Inferring exemplar discriminability in brain representations
Representational distinctions within categories are important in all perceptual modalities and also in cognitive and motor representations. Recent pattern-information studies of brain activity have used condition-rich designs to sample the stimulus space more densely. To test whether brain response...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | PloS one 2020-06, Vol.15 (6), p.e0232551-e0232551 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | e0232551 |
---|---|
container_issue | 6 |
container_start_page | e0232551 |
container_title | PloS one |
container_volume | 15 |
creator | Nili, Hamed Walther, Alexander Alink, Arjen Kriegeskorte, Nikolaus |
description | Representational distinctions within categories are important in all perceptual modalities and also in cognitive and motor representations. Recent pattern-information studies of brain activity have used condition-rich designs to sample the stimulus space more densely. To test whether brain response patterns discriminate among a set of stimuli (e.g. exemplars within a category) with good sensitivity, we can pool statistical evidence over all pairwise comparisons. Here we describe a wide range of statistical tests of exemplar discriminability and assess the validity (specificity) and power (sensitivity) of each test. The tests include previously used and novel, parametric and nonparametric tests, which treat subject as a random or fixed effect, and are based on different dissimilarity measures, different test statistics, and different inference procedures. We use simulated and real data to determine which tests are valid and which are most sensitive. A popular test statistic reflecting exemplar information is the exemplar discriminability index (EDI), which is defined as the average of the pattern dissimilarity estimates between different exemplars minus the average of the pattern dissimilarity estimates between repetitions of identical exemplars. The popular across-subject t test of the EDI (typically using correlation distance as the pattern dissimilarity measure) requires the assumption that the EDI is 0-mean normal under H0. Although this assumption is not strictly true, our simulations suggest that the test controls the false-positives rate at the nominal level, and is thus valid, in practice. However, test statistics based on average Mahalanobis distances or average linear-discriminant t values (both accounting for the multivariate error covariance among responses) are substantially more powerful for both random- and fixed-effects inference. Unlike average cross-validated distances, the EDI is sensitive to differences between the distributions associated with different exemplars (e.g. greater variability for some exemplars than for others), which complicates its interpretation. We suggest preferred procedures for safely and sensitively detecting subtle pattern differences between exemplars. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1371/journal.pone.0232551 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>gale_plos_</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_plos_journals_2411517184</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><galeid>A626248363</galeid><doaj_id>oai_doaj_org_article_d7d007240306455092edfd0ad5796514</doaj_id><sourcerecordid>A626248363</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c743t-728f70651035a2fa27ce415a2d7f76c3aebe55cf091a321d6690742e0ebe474e3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqNkmtr2zAUhs3YWLtu_2BsgcHYPiTTXTGMQSm7BAqF3b4KxTp2FGTJlezR_vspiVvi0Q_DIIuj57w65-gtipcYLTCV-MM2DNFrt-iChwUilHCOHxWnuKRkLgiij4_2J8WzlLYIcboU4mlxkmGCSkFOi48rX0OM1jczuIG2czrOjE1VtK31em2d7W9n1s_WUec1Qhchge91b4NPz4sntXYJXoz_s-LXl88_L77NL6--ri7OL-eVZLSfS7KsJRIcI8o1qTWRFTCct0bWUlRUwxo4r2pUYk0JNkKUSDICKMeZZEDPitcH3c6FpMbGkyIMY44lXrJMrA6ECXqruly9jrcqaKv2gRAbpWNvKwfKSIOQJAxRJBjnqCRgaoO04bLMNe60Po23DesWTJXbjdpNRKcn3m5UE_6o3GcWWGaBd6NADNcDpF61eaLgnPYQhn3dhOCy5CSjb_5BH-5upBqdG7C-DvneaieqzgURhC2poJlaPEDlz0Brq-yS2ub4JOH9JCEzPdz0jR5SUqsf3_-fvfo9Zd8esRvQrt-k4Ia9Z6YgO4BVDClFqO-HjJHamfxuGmpncjWaPKe9On6g-6Q7V9O_G5P1UQ</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Website</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2411517184</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Inferring exemplar discriminability in brain representations</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</source><source>Public Library of Science (PLoS)</source><source>EZB-FREE-00999 freely available EZB journals</source><source>PubMed Central</source><source>Free Full-Text Journals in Chemistry</source><creator>Nili, Hamed ; Walther, Alexander ; Alink, Arjen ; Kriegeskorte, Nikolaus</creator><contributor>Malo, J</contributor><creatorcontrib>Nili, Hamed ; Walther, Alexander ; Alink, Arjen ; Kriegeskorte, Nikolaus ; Malo, J</creatorcontrib><description>Representational distinctions within categories are important in all perceptual modalities and also in cognitive and motor representations. Recent pattern-information studies of brain activity have used condition-rich designs to sample the stimulus space more densely. To test whether brain response patterns discriminate among a set of stimuli (e.g. exemplars within a category) with good sensitivity, we can pool statistical evidence over all pairwise comparisons. Here we describe a wide range of statistical tests of exemplar discriminability and assess the validity (specificity) and power (sensitivity) of each test. The tests include previously used and novel, parametric and nonparametric tests, which treat subject as a random or fixed effect, and are based on different dissimilarity measures, different test statistics, and different inference procedures. We use simulated and real data to determine which tests are valid and which are most sensitive. A popular test statistic reflecting exemplar information is the exemplar discriminability index (EDI), which is defined as the average of the pattern dissimilarity estimates between different exemplars minus the average of the pattern dissimilarity estimates between repetitions of identical exemplars. The popular across-subject t test of the EDI (typically using correlation distance as the pattern dissimilarity measure) requires the assumption that the EDI is 0-mean normal under H0. Although this assumption is not strictly true, our simulations suggest that the test controls the false-positives rate at the nominal level, and is thus valid, in practice. However, test statistics based on average Mahalanobis distances or average linear-discriminant t values (both accounting for the multivariate error covariance among responses) are substantially more powerful for both random- and fixed-effects inference. Unlike average cross-validated distances, the EDI is sensitive to differences between the distributions associated with different exemplars (e.g. greater variability for some exemplars than for others), which complicates its interpretation. We suggest preferred procedures for safely and sensitively detecting subtle pattern differences between exemplars.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0232551</identifier><identifier>PMID: 32520962</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: Public Library of Science</publisher><subject>Adult ; Biology and Life Sciences ; Brain ; Brain - diagnostic imaging ; Brain - physiology ; Brain Mapping - methods ; Brain research ; Cognitive ability ; Cognitive neuroscience ; Computer Simulation ; Covariance ; Data Interpretation, Statistical ; Engineering and Technology ; Estimates ; Female ; Humans ; Magnetic Resonance Imaging - methods ; Male ; Medicine and Health Sciences ; Mental representations ; Neuroimaging ; Normal distribution ; Pattern Recognition, Automated - methods ; Physical Sciences ; Power ; Representations ; Research and Analysis Methods ; Sensitivity analysis ; Sensitivity and Specificity ; Statistical analysis ; Statistical inference ; Statistical tests ; Stimulus discrimination ; Studies ; Visual Perception - physiology ; Young Adult</subject><ispartof>PloS one, 2020-06, Vol.15 (6), p.e0232551-e0232551</ispartof><rights>COPYRIGHT 2020 Public Library of Science</rights><rights>This is an open access article, free of all copyright, and may be freely reproduced, distributed, transmitted, modified, built upon, or otherwise used by anyone for any lawful purpose. The work is made available under the Creative Commons CC0 public domain dedication: https://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c743t-728f70651035a2fa27ce415a2d7f76c3aebe55cf091a321d6690742e0ebe474e3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c743t-728f70651035a2fa27ce415a2d7f76c3aebe55cf091a321d6690742e0ebe474e3</cites><orcidid>0000-0002-6629-7957</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7286518/pdf/$$EPDF$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$Hfree_for_read</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7286518/$$EHTML$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$Hfree_for_read</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,723,776,780,860,881,2095,2914,23846,27903,27904,53770,53772,79347,79348</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32520962$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><contributor>Malo, J</contributor><creatorcontrib>Nili, Hamed</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Walther, Alexander</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Alink, Arjen</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kriegeskorte, Nikolaus</creatorcontrib><title>Inferring exemplar discriminability in brain representations</title><title>PloS one</title><addtitle>PLoS One</addtitle><description>Representational distinctions within categories are important in all perceptual modalities and also in cognitive and motor representations. Recent pattern-information studies of brain activity have used condition-rich designs to sample the stimulus space more densely. To test whether brain response patterns discriminate among a set of stimuli (e.g. exemplars within a category) with good sensitivity, we can pool statistical evidence over all pairwise comparisons. Here we describe a wide range of statistical tests of exemplar discriminability and assess the validity (specificity) and power (sensitivity) of each test. The tests include previously used and novel, parametric and nonparametric tests, which treat subject as a random or fixed effect, and are based on different dissimilarity measures, different test statistics, and different inference procedures. We use simulated and real data to determine which tests are valid and which are most sensitive. A popular test statistic reflecting exemplar information is the exemplar discriminability index (EDI), which is defined as the average of the pattern dissimilarity estimates between different exemplars minus the average of the pattern dissimilarity estimates between repetitions of identical exemplars. The popular across-subject t test of the EDI (typically using correlation distance as the pattern dissimilarity measure) requires the assumption that the EDI is 0-mean normal under H0. Although this assumption is not strictly true, our simulations suggest that the test controls the false-positives rate at the nominal level, and is thus valid, in practice. However, test statistics based on average Mahalanobis distances or average linear-discriminant t values (both accounting for the multivariate error covariance among responses) are substantially more powerful for both random- and fixed-effects inference. Unlike average cross-validated distances, the EDI is sensitive to differences between the distributions associated with different exemplars (e.g. greater variability for some exemplars than for others), which complicates its interpretation. We suggest preferred procedures for safely and sensitively detecting subtle pattern differences between exemplars.</description><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject><subject>Brain</subject><subject>Brain - diagnostic imaging</subject><subject>Brain - physiology</subject><subject>Brain Mapping - methods</subject><subject>Brain research</subject><subject>Cognitive ability</subject><subject>Cognitive neuroscience</subject><subject>Computer Simulation</subject><subject>Covariance</subject><subject>Data Interpretation, Statistical</subject><subject>Engineering and Technology</subject><subject>Estimates</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Magnetic Resonance Imaging - methods</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Medicine and Health Sciences</subject><subject>Mental representations</subject><subject>Neuroimaging</subject><subject>Normal distribution</subject><subject>Pattern Recognition, Automated - methods</subject><subject>Physical Sciences</subject><subject>Power</subject><subject>Representations</subject><subject>Research and Analysis Methods</subject><subject>Sensitivity analysis</subject><subject>Sensitivity and Specificity</subject><subject>Statistical analysis</subject><subject>Statistical inference</subject><subject>Statistical tests</subject><subject>Stimulus discrimination</subject><subject>Studies</subject><subject>Visual Perception - physiology</subject><subject>Young Adult</subject><issn>1932-6203</issn><issn>1932-6203</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2020</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>ABUWG</sourceid><sourceid>AFKRA</sourceid><sourceid>AZQEC</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>CCPQU</sourceid><sourceid>DWQXO</sourceid><sourceid>GNUQQ</sourceid><sourceid>DOA</sourceid><recordid>eNqNkmtr2zAUhs3YWLtu_2BsgcHYPiTTXTGMQSm7BAqF3b4KxTp2FGTJlezR_vspiVvi0Q_DIIuj57w65-gtipcYLTCV-MM2DNFrt-iChwUilHCOHxWnuKRkLgiij4_2J8WzlLYIcboU4mlxkmGCSkFOi48rX0OM1jczuIG2czrOjE1VtK31em2d7W9n1s_WUec1Qhchge91b4NPz4sntXYJXoz_s-LXl88_L77NL6--ri7OL-eVZLSfS7KsJRIcI8o1qTWRFTCct0bWUlRUwxo4r2pUYk0JNkKUSDICKMeZZEDPitcH3c6FpMbGkyIMY44lXrJMrA6ECXqruly9jrcqaKv2gRAbpWNvKwfKSIOQJAxRJBjnqCRgaoO04bLMNe60Po23DesWTJXbjdpNRKcn3m5UE_6o3GcWWGaBd6NADNcDpF61eaLgnPYQhn3dhOCy5CSjb_5BH-5upBqdG7C-DvneaieqzgURhC2poJlaPEDlz0Brq-yS2ub4JOH9JCEzPdz0jR5SUqsf3_-fvfo9Zd8esRvQrt-k4Ia9Z6YgO4BVDClFqO-HjJHamfxuGmpncjWaPKe9On6g-6Q7V9O_G5P1UQ</recordid><startdate>20200610</startdate><enddate>20200610</enddate><creator>Nili, Hamed</creator><creator>Walther, Alexander</creator><creator>Alink, Arjen</creator><creator>Kriegeskorte, Nikolaus</creator><general>Public Library of Science</general><general>Public Library of Science (PLoS)</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>IOV</scope><scope>ISR</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>7QL</scope><scope>7QO</scope><scope>7RV</scope><scope>7SN</scope><scope>7SS</scope><scope>7T5</scope><scope>7TG</scope><scope>7TM</scope><scope>7U9</scope><scope>7X2</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8C1</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FE</scope><scope>8FG</scope><scope>8FH</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABJCF</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AEUYN</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ARAPS</scope><scope>ATCPS</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BBNVY</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BGLVJ</scope><scope>BHPHI</scope><scope>C1K</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>D1I</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>H94</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>KB.</scope><scope>KB0</scope><scope>KL.</scope><scope>L6V</scope><scope>LK8</scope><scope>M0K</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M7N</scope><scope>M7P</scope><scope>M7S</scope><scope>NAPCQ</scope><scope>P5Z</scope><scope>P62</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PATMY</scope><scope>PDBOC</scope><scope>PIMPY</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PTHSS</scope><scope>PYCSY</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope><scope>DOA</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6629-7957</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>20200610</creationdate><title>Inferring exemplar discriminability in brain representations</title><author>Nili, Hamed ; Walther, Alexander ; Alink, Arjen ; Kriegeskorte, Nikolaus</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c743t-728f70651035a2fa27ce415a2d7f76c3aebe55cf091a321d6690742e0ebe474e3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2020</creationdate><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Biology and Life Sciences</topic><topic>Brain</topic><topic>Brain - diagnostic imaging</topic><topic>Brain - physiology</topic><topic>Brain Mapping - methods</topic><topic>Brain research</topic><topic>Cognitive ability</topic><topic>Cognitive neuroscience</topic><topic>Computer Simulation</topic><topic>Covariance</topic><topic>Data Interpretation, Statistical</topic><topic>Engineering and Technology</topic><topic>Estimates</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Magnetic Resonance Imaging - methods</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Medicine and Health Sciences</topic><topic>Mental representations</topic><topic>Neuroimaging</topic><topic>Normal distribution</topic><topic>Pattern Recognition, Automated - methods</topic><topic>Physical Sciences</topic><topic>Power</topic><topic>Representations</topic><topic>Research and Analysis Methods</topic><topic>Sensitivity analysis</topic><topic>Sensitivity and Specificity</topic><topic>Statistical analysis</topic><topic>Statistical inference</topic><topic>Statistical tests</topic><topic>Stimulus discrimination</topic><topic>Studies</topic><topic>Visual Perception - physiology</topic><topic>Young Adult</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Nili, Hamed</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Walther, Alexander</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Alink, Arjen</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kriegeskorte, Nikolaus</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Gale In Context: Opposing Viewpoints</collection><collection>Gale In Context: Science</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B)</collection><collection>Biotechnology Research Abstracts</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Database</collection><collection>Ecology Abstracts</collection><collection>Entomology Abstracts (Full archive)</collection><collection>Immunology Abstracts</collection><collection>Meteorological & Geoastrophysical Abstracts</collection><collection>Nucleic Acids Abstracts</collection><collection>Virology and AIDS Abstracts</collection><collection>Agricultural Science Collection</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Public Health Database</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>ProQuest SciTech Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Technology Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Materials Science & Engineering Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Sustainability</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>Agricultural & Environmental Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Technology Collection</collection><collection>Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Materials Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Materials Science Database</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Meteorological & Geoastrophysical Abstracts - Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest Engineering Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>Agricultural Science Database</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C)</collection><collection>Biological Science Database</collection><collection>Engineering Database</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Premium</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>Environmental Science Database</collection><collection>Materials Science Collection</collection><collection>Publicly Available Content Database</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>Engineering Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Science Collection</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><collection>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</collection><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Nili, Hamed</au><au>Walther, Alexander</au><au>Alink, Arjen</au><au>Kriegeskorte, Nikolaus</au><au>Malo, J</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Inferring exemplar discriminability in brain representations</atitle><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle><addtitle>PLoS One</addtitle><date>2020-06-10</date><risdate>2020</risdate><volume>15</volume><issue>6</issue><spage>e0232551</spage><epage>e0232551</epage><pages>e0232551-e0232551</pages><issn>1932-6203</issn><eissn>1932-6203</eissn><abstract>Representational distinctions within categories are important in all perceptual modalities and also in cognitive and motor representations. Recent pattern-information studies of brain activity have used condition-rich designs to sample the stimulus space more densely. To test whether brain response patterns discriminate among a set of stimuli (e.g. exemplars within a category) with good sensitivity, we can pool statistical evidence over all pairwise comparisons. Here we describe a wide range of statistical tests of exemplar discriminability and assess the validity (specificity) and power (sensitivity) of each test. The tests include previously used and novel, parametric and nonparametric tests, which treat subject as a random or fixed effect, and are based on different dissimilarity measures, different test statistics, and different inference procedures. We use simulated and real data to determine which tests are valid and which are most sensitive. A popular test statistic reflecting exemplar information is the exemplar discriminability index (EDI), which is defined as the average of the pattern dissimilarity estimates between different exemplars minus the average of the pattern dissimilarity estimates between repetitions of identical exemplars. The popular across-subject t test of the EDI (typically using correlation distance as the pattern dissimilarity measure) requires the assumption that the EDI is 0-mean normal under H0. Although this assumption is not strictly true, our simulations suggest that the test controls the false-positives rate at the nominal level, and is thus valid, in practice. However, test statistics based on average Mahalanobis distances or average linear-discriminant t values (both accounting for the multivariate error covariance among responses) are substantially more powerful for both random- and fixed-effects inference. Unlike average cross-validated distances, the EDI is sensitive to differences between the distributions associated with different exemplars (e.g. greater variability for some exemplars than for others), which complicates its interpretation. We suggest preferred procedures for safely and sensitively detecting subtle pattern differences between exemplars.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>Public Library of Science</pub><pmid>32520962</pmid><doi>10.1371/journal.pone.0232551</doi><tpages>e0232551</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6629-7957</orcidid><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 1932-6203 |
ispartof | PloS one, 2020-06, Vol.15 (6), p.e0232551-e0232551 |
issn | 1932-6203 1932-6203 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_plos_journals_2411517184 |
source | MEDLINE; DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals; Public Library of Science (PLoS); EZB-FREE-00999 freely available EZB journals; PubMed Central; Free Full-Text Journals in Chemistry |
subjects | Adult Biology and Life Sciences Brain Brain - diagnostic imaging Brain - physiology Brain Mapping - methods Brain research Cognitive ability Cognitive neuroscience Computer Simulation Covariance Data Interpretation, Statistical Engineering and Technology Estimates Female Humans Magnetic Resonance Imaging - methods Male Medicine and Health Sciences Mental representations Neuroimaging Normal distribution Pattern Recognition, Automated - methods Physical Sciences Power Representations Research and Analysis Methods Sensitivity analysis Sensitivity and Specificity Statistical analysis Statistical inference Statistical tests Stimulus discrimination Studies Visual Perception - physiology Young Adult |
title | Inferring exemplar discriminability in brain representations |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-23T01%3A45%3A12IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-gale_plos_&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Inferring%20exemplar%20discriminability%20in%20brain%20representations&rft.jtitle=PloS%20one&rft.au=Nili,%20Hamed&rft.date=2020-06-10&rft.volume=15&rft.issue=6&rft.spage=e0232551&rft.epage=e0232551&rft.pages=e0232551-e0232551&rft.issn=1932-6203&rft.eissn=1932-6203&rft_id=info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0232551&rft_dat=%3Cgale_plos_%3EA626248363%3C/gale_plos_%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2411517184&rft_id=info:pmid/32520962&rft_galeid=A626248363&rft_doaj_id=oai_doaj_org_article_d7d007240306455092edfd0ad5796514&rfr_iscdi=true |