoS); PubMed Central; Free Full-Text Journals in Chemistry |
subjects | Behavior Biology and Life Sciences Child Behavior Child, Preschool Children Cooperation Cooperative Behavior Female France Gender Helping behavior Humanities and Social Sciences Humans Hungary Language Language and languages Learning Linguistic research Local culture Male Peers People and Places Preschool children Psycholinguistics Psychological aspects Selectivity Social aspects Social Behavior Social Sciences Task Performance and Analysis |
title | The effect of language on prosocial behaviors in preschool children |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-12T13%3A02%3A27IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-gale_plos_&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=The%20effect%20of%20language%20on%20prosocial%20behaviors%20in%20preschool%20children&rft.jtitle=PloS%20one&rft.au=Somogyi,%20Eszter&rft.date=2020-10-06&rft.volume=15&rft.issue=10&rft.spage=e0240028&rft.epage=e0240028&rft.pages=e0240028-e0240028&rft.issn=1932-6203&rft.eissn=1932-6203&rft_id=info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0240028&rft_dat=%3Cgale_plos_%3EA637613600%3C/gale_plos_%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2448835115&rft_id=info:pmid/33022022&rft_galeid=A637613600&rft_doaj_id=oai_doaj_org_article_65cdd9f7eb674b86a6fbdc657d74c9fb&rfr_iscdi=true |