Conflict resolution in the Eriksen flanker task: Similarities and differences to the Simon task

In the Eriksen flanker task as well as in the Simon task irrelevant activation produces a response conflict that has to be resolved by mental control mechanisms. Despite these similarities, however, the tasks differ with respect to their delta functions, which express how the congruency effects deve...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:PloS one 2019-03, Vol.14 (3), p.e0214203-e0214203
Hauptverfasser: Hübner, Ronald, Töbel, Lisa
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page e0214203
container_issue 3
container_start_page e0214203
container_title PloS one
container_volume 14
creator Hübner, Ronald
Töbel, Lisa
description In the Eriksen flanker task as well as in the Simon task irrelevant activation produces a response conflict that has to be resolved by mental control mechanisms. Despite these similarities, however, the tasks differ with respect to their delta functions, which express how the congruency effects develop with response time. The slope of the delta function is mostly positive for the flanker task, but negative for the Simon task. Much effort has been spent to explain this difference and to investigate whether it results from task-specific control. A prominent account is that the temporal overlap between irrelevant and relevant response activation is larger in the flanker task than in the Simon task. To test this hypothesis, we increased the temporal distance in a flanker task by presenting the flankers ahead of the target. This not only produced negatively sloped delta functions but also caused reversed congruency effects. We also conducted a Simon-task experiment in which we varied the proportion of congruent stimuli. As a result, the delta function was negatively sloped only if the proportion was low. These results demonstrate that a long temporal distance is necessary but not sufficient for observing negatively sloped delta functions. Finally, we modeled the data with drift-diffusion models. Together, our results show that differently sloped delta functions can be produced with both tasks. They further indicate that activation suppression is an important control mechanism that can be adapted rather flexibly to the control demands.
doi_str_mv 10.1371/journal.pone.0214203
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>gale_plos_</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_plos_journals_2200226262</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><galeid>A580430251</galeid><doaj_id>oai_doaj_org_article_c73f080b9e0545bcbfb2c7816d09fc31</doaj_id><sourcerecordid>A580430251</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c653t-f3ec932dfd617933cbf328d940374b38186b399d575b21f62c9da5a3bc0b1ddb3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqNkluLEzEYhgdR3HX1H4gOCKIXrTnMZGa8EJayamFhwVVvQ45t2jSpSUb035vZTpeOeCG5yOn53u_AWxTPIZhD3MB3G98Hx-x8752aAwQrBPCD4hx2GM1IPj88OZ8VT2LcAFDjlpDHxRkGHYKYtOcFXXinrRGpDCp62yfjXWlcmdaqvApmG5UrtWVuq0KZWNy-L2_NzlgWTDIqlszJUhqtVVBO5Hvyd5GZyTID_7R4pJmN6tm4XxTfPl59XXyeXd98Wi4ur2eC1DjNNFYiFyu1JLDpMBZcY9TKrgK4qThuYUs47jpZNzVHUBMkOslqhrkAHErJ8UXx8qC7tz7ScTaRIgQAQiSvTCwPhPRsQ_fB7Fj4TT0z9O7BhxVlIRlhFRUN1qAFvFOgrmqei-FINC0kEnRaYJi1PozZer5TUiiXArMT0emPM2u68j8pqXBbkSYLvBkFgv_Rq5jozkShbJ608v2h7qZpOjLkevUX-u_uRmrFcgPGaZ_zikGUXtYtqDBA9aD1dkIJ75L6lVasj5Eub7_8P3vzfcq-PmHXitm0PropTsHqAIrgYwxK388MAjrY-tgcHWxNR1vnsBen874POvoY_wEzyfJ-</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Website</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2200226262</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Conflict resolution in the Eriksen flanker task: Similarities and differences to the Simon task</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</source><source>Elektronische Zeitschriftenbibliothek - Frei zugängliche E-Journals</source><source>PubMed Central</source><source>Free Full-Text Journals in Chemistry</source><source>Public Library of Science (PLoS)</source><creator>Hübner, Ronald ; Töbel, Lisa</creator><creatorcontrib>Hübner, Ronald ; Töbel, Lisa</creatorcontrib><description>In the Eriksen flanker task as well as in the Simon task irrelevant activation produces a response conflict that has to be resolved by mental control mechanisms. Despite these similarities, however, the tasks differ with respect to their delta functions, which express how the congruency effects develop with response time. The slope of the delta function is mostly positive for the flanker task, but negative for the Simon task. Much effort has been spent to explain this difference and to investigate whether it results from task-specific control. A prominent account is that the temporal overlap between irrelevant and relevant response activation is larger in the flanker task than in the Simon task. To test this hypothesis, we increased the temporal distance in a flanker task by presenting the flankers ahead of the target. This not only produced negatively sloped delta functions but also caused reversed congruency effects. We also conducted a Simon-task experiment in which we varied the proportion of congruent stimuli. As a result, the delta function was negatively sloped only if the proportion was low. These results demonstrate that a long temporal distance is necessary but not sufficient for observing negatively sloped delta functions. Finally, we modeled the data with drift-diffusion models. Together, our results show that differently sloped delta functions can be produced with both tasks. They further indicate that activation suppression is an important control mechanism that can be adapted rather flexibly to the control demands.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0214203</identifier><identifier>PMID: 30921368</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: Public Library of Science</publisher><subject>Activation ; Adult ; Analogies ; Analysis ; Attention ; Biology and Life Sciences ; Cognition &amp; reasoning ; Conflict management ; Conflict resolution ; Conflict, Psychological ; Delta function ; Diffusion models ; Experimental psychology ; Female ; Human performance ; Humans ; Male ; Mathematical models ; Medicine and Health Sciences ; Negotiating - psychology ; Paradigms ; Physical Sciences ; Psychomotor Performance ; Reaction time (Psychology) ; Research and Analysis Methods ; Response time ; Slopes ; Social Sciences ; Stimuli (Psychology)</subject><ispartof>PloS one, 2019-03, Vol.14 (3), p.e0214203-e0214203</ispartof><rights>COPYRIGHT 2019 Public Library of Science</rights><rights>2019 Hübner, Töbel. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.</rights><rights>2019 Hübner, Töbel 2019 Hübner, Töbel</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c653t-f3ec932dfd617933cbf328d940374b38186b399d575b21f62c9da5a3bc0b1ddb3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c653t-f3ec932dfd617933cbf328d940374b38186b399d575b21f62c9da5a3bc0b1ddb3</cites><orcidid>0000-0002-2788-6214</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6438467/pdf/$$EPDF$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$Hfree_for_read</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6438467/$$EHTML$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$Hfree_for_read</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,723,776,780,860,881,2096,2915,23845,27901,27902,53766,53768,79569,79570</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30921368$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Hübner, Ronald</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Töbel, Lisa</creatorcontrib><title>Conflict resolution in the Eriksen flanker task: Similarities and differences to the Simon task</title><title>PloS one</title><addtitle>PLoS One</addtitle><description>In the Eriksen flanker task as well as in the Simon task irrelevant activation produces a response conflict that has to be resolved by mental control mechanisms. Despite these similarities, however, the tasks differ with respect to their delta functions, which express how the congruency effects develop with response time. The slope of the delta function is mostly positive for the flanker task, but negative for the Simon task. Much effort has been spent to explain this difference and to investigate whether it results from task-specific control. A prominent account is that the temporal overlap between irrelevant and relevant response activation is larger in the flanker task than in the Simon task. To test this hypothesis, we increased the temporal distance in a flanker task by presenting the flankers ahead of the target. This not only produced negatively sloped delta functions but also caused reversed congruency effects. We also conducted a Simon-task experiment in which we varied the proportion of congruent stimuli. As a result, the delta function was negatively sloped only if the proportion was low. These results demonstrate that a long temporal distance is necessary but not sufficient for observing negatively sloped delta functions. Finally, we modeled the data with drift-diffusion models. Together, our results show that differently sloped delta functions can be produced with both tasks. They further indicate that activation suppression is an important control mechanism that can be adapted rather flexibly to the control demands.</description><subject>Activation</subject><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Analogies</subject><subject>Analysis</subject><subject>Attention</subject><subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject><subject>Cognition &amp; reasoning</subject><subject>Conflict management</subject><subject>Conflict resolution</subject><subject>Conflict, Psychological</subject><subject>Delta function</subject><subject>Diffusion models</subject><subject>Experimental psychology</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Human performance</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Mathematical models</subject><subject>Medicine and Health Sciences</subject><subject>Negotiating - psychology</subject><subject>Paradigms</subject><subject>Physical Sciences</subject><subject>Psychomotor Performance</subject><subject>Reaction time (Psychology)</subject><subject>Research and Analysis Methods</subject><subject>Response time</subject><subject>Slopes</subject><subject>Social Sciences</subject><subject>Stimuli (Psychology)</subject><issn>1932-6203</issn><issn>1932-6203</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2019</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>DOA</sourceid><recordid>eNqNkluLEzEYhgdR3HX1H4gOCKIXrTnMZGa8EJayamFhwVVvQ45t2jSpSUb035vZTpeOeCG5yOn53u_AWxTPIZhD3MB3G98Hx-x8752aAwQrBPCD4hx2GM1IPj88OZ8VT2LcAFDjlpDHxRkGHYKYtOcFXXinrRGpDCp62yfjXWlcmdaqvApmG5UrtWVuq0KZWNy-L2_NzlgWTDIqlszJUhqtVVBO5Hvyd5GZyTID_7R4pJmN6tm4XxTfPl59XXyeXd98Wi4ur2eC1DjNNFYiFyu1JLDpMBZcY9TKrgK4qThuYUs47jpZNzVHUBMkOslqhrkAHErJ8UXx8qC7tz7ScTaRIgQAQiSvTCwPhPRsQ_fB7Fj4TT0z9O7BhxVlIRlhFRUN1qAFvFOgrmqei-FINC0kEnRaYJi1PozZer5TUiiXArMT0emPM2u68j8pqXBbkSYLvBkFgv_Rq5jozkShbJ608v2h7qZpOjLkevUX-u_uRmrFcgPGaZ_zikGUXtYtqDBA9aD1dkIJ75L6lVasj5Eub7_8P3vzfcq-PmHXitm0PropTsHqAIrgYwxK388MAjrY-tgcHWxNR1vnsBen874POvoY_wEzyfJ-</recordid><startdate>20190328</startdate><enddate>20190328</enddate><creator>Hübner, Ronald</creator><creator>Töbel, Lisa</creator><general>Public Library of Science</general><general>Public Library of Science (PLoS)</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>IOV</scope><scope>ISR</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>7QL</scope><scope>7QO</scope><scope>7RV</scope><scope>7SN</scope><scope>7SS</scope><scope>7T5</scope><scope>7TG</scope><scope>7TM</scope><scope>7U9</scope><scope>7X2</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8C1</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FE</scope><scope>8FG</scope><scope>8FH</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABJCF</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AEUYN</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ARAPS</scope><scope>ATCPS</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BBNVY</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BGLVJ</scope><scope>BHPHI</scope><scope>C1K</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>D1I</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>H94</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>KB.</scope><scope>KB0</scope><scope>KL.</scope><scope>L6V</scope><scope>LK8</scope><scope>M0K</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M7N</scope><scope>M7P</scope><scope>M7S</scope><scope>NAPCQ</scope><scope>P5Z</scope><scope>P62</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PATMY</scope><scope>PDBOC</scope><scope>PIMPY</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PTHSS</scope><scope>PYCSY</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope><scope>DOA</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-2788-6214</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>20190328</creationdate><title>Conflict resolution in the Eriksen flanker task: Similarities and differences to the Simon task</title><author>Hübner, Ronald ; Töbel, Lisa</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c653t-f3ec932dfd617933cbf328d940374b38186b399d575b21f62c9da5a3bc0b1ddb3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2019</creationdate><topic>Activation</topic><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Analogies</topic><topic>Analysis</topic><topic>Attention</topic><topic>Biology and Life Sciences</topic><topic>Cognition &amp; reasoning</topic><topic>Conflict management</topic><topic>Conflict resolution</topic><topic>Conflict, Psychological</topic><topic>Delta function</topic><topic>Diffusion models</topic><topic>Experimental psychology</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Human performance</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Mathematical models</topic><topic>Medicine and Health Sciences</topic><topic>Negotiating - psychology</topic><topic>Paradigms</topic><topic>Physical Sciences</topic><topic>Psychomotor Performance</topic><topic>Reaction time (Psychology)</topic><topic>Research and Analysis Methods</topic><topic>Response time</topic><topic>Slopes</topic><topic>Social Sciences</topic><topic>Stimuli (Psychology)</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Hübner, Ronald</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Töbel, Lisa</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Gale In Context: Opposing Viewpoints</collection><collection>Gale In Context: Science</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B)</collection><collection>Biotechnology Research Abstracts</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Database</collection><collection>Ecology Abstracts</collection><collection>Entomology Abstracts (Full archive)</collection><collection>Immunology Abstracts</collection><collection>Meteorological &amp; Geoastrophysical Abstracts</collection><collection>Nucleic Acids Abstracts</collection><collection>Virology and AIDS Abstracts</collection><collection>Agricultural Science Collection</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Public Health Database</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>ProQuest SciTech Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Technology Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Materials Science &amp; Engineering Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Sustainability</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies &amp; Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>Agricultural &amp; Environmental Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Technology Collection</collection><collection>Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Materials Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Health &amp; Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Materials Science Database</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Meteorological &amp; Geoastrophysical Abstracts - Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest Engineering Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>Agricultural Science Database</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C)</collection><collection>Biological Science Database</collection><collection>Engineering Database</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Premium</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies &amp; Aerospace Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Advanced Technologies &amp; Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>Environmental Science Database</collection><collection>Materials Science Collection</collection><collection>Publicly Available Content Database</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>Engineering Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Science Collection</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><collection>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</collection><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Hübner, Ronald</au><au>Töbel, Lisa</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Conflict resolution in the Eriksen flanker task: Similarities and differences to the Simon task</atitle><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle><addtitle>PLoS One</addtitle><date>2019-03-28</date><risdate>2019</risdate><volume>14</volume><issue>3</issue><spage>e0214203</spage><epage>e0214203</epage><pages>e0214203-e0214203</pages><issn>1932-6203</issn><eissn>1932-6203</eissn><abstract>In the Eriksen flanker task as well as in the Simon task irrelevant activation produces a response conflict that has to be resolved by mental control mechanisms. Despite these similarities, however, the tasks differ with respect to their delta functions, which express how the congruency effects develop with response time. The slope of the delta function is mostly positive for the flanker task, but negative for the Simon task. Much effort has been spent to explain this difference and to investigate whether it results from task-specific control. A prominent account is that the temporal overlap between irrelevant and relevant response activation is larger in the flanker task than in the Simon task. To test this hypothesis, we increased the temporal distance in a flanker task by presenting the flankers ahead of the target. This not only produced negatively sloped delta functions but also caused reversed congruency effects. We also conducted a Simon-task experiment in which we varied the proportion of congruent stimuli. As a result, the delta function was negatively sloped only if the proportion was low. These results demonstrate that a long temporal distance is necessary but not sufficient for observing negatively sloped delta functions. Finally, we modeled the data with drift-diffusion models. Together, our results show that differently sloped delta functions can be produced with both tasks. They further indicate that activation suppression is an important control mechanism that can be adapted rather flexibly to the control demands.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>Public Library of Science</pub><pmid>30921368</pmid><doi>10.1371/journal.pone.0214203</doi><tpages>e0214203</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-2788-6214</orcidid><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 1932-6203
ispartof PloS one, 2019-03, Vol.14 (3), p.e0214203-e0214203
issn 1932-6203
1932-6203
language eng
recordid cdi_plos_journals_2200226262
source MEDLINE; DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals; Elektronische Zeitschriftenbibliothek - Frei zugängliche E-Journals; PubMed Central; Free Full-Text Journals in Chemistry; Public Library of Science (PLoS)
subjects Activation
Adult
Analogies
Analysis
Attention
Biology and Life Sciences
Cognition & reasoning
Conflict management
Conflict resolution
Conflict, Psychological
Delta function
Diffusion models
Experimental psychology
Female
Human performance
Humans
Male
Mathematical models
Medicine and Health Sciences
Negotiating - psychology
Paradigms
Physical Sciences
Psychomotor Performance
Reaction time (Psychology)
Research and Analysis Methods
Response time
Slopes
Social Sciences
Stimuli (Psychology)
title Conflict resolution in the Eriksen flanker task: Similarities and differences to the Simon task
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-18T07%3A13%3A34IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-gale_plos_&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Conflict%20resolution%20in%20the%20Eriksen%20flanker%20task:%20Similarities%20and%20differences%20to%20the%20Simon%20task&rft.jtitle=PloS%20one&rft.au=H%C3%BCbner,%20Ronald&rft.date=2019-03-28&rft.volume=14&rft.issue=3&rft.spage=e0214203&rft.epage=e0214203&rft.pages=e0214203-e0214203&rft.issn=1932-6203&rft.eissn=1932-6203&rft_id=info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0214203&rft_dat=%3Cgale_plos_%3EA580430251%3C/gale_plos_%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2200226262&rft_id=info:pmid/30921368&rft_galeid=A580430251&rft_doaj_id=oai_doaj_org_article_c73f080b9e0545bcbfb2c7816d09fc31&rfr_iscdi=true