Are unpopular children more likely to get sick? Longitudinal links between popularity and infectious diseases in early childhood
Social stress and inflammatory processes are strong regulators of one another. Considerable evidence shows that social threats trigger inflammatory responses that increase infection susceptibility in both humans and animals, while infectious disease triggers inflammation that in turn regulates socia...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | PloS one 2019-09, Vol.14 (9), p.e0222222-e0222222 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , , , , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | e0222222 |
---|---|
container_issue | 9 |
container_start_page | e0222222 |
container_title | PloS one |
container_volume | 14 |
creator | Ulset, Vidar Sandsaunet Czajkowski, Nikolai Olavi Kraft, Brage Kraft, Pål Wikenius, Ellen Kleppestø, Thomas Haarklau Bekkhus, Mona |
description | Social stress and inflammatory processes are strong regulators of one another. Considerable evidence shows that social threats trigger inflammatory responses that increase infection susceptibility in both humans and animals, while infectious disease triggers inflammation that in turn regulates social behaviours. However, no previous study has examined whether young children's popularity and their rate of infectious disease are associated. We investigated the longitudinal bidirectional links between children's popularity status as perceived by peers, and parent reports of a variety of infectious diseases that are common in early childhood (i.e. common cold as well as eye, ear, throat, lung and gastric infections). We used data from the 'Matter of the First Friendship Study' (MOFF), a longitudinal prospective multi-informant study, following 579 Norwegian pre-schoolers (292 girls, median age at baseline = six years) with annual assessments over a period of three years. Social network analysis was used to estimate each child's level of popularity. Cross-lagged autoregressive analyses revealed negative dose-response relations between children's popularity scores and subsequent infection (b = -0.18, CI = -0.29, -0.06, and b = -0.13, CI = -0.23, -0.03). In conclusion, the results suggest that children who are unpopular in early childhood are at increased risk of contracting infection the following year. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1371/journal.pone.0222222 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>gale_plos_</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_plos_journals_2288192863</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><galeid>A599005815</galeid><doaj_id>oai_doaj_org_article_36dd6a0364314eb08d4a477070937ecc</doaj_id><sourcerecordid>A599005815</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c716t-4ce11a50d27cbdd519020f1f2b0dcbd4b0084e6d4ca4444f4e891e3737b8a0b83</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqNk0uP0zAQxyMEYpeFb4AgEhKCQ4sd5-FcQNWKR6VKK_G6Wo49Sd26dtd2gN746LjddGnQHnAOiSa_-c_DM0nyFKMpJhV-s7K9M1xPt9bAFGWHcy85xzXJJmWGyP2T77PkkfcrhApCy_JhckZwgXJU0PPk98xB2put3faau1QslZYOTLqx0a7VGvQuDTbtIKReifW7dGFNp0IvVYwdAbP2aQPhJ0SfQUSFXcqNTJVpQQRle59K5YF78NGWAndR8xBoaa18nDxoufbwZHhfJN8-vP96-WmyuPo4v5wtJqLCZZjkAjDmBZJZJRopC1yjDLW4zRokoyFvEKI5lDIXPI-nzYHWGEhFqoZy1FBykTy_0d1q69nQPM-yjFJcZ7QkkZjfENLyFds6teFuxyxX7GCwrmPcBSU0MFJKWXJEypzgHBpEZc7zqkIVqkkFQkStt0O0vtmAFGCC43okOv5j1JJ19gcrK1JmpPybrnDKB2WYsY4zjGiRsaqoyT7dV0MIZ6978IFtlBegNTcQe36orMKUYBTRF_-gd9c_UB2PJcbbszEzsRdls6Ku4_RQXERqegcVHwkbJeIstiraRw6vRw6RCfArdLz3ns2_fP5_9ur7mH15wi6B67D0Vvdx4owfg_mxldZ7B-3tNWDE9qt07AbbrxIbVim6PTu9wlun4-6QPzn3GR8</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Website</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2288192863</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Are unpopular children more likely to get sick? Longitudinal links between popularity and infectious diseases in early childhood</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>NORA - Norwegian Open Research Archives</source><source>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</source><source>Public Library of Science (PLoS)</source><source>EZB-FREE-00999 freely available EZB journals</source><source>PubMed Central</source><source>Free Full-Text Journals in Chemistry</source><creator>Ulset, Vidar Sandsaunet ; Czajkowski, Nikolai Olavi ; Kraft, Brage ; Kraft, Pål ; Wikenius, Ellen ; Kleppestø, Thomas Haarklau ; Bekkhus, Mona</creator><contributor>Fischer, Florian</contributor><creatorcontrib>Ulset, Vidar Sandsaunet ; Czajkowski, Nikolai Olavi ; Kraft, Brage ; Kraft, Pål ; Wikenius, Ellen ; Kleppestø, Thomas Haarklau ; Bekkhus, Mona ; Fischer, Florian</creatorcontrib><description>Social stress and inflammatory processes are strong regulators of one another. Considerable evidence shows that social threats trigger inflammatory responses that increase infection susceptibility in both humans and animals, while infectious disease triggers inflammation that in turn regulates social behaviours. However, no previous study has examined whether young children's popularity and their rate of infectious disease are associated. We investigated the longitudinal bidirectional links between children's popularity status as perceived by peers, and parent reports of a variety of infectious diseases that are common in early childhood (i.e. common cold as well as eye, ear, throat, lung and gastric infections). We used data from the 'Matter of the First Friendship Study' (MOFF), a longitudinal prospective multi-informant study, following 579 Norwegian pre-schoolers (292 girls, median age at baseline = six years) with annual assessments over a period of three years. Social network analysis was used to estimate each child's level of popularity. Cross-lagged autoregressive analyses revealed negative dose-response relations between children's popularity scores and subsequent infection (b = -0.18, CI = -0.29, -0.06, and b = -0.13, CI = -0.23, -0.03). In conclusion, the results suggest that children who are unpopular in early childhood are at increased risk of contracting infection the following year.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0222222</identifier><identifier>PMID: 31504058</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: Public Library of Science</publisher><subject>Aggressiveness ; Analysis ; Animal diseases ; Animals ; Behavior ; Biology and Life Sciences ; Child health ; Child, Preschool ; Childhood ; Children ; Children & youth ; Common cold ; Communicable diseases ; Communicable Diseases - psychology ; Computer and Information Sciences ; Cytokines ; Disease susceptibility ; Female ; Gastrointestinal diseases ; Gene expression ; Girls ; Health risks ; Hormones ; Humans ; Immune system ; Infection ; Infections ; Infectious diseases ; Inflammation ; Influence ; Longitude ; Longitudinal Studies ; Lungs ; Male ; Medical research ; Medicine and Health Sciences ; Network analysis ; Pediatric diseases ; Peers ; People and Places ; Pharynx ; Popularity ; Psychology, Social ; Regulators ; Risk Factors ; Social behavior ; Social Distance ; Social interactions ; Social Networking ; Social networks ; Social organization ; Social Sciences ; Stress, Psychological - psychology ; Studies</subject><ispartof>PloS one, 2019-09, Vol.14 (9), p.e0222222-e0222222</ispartof><rights>COPYRIGHT 2019 Public Library of Science</rights><rights>2019 Ulset et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.</rights><rights>info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess</rights><rights>2019 Ulset et al 2019 Ulset et al</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c716t-4ce11a50d27cbdd519020f1f2b0dcbd4b0084e6d4ca4444f4e891e3737b8a0b83</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c716t-4ce11a50d27cbdd519020f1f2b0dcbd4b0084e6d4ca4444f4e891e3737b8a0b83</cites><orcidid>0000-0002-7160-1298</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6736236/pdf/$$EPDF$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$Hfree_for_read</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6736236/$$EHTML$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$Hfree_for_read</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,727,780,784,864,885,2100,2926,23864,26565,27922,27923,53789,53791,79370,79371</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31504058$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><contributor>Fischer, Florian</contributor><creatorcontrib>Ulset, Vidar Sandsaunet</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Czajkowski, Nikolai Olavi</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kraft, Brage</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kraft, Pål</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Wikenius, Ellen</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kleppestø, Thomas Haarklau</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Bekkhus, Mona</creatorcontrib><title>Are unpopular children more likely to get sick? Longitudinal links between popularity and infectious diseases in early childhood</title><title>PloS one</title><addtitle>PLoS One</addtitle><description>Social stress and inflammatory processes are strong regulators of one another. Considerable evidence shows that social threats trigger inflammatory responses that increase infection susceptibility in both humans and animals, while infectious disease triggers inflammation that in turn regulates social behaviours. However, no previous study has examined whether young children's popularity and their rate of infectious disease are associated. We investigated the longitudinal bidirectional links between children's popularity status as perceived by peers, and parent reports of a variety of infectious diseases that are common in early childhood (i.e. common cold as well as eye, ear, throat, lung and gastric infections). We used data from the 'Matter of the First Friendship Study' (MOFF), a longitudinal prospective multi-informant study, following 579 Norwegian pre-schoolers (292 girls, median age at baseline = six years) with annual assessments over a period of three years. Social network analysis was used to estimate each child's level of popularity. Cross-lagged autoregressive analyses revealed negative dose-response relations between children's popularity scores and subsequent infection (b = -0.18, CI = -0.29, -0.06, and b = -0.13, CI = -0.23, -0.03). In conclusion, the results suggest that children who are unpopular in early childhood are at increased risk of contracting infection the following year.</description><subject>Aggressiveness</subject><subject>Analysis</subject><subject>Animal diseases</subject><subject>Animals</subject><subject>Behavior</subject><subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject><subject>Child health</subject><subject>Child, Preschool</subject><subject>Childhood</subject><subject>Children</subject><subject>Children & youth</subject><subject>Common cold</subject><subject>Communicable diseases</subject><subject>Communicable Diseases - psychology</subject><subject>Computer and Information Sciences</subject><subject>Cytokines</subject><subject>Disease susceptibility</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Gastrointestinal diseases</subject><subject>Gene expression</subject><subject>Girls</subject><subject>Health risks</subject><subject>Hormones</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Immune system</subject><subject>Infection</subject><subject>Infections</subject><subject>Infectious diseases</subject><subject>Inflammation</subject><subject>Influence</subject><subject>Longitude</subject><subject>Longitudinal Studies</subject><subject>Lungs</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Medical research</subject><subject>Medicine and Health Sciences</subject><subject>Network analysis</subject><subject>Pediatric diseases</subject><subject>Peers</subject><subject>People and Places</subject><subject>Pharynx</subject><subject>Popularity</subject><subject>Psychology, Social</subject><subject>Regulators</subject><subject>Risk Factors</subject><subject>Social behavior</subject><subject>Social Distance</subject><subject>Social interactions</subject><subject>Social Networking</subject><subject>Social networks</subject><subject>Social organization</subject><subject>Social Sciences</subject><subject>Stress, Psychological - psychology</subject><subject>Studies</subject><issn>1932-6203</issn><issn>1932-6203</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2019</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>ABUWG</sourceid><sourceid>AFKRA</sourceid><sourceid>AZQEC</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>CCPQU</sourceid><sourceid>DWQXO</sourceid><sourceid>GNUQQ</sourceid><sourceid>3HK</sourceid><sourceid>DOA</sourceid><recordid>eNqNk0uP0zAQxyMEYpeFb4AgEhKCQ4sd5-FcQNWKR6VKK_G6Wo49Sd26dtd2gN746LjddGnQHnAOiSa_-c_DM0nyFKMpJhV-s7K9M1xPt9bAFGWHcy85xzXJJmWGyP2T77PkkfcrhApCy_JhckZwgXJU0PPk98xB2put3faau1QslZYOTLqx0a7VGvQuDTbtIKReifW7dGFNp0IvVYwdAbP2aQPhJ0SfQUSFXcqNTJVpQQRle59K5YF78NGWAndR8xBoaa18nDxoufbwZHhfJN8-vP96-WmyuPo4v5wtJqLCZZjkAjDmBZJZJRopC1yjDLW4zRokoyFvEKI5lDIXPI-nzYHWGEhFqoZy1FBykTy_0d1q69nQPM-yjFJcZ7QkkZjfENLyFds6teFuxyxX7GCwrmPcBSU0MFJKWXJEypzgHBpEZc7zqkIVqkkFQkStt0O0vtmAFGCC43okOv5j1JJ19gcrK1JmpPybrnDKB2WYsY4zjGiRsaqoyT7dV0MIZ6978IFtlBegNTcQe36orMKUYBTRF_-gd9c_UB2PJcbbszEzsRdls6Ku4_RQXERqegcVHwkbJeIstiraRw6vRw6RCfArdLz3ns2_fP5_9ur7mH15wi6B67D0Vvdx4owfg_mxldZ7B-3tNWDE9qt07AbbrxIbVim6PTu9wlun4-6QPzn3GR8</recordid><startdate>20190910</startdate><enddate>20190910</enddate><creator>Ulset, Vidar Sandsaunet</creator><creator>Czajkowski, Nikolai Olavi</creator><creator>Kraft, Brage</creator><creator>Kraft, Pål</creator><creator>Wikenius, Ellen</creator><creator>Kleppestø, Thomas Haarklau</creator><creator>Bekkhus, Mona</creator><general>Public Library of Science</general><general>PLOS</general><general>Public Library of Science (PLoS)</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>IOV</scope><scope>ISR</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>7QL</scope><scope>7QO</scope><scope>7RV</scope><scope>7SN</scope><scope>7SS</scope><scope>7T5</scope><scope>7TG</scope><scope>7TM</scope><scope>7U9</scope><scope>7X2</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8C1</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FE</scope><scope>8FG</scope><scope>8FH</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABJCF</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AEUYN</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ARAPS</scope><scope>ATCPS</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BBNVY</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BGLVJ</scope><scope>BHPHI</scope><scope>C1K</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>D1I</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>H94</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>KB.</scope><scope>KB0</scope><scope>KL.</scope><scope>L6V</scope><scope>LK8</scope><scope>M0K</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M7N</scope><scope>M7P</scope><scope>M7S</scope><scope>NAPCQ</scope><scope>P5Z</scope><scope>P62</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PATMY</scope><scope>PDBOC</scope><scope>PIMPY</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PTHSS</scope><scope>PYCSY</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>3HK</scope><scope>5PM</scope><scope>DOA</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7160-1298</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>20190910</creationdate><title>Are unpopular children more likely to get sick? Longitudinal links between popularity and infectious diseases in early childhood</title><author>Ulset, Vidar Sandsaunet ; Czajkowski, Nikolai Olavi ; Kraft, Brage ; Kraft, Pål ; Wikenius, Ellen ; Kleppestø, Thomas Haarklau ; Bekkhus, Mona</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c716t-4ce11a50d27cbdd519020f1f2b0dcbd4b0084e6d4ca4444f4e891e3737b8a0b83</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2019</creationdate><topic>Aggressiveness</topic><topic>Analysis</topic><topic>Animal diseases</topic><topic>Animals</topic><topic>Behavior</topic><topic>Biology and Life Sciences</topic><topic>Child health</topic><topic>Child, Preschool</topic><topic>Childhood</topic><topic>Children</topic><topic>Children & youth</topic><topic>Common cold</topic><topic>Communicable diseases</topic><topic>Communicable Diseases - psychology</topic><topic>Computer and Information Sciences</topic><topic>Cytokines</topic><topic>Disease susceptibility</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Gastrointestinal diseases</topic><topic>Gene expression</topic><topic>Girls</topic><topic>Health risks</topic><topic>Hormones</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Immune system</topic><topic>Infection</topic><topic>Infections</topic><topic>Infectious diseases</topic><topic>Inflammation</topic><topic>Influence</topic><topic>Longitude</topic><topic>Longitudinal Studies</topic><topic>Lungs</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Medical research</topic><topic>Medicine and Health Sciences</topic><topic>Network analysis</topic><topic>Pediatric diseases</topic><topic>Peers</topic><topic>People and Places</topic><topic>Pharynx</topic><topic>Popularity</topic><topic>Psychology, Social</topic><topic>Regulators</topic><topic>Risk Factors</topic><topic>Social behavior</topic><topic>Social Distance</topic><topic>Social interactions</topic><topic>Social Networking</topic><topic>Social networks</topic><topic>Social organization</topic><topic>Social Sciences</topic><topic>Stress, Psychological - psychology</topic><topic>Studies</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Ulset, Vidar Sandsaunet</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Czajkowski, Nikolai Olavi</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kraft, Brage</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kraft, Pål</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Wikenius, Ellen</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kleppestø, Thomas Haarklau</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Bekkhus, Mona</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Gale In Context: Opposing Viewpoints</collection><collection>Gale In Context: Science</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B)</collection><collection>Biotechnology Research Abstracts</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Database</collection><collection>Ecology Abstracts</collection><collection>Entomology Abstracts (Full archive)</collection><collection>Immunology Abstracts</collection><collection>Meteorological & Geoastrophysical Abstracts</collection><collection>Nucleic Acids Abstracts</collection><collection>Virology and AIDS Abstracts</collection><collection>Agricultural Science Collection</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Public Health Database</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>ProQuest SciTech Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Technology Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Materials Science & Engineering Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Sustainability</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>Agricultural & Environmental Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Technology Collection</collection><collection>Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Materials Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Materials Science Database</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Meteorological & Geoastrophysical Abstracts - Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest Engineering Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>Agricultural Science Database</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C)</collection><collection>Biological Science Database</collection><collection>Engineering Database</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Premium</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>Environmental Science Database</collection><collection>Materials Science Collection</collection><collection>Publicly Available Content Database</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>Engineering Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Science Collection</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>NORA - Norwegian Open Research Archives</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><collection>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</collection><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Ulset, Vidar Sandsaunet</au><au>Czajkowski, Nikolai Olavi</au><au>Kraft, Brage</au><au>Kraft, Pål</au><au>Wikenius, Ellen</au><au>Kleppestø, Thomas Haarklau</au><au>Bekkhus, Mona</au><au>Fischer, Florian</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Are unpopular children more likely to get sick? Longitudinal links between popularity and infectious diseases in early childhood</atitle><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle><addtitle>PLoS One</addtitle><date>2019-09-10</date><risdate>2019</risdate><volume>14</volume><issue>9</issue><spage>e0222222</spage><epage>e0222222</epage><pages>e0222222-e0222222</pages><issn>1932-6203</issn><eissn>1932-6203</eissn><abstract>Social stress and inflammatory processes are strong regulators of one another. Considerable evidence shows that social threats trigger inflammatory responses that increase infection susceptibility in both humans and animals, while infectious disease triggers inflammation that in turn regulates social behaviours. However, no previous study has examined whether young children's popularity and their rate of infectious disease are associated. We investigated the longitudinal bidirectional links between children's popularity status as perceived by peers, and parent reports of a variety of infectious diseases that are common in early childhood (i.e. common cold as well as eye, ear, throat, lung and gastric infections). We used data from the 'Matter of the First Friendship Study' (MOFF), a longitudinal prospective multi-informant study, following 579 Norwegian pre-schoolers (292 girls, median age at baseline = six years) with annual assessments over a period of three years. Social network analysis was used to estimate each child's level of popularity. Cross-lagged autoregressive analyses revealed negative dose-response relations between children's popularity scores and subsequent infection (b = -0.18, CI = -0.29, -0.06, and b = -0.13, CI = -0.23, -0.03). In conclusion, the results suggest that children who are unpopular in early childhood are at increased risk of contracting infection the following year.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>Public Library of Science</pub><pmid>31504058</pmid><doi>10.1371/journal.pone.0222222</doi><tpages>e0222222</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7160-1298</orcidid><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 1932-6203 |
ispartof | PloS one, 2019-09, Vol.14 (9), p.e0222222-e0222222 |
issn | 1932-6203 1932-6203 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_plos_journals_2288192863 |
source | MEDLINE; NORA - Norwegian Open Research Archives; DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals; Public Library of Science (PLoS); EZB-FREE-00999 freely available EZB journals; PubMed Central; Free Full-Text Journals in Chemistry |
subjects | Aggressiveness Analysis Animal diseases Animals Behavior Biology and Life Sciences Child health Child, Preschool Childhood Children Children & youth Common cold Communicable diseases Communicable Diseases - psychology Computer and Information Sciences Cytokines Disease susceptibility Female Gastrointestinal diseases Gene expression Girls Health risks Hormones Humans Immune system Infection Infections Infectious diseases Inflammation Influence Longitude Longitudinal Studies Lungs Male Medical research Medicine and Health Sciences Network analysis Pediatric diseases Peers People and Places Pharynx Popularity Psychology, Social Regulators Risk Factors Social behavior Social Distance Social interactions Social Networking Social networks Social organization Social Sciences Stress, Psychological - psychology Studies |
title | Are unpopular children more likely to get sick? Longitudinal links between popularity and infectious diseases in early childhood |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-13T19%3A05%3A30IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-gale_plos_&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Are%20unpopular%20children%20more%20likely%20to%20get%20sick?%20Longitudinal%20links%20between%20popularity%20and%20infectious%20diseases%20in%20early%20childhood&rft.jtitle=PloS%20one&rft.au=Ulset,%20Vidar%20Sandsaunet&rft.date=2019-09-10&rft.volume=14&rft.issue=9&rft.spage=e0222222&rft.epage=e0222222&rft.pages=e0222222-e0222222&rft.issn=1932-6203&rft.eissn=1932-6203&rft_id=info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0222222&rft_dat=%3Cgale_plos_%3EA599005815%3C/gale_plos_%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2288192863&rft_id=info:pmid/31504058&rft_galeid=A599005815&rft_doaj_id=oai_doaj_org_article_36dd6a0364314eb08d4a477070937ecc&rfr_iscdi=true |