Body-part compatibility effects are modulated by the tendency for women to experience negative social comparative emotions and the body-type of the model

Although exposure to physique-salient media images of women's bodies has been consistently linked with negative psychological consequences, little is known about the cognitive processes that lead to these negative effects. The present study employed a novel adaptation of a computerized response...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:PloS one 2017-06, Vol.12 (6), p.e0179552-e0179552
Hauptverfasser: Pila, Eva, Jovanov, Kimberely, Welsh, Timothy N, Sabiston, Catherine M
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page e0179552
container_issue 6
container_start_page e0179552
container_title PloS one
container_volume 12
creator Pila, Eva
Jovanov, Kimberely
Welsh, Timothy N
Sabiston, Catherine M
description Although exposure to physique-salient media images of women's bodies has been consistently linked with negative psychological consequences, little is known about the cognitive processes that lead to these negative effects. The present study employed a novel adaptation of a computerized response time (RT) task to (i) assess implicit cognitive processing when exposed to the body of another individual, and (ii) examine individual differences in social comparative emotions that may influence the cognitive processing of human bodies. Adult females with low (n = 44) or high (n = 23) tendencies for comparative emotions completed a task in which they executed responses to coloured targets presented on the hands or feet of images of ultra-thin, average-size, and above average-size female models. Although the colour of the target is the only relevant target feature, it is typically found that the to-be-ignored location of the target on the body of the model influences RTs such that RTs are shorter when the target is on a body-part that is compatible with the responding limb (e.g., hand response when target was on hand) than on a body-part that is incompatible with the responding limb (e.g., hand response when target was on foot). Findings from the present study revealed that the magnitude of the body-part compatibility effect (i.e., the index of the cognitive processing of the model) was modulated by tendencies for affective body-related comparisons. Specifically, women who were prone to experiencing social comparative emotions demonstrated stronger and more consistent body-part compatibility effects across models. Therefore, women with higher social comparison tendencies have heightened processing of bodies at a neurocognitive level and may be at higher risk of the negative outcomes linked with physique-salient media exposure.
doi_str_mv 10.1371/journal.pone.0179552
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>gale_plos_</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_plos_journals_1911580640</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><galeid>A496023729</galeid><doaj_id>oai_doaj_org_article_1543783ff9db4b55a2d1f0cac0eb7d1a</doaj_id><sourcerecordid>A496023729</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c692t-afc77a36601ed827d8ae29d955c9dd4f8ba53df85f87e6e7044780392d70c76c3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqNk9tu1DAQhiMEoqXwBggsISG42MWHxE5ukErFYaVKlTjdWo493nWVxMF2SvdReFu8h1Zd1AuUC0e_v_lnPPYUxXOC54QJ8u7ST2FQ3Xz0A8wxEU1V0QfFMWkYnXGK2cM7_0fFkxgvMa5Yzfnj4ojWnFFR8uPizwdv1rNRhYS070eVXOs6l9YIrAWdIlIBUO_N1KkEBrVrlFaAEgwGBr1G1gf02_cwoOQRXI8QXNYBDbDMVleAotdOdTvvsJOg98n5IVsPZuvWbkpI6xGQt1sh54PuafHIqi7Cs_16Uvz49PH72ZfZ-cXnxdnp-UzzhqaZsloIxTjHBExNhakV0MbkbujGmNLWraqYsXVlawEcBC5LUWPWUCOwFlyzk-LlznfsfJT7rkZJGkKqGvMSZ2KxI4xXl3IMrldhLb1yciv4sJS5f053IElVMlEzaxvTlm1VKWqIxVppDK0wRGWv9_tsU9uD0TCkoLoD08Odwa3k0l_JKpdNKpEN3uwNgv81QUyyd1FD16kB_LStm5KmrPmm7lf_oPefbk8tVT6AG6zPefXGVJ6WDceUCdpkan4PlT8DvdP5CVqX9YOAtwcBmUlwnZZqilEuvn39f_bi5yH7-g67AtWlVfTdtH1Sh2C5A3XwMQawt00mWG4m6KYbcjNBcj9BOezF3Qu6DboZGfYXGEUZaQ</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Website</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1911580640</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Body-part compatibility effects are modulated by the tendency for women to experience negative social comparative emotions and the body-type of the model</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</source><source>Public Library of Science (PLoS) Journals Open Access</source><source>EZB-FREE-00999 freely available EZB journals</source><source>PubMed Central</source><source>Free Full-Text Journals in Chemistry</source><creator>Pila, Eva ; Jovanov, Kimberely ; Welsh, Timothy N ; Sabiston, Catherine M</creator><contributor>Urgesi, Cosimo</contributor><creatorcontrib>Pila, Eva ; Jovanov, Kimberely ; Welsh, Timothy N ; Sabiston, Catherine M ; Urgesi, Cosimo</creatorcontrib><description>Although exposure to physique-salient media images of women's bodies has been consistently linked with negative psychological consequences, little is known about the cognitive processes that lead to these negative effects. The present study employed a novel adaptation of a computerized response time (RT) task to (i) assess implicit cognitive processing when exposed to the body of another individual, and (ii) examine individual differences in social comparative emotions that may influence the cognitive processing of human bodies. Adult females with low (n = 44) or high (n = 23) tendencies for comparative emotions completed a task in which they executed responses to coloured targets presented on the hands or feet of images of ultra-thin, average-size, and above average-size female models. Although the colour of the target is the only relevant target feature, it is typically found that the to-be-ignored location of the target on the body of the model influences RTs such that RTs are shorter when the target is on a body-part that is compatible with the responding limb (e.g., hand response when target was on hand) than on a body-part that is incompatible with the responding limb (e.g., hand response when target was on foot). Findings from the present study revealed that the magnitude of the body-part compatibility effect (i.e., the index of the cognitive processing of the model) was modulated by tendencies for affective body-related comparisons. Specifically, women who were prone to experiencing social comparative emotions demonstrated stronger and more consistent body-part compatibility effects across models. Therefore, women with higher social comparison tendencies have heightened processing of bodies at a neurocognitive level and may be at higher risk of the negative outcomes linked with physique-salient media exposure.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0179552</identifier><identifier>PMID: 28632746</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: Public Library of Science</publisher><subject>Adaptation ; Adolescent ; Adult ; Anorexia ; Biology and Life Sciences ; Body image ; Body Image - psychology ; Cognition ; Cognition - physiology ; Cognitive ability ; Color ; Compatibility ; Computer and Information Sciences ; Emotions ; Emotions - physiology ; Exposure ; Feet ; Female ; Females ; Humans ; Information processing ; Kinesiology ; Media ; Medicine and Health Sciences ; Neurosciences ; Photic Stimulation ; Physical education ; Physiological aspects ; Psychological aspects ; Reaction Time ; Response time (computers) ; Self image ; Social behavior ; Social Sciences ; Studies ; Surveys and Questionnaires ; Women - psychology ; Young Adult</subject><ispartof>PloS one, 2017-06, Vol.12 (6), p.e0179552-e0179552</ispartof><rights>COPYRIGHT 2017 Public Library of Science</rights><rights>2017 Pila et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.</rights><rights>2017 Pila et al 2017 Pila et al</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c692t-afc77a36601ed827d8ae29d955c9dd4f8ba53df85f87e6e7044780392d70c76c3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c692t-afc77a36601ed827d8ae29d955c9dd4f8ba53df85f87e6e7044780392d70c76c3</cites><orcidid>0000-0001-5175-4329</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5478157/pdf/$$EPDF$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$Hfree_for_read</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5478157/$$EHTML$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$Hfree_for_read</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,727,780,784,864,885,2102,2928,23866,27924,27925,53791,53793,79600,79601</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28632746$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><contributor>Urgesi, Cosimo</contributor><creatorcontrib>Pila, Eva</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Jovanov, Kimberely</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Welsh, Timothy N</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Sabiston, Catherine M</creatorcontrib><title>Body-part compatibility effects are modulated by the tendency for women to experience negative social comparative emotions and the body-type of the model</title><title>PloS one</title><addtitle>PLoS One</addtitle><description>Although exposure to physique-salient media images of women's bodies has been consistently linked with negative psychological consequences, little is known about the cognitive processes that lead to these negative effects. The present study employed a novel adaptation of a computerized response time (RT) task to (i) assess implicit cognitive processing when exposed to the body of another individual, and (ii) examine individual differences in social comparative emotions that may influence the cognitive processing of human bodies. Adult females with low (n = 44) or high (n = 23) tendencies for comparative emotions completed a task in which they executed responses to coloured targets presented on the hands or feet of images of ultra-thin, average-size, and above average-size female models. Although the colour of the target is the only relevant target feature, it is typically found that the to-be-ignored location of the target on the body of the model influences RTs such that RTs are shorter when the target is on a body-part that is compatible with the responding limb (e.g., hand response when target was on hand) than on a body-part that is incompatible with the responding limb (e.g., hand response when target was on foot). Findings from the present study revealed that the magnitude of the body-part compatibility effect (i.e., the index of the cognitive processing of the model) was modulated by tendencies for affective body-related comparisons. Specifically, women who were prone to experiencing social comparative emotions demonstrated stronger and more consistent body-part compatibility effects across models. Therefore, women with higher social comparison tendencies have heightened processing of bodies at a neurocognitive level and may be at higher risk of the negative outcomes linked with physique-salient media exposure.</description><subject>Adaptation</subject><subject>Adolescent</subject><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Anorexia</subject><subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject><subject>Body image</subject><subject>Body Image - psychology</subject><subject>Cognition</subject><subject>Cognition - physiology</subject><subject>Cognitive ability</subject><subject>Color</subject><subject>Compatibility</subject><subject>Computer and Information Sciences</subject><subject>Emotions</subject><subject>Emotions - physiology</subject><subject>Exposure</subject><subject>Feet</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Females</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Information processing</subject><subject>Kinesiology</subject><subject>Media</subject><subject>Medicine and Health Sciences</subject><subject>Neurosciences</subject><subject>Photic Stimulation</subject><subject>Physical education</subject><subject>Physiological aspects</subject><subject>Psychological aspects</subject><subject>Reaction Time</subject><subject>Response time (computers)</subject><subject>Self image</subject><subject>Social behavior</subject><subject>Social Sciences</subject><subject>Studies</subject><subject>Surveys and Questionnaires</subject><subject>Women - psychology</subject><subject>Young Adult</subject><issn>1932-6203</issn><issn>1932-6203</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2017</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>ABUWG</sourceid><sourceid>AFKRA</sourceid><sourceid>AZQEC</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>CCPQU</sourceid><sourceid>DWQXO</sourceid><sourceid>GNUQQ</sourceid><sourceid>DOA</sourceid><recordid>eNqNk9tu1DAQhiMEoqXwBggsISG42MWHxE5ukErFYaVKlTjdWo493nWVxMF2SvdReFu8h1Zd1AuUC0e_v_lnPPYUxXOC54QJ8u7ST2FQ3Xz0A8wxEU1V0QfFMWkYnXGK2cM7_0fFkxgvMa5Yzfnj4ojWnFFR8uPizwdv1rNRhYS070eVXOs6l9YIrAWdIlIBUO_N1KkEBrVrlFaAEgwGBr1G1gf02_cwoOQRXI8QXNYBDbDMVleAotdOdTvvsJOg98n5IVsPZuvWbkpI6xGQt1sh54PuafHIqi7Cs_16Uvz49PH72ZfZ-cXnxdnp-UzzhqaZsloIxTjHBExNhakV0MbkbujGmNLWraqYsXVlawEcBC5LUWPWUCOwFlyzk-LlznfsfJT7rkZJGkKqGvMSZ2KxI4xXl3IMrldhLb1yciv4sJS5f053IElVMlEzaxvTlm1VKWqIxVppDK0wRGWv9_tsU9uD0TCkoLoD08Odwa3k0l_JKpdNKpEN3uwNgv81QUyyd1FD16kB_LStm5KmrPmm7lf_oPefbk8tVT6AG6zPefXGVJ6WDceUCdpkan4PlT8DvdP5CVqX9YOAtwcBmUlwnZZqilEuvn39f_bi5yH7-g67AtWlVfTdtH1Sh2C5A3XwMQawt00mWG4m6KYbcjNBcj9BOezF3Qu6DboZGfYXGEUZaQ</recordid><startdate>20170620</startdate><enddate>20170620</enddate><creator>Pila, Eva</creator><creator>Jovanov, Kimberely</creator><creator>Welsh, Timothy N</creator><creator>Sabiston, Catherine M</creator><general>Public Library of Science</general><general>Public Library of Science (PLoS)</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>IOV</scope><scope>ISR</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>7QL</scope><scope>7QO</scope><scope>7RV</scope><scope>7SN</scope><scope>7SS</scope><scope>7T5</scope><scope>7TG</scope><scope>7TM</scope><scope>7U9</scope><scope>7X2</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8C1</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FE</scope><scope>8FG</scope><scope>8FH</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABJCF</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ARAPS</scope><scope>ATCPS</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BBNVY</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BGLVJ</scope><scope>BHPHI</scope><scope>C1K</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>D1I</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>H94</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>KB.</scope><scope>KB0</scope><scope>KL.</scope><scope>L6V</scope><scope>LK8</scope><scope>M0K</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M7N</scope><scope>M7P</scope><scope>M7S</scope><scope>NAPCQ</scope><scope>P5Z</scope><scope>P62</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PATMY</scope><scope>PDBOC</scope><scope>PIMPY</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PTHSS</scope><scope>PYCSY</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope><scope>DOA</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-5175-4329</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>20170620</creationdate><title>Body-part compatibility effects are modulated by the tendency for women to experience negative social comparative emotions and the body-type of the model</title><author>Pila, Eva ; Jovanov, Kimberely ; Welsh, Timothy N ; Sabiston, Catherine M</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c692t-afc77a36601ed827d8ae29d955c9dd4f8ba53df85f87e6e7044780392d70c76c3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2017</creationdate><topic>Adaptation</topic><topic>Adolescent</topic><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Anorexia</topic><topic>Biology and Life Sciences</topic><topic>Body image</topic><topic>Body Image - psychology</topic><topic>Cognition</topic><topic>Cognition - physiology</topic><topic>Cognitive ability</topic><topic>Color</topic><topic>Compatibility</topic><topic>Computer and Information Sciences</topic><topic>Emotions</topic><topic>Emotions - physiology</topic><topic>Exposure</topic><topic>Feet</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Females</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Information processing</topic><topic>Kinesiology</topic><topic>Media</topic><topic>Medicine and Health Sciences</topic><topic>Neurosciences</topic><topic>Photic Stimulation</topic><topic>Physical education</topic><topic>Physiological aspects</topic><topic>Psychological aspects</topic><topic>Reaction Time</topic><topic>Response time (computers)</topic><topic>Self image</topic><topic>Social behavior</topic><topic>Social Sciences</topic><topic>Studies</topic><topic>Surveys and Questionnaires</topic><topic>Women - psychology</topic><topic>Young Adult</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Pila, Eva</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Jovanov, Kimberely</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Welsh, Timothy N</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Sabiston, Catherine M</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Gale In Context: Opposing Viewpoints</collection><collection>Gale In Context: Science</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B)</collection><collection>Biotechnology Research Abstracts</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Database</collection><collection>Ecology Abstracts</collection><collection>Entomology Abstracts (Full archive)</collection><collection>Immunology Abstracts</collection><collection>Meteorological &amp; Geoastrophysical Abstracts</collection><collection>Nucleic Acids Abstracts</collection><collection>Virology and AIDS Abstracts</collection><collection>Agricultural Science Collection</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Public Health Database</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>ProQuest SciTech Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Technology Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Materials Science &amp; Engineering Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies &amp; Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>Agricultural &amp; Environmental Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Technology Collection</collection><collection>Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Materials Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Health &amp; Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Materials Science Database</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Meteorological &amp; Geoastrophysical Abstracts - Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest Engineering Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>Agricultural Science Database</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C)</collection><collection>Biological Science Database</collection><collection>Engineering Database</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Premium</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies &amp; Aerospace Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Advanced Technologies &amp; Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>Environmental Science Database</collection><collection>Materials Science Collection</collection><collection>Access via ProQuest (Open Access)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>Engineering Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Science Collection</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><collection>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</collection><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Pila, Eva</au><au>Jovanov, Kimberely</au><au>Welsh, Timothy N</au><au>Sabiston, Catherine M</au><au>Urgesi, Cosimo</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Body-part compatibility effects are modulated by the tendency for women to experience negative social comparative emotions and the body-type of the model</atitle><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle><addtitle>PLoS One</addtitle><date>2017-06-20</date><risdate>2017</risdate><volume>12</volume><issue>6</issue><spage>e0179552</spage><epage>e0179552</epage><pages>e0179552-e0179552</pages><issn>1932-6203</issn><eissn>1932-6203</eissn><abstract>Although exposure to physique-salient media images of women's bodies has been consistently linked with negative psychological consequences, little is known about the cognitive processes that lead to these negative effects. The present study employed a novel adaptation of a computerized response time (RT) task to (i) assess implicit cognitive processing when exposed to the body of another individual, and (ii) examine individual differences in social comparative emotions that may influence the cognitive processing of human bodies. Adult females with low (n = 44) or high (n = 23) tendencies for comparative emotions completed a task in which they executed responses to coloured targets presented on the hands or feet of images of ultra-thin, average-size, and above average-size female models. Although the colour of the target is the only relevant target feature, it is typically found that the to-be-ignored location of the target on the body of the model influences RTs such that RTs are shorter when the target is on a body-part that is compatible with the responding limb (e.g., hand response when target was on hand) than on a body-part that is incompatible with the responding limb (e.g., hand response when target was on foot). Findings from the present study revealed that the magnitude of the body-part compatibility effect (i.e., the index of the cognitive processing of the model) was modulated by tendencies for affective body-related comparisons. Specifically, women who were prone to experiencing social comparative emotions demonstrated stronger and more consistent body-part compatibility effects across models. Therefore, women with higher social comparison tendencies have heightened processing of bodies at a neurocognitive level and may be at higher risk of the negative outcomes linked with physique-salient media exposure.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>Public Library of Science</pub><pmid>28632746</pmid><doi>10.1371/journal.pone.0179552</doi><tpages>e0179552</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-5175-4329</orcidid><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 1932-6203
ispartof PloS one, 2017-06, Vol.12 (6), p.e0179552-e0179552
issn 1932-6203
1932-6203
language eng
recordid cdi_plos_journals_1911580640
source MEDLINE; DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals; Public Library of Science (PLoS) Journals Open Access; EZB-FREE-00999 freely available EZB journals; PubMed Central; Free Full-Text Journals in Chemistry
subjects Adaptation
Adolescent
Adult
Anorexia
Biology and Life Sciences
Body image
Body Image - psychology
Cognition
Cognition - physiology
Cognitive ability
Color
Compatibility
Computer and Information Sciences
Emotions
Emotions - physiology
Exposure
Feet
Female
Females
Humans
Information processing
Kinesiology
Media
Medicine and Health Sciences
Neurosciences
Photic Stimulation
Physical education
Physiological aspects
Psychological aspects
Reaction Time
Response time (computers)
Self image
Social behavior
Social Sciences
Studies
Surveys and Questionnaires
Women - psychology
Young Adult
title Body-part compatibility effects are modulated by the tendency for women to experience negative social comparative emotions and the body-type of the model
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-02T13%3A34%3A52IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-gale_plos_&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Body-part%20compatibility%20effects%20are%20modulated%20by%20the%20tendency%20for%20women%20to%20experience%20negative%20social%20comparative%20emotions%20and%20the%20body-type%20of%20the%20model&rft.jtitle=PloS%20one&rft.au=Pila,%20Eva&rft.date=2017-06-20&rft.volume=12&rft.issue=6&rft.spage=e0179552&rft.epage=e0179552&rft.pages=e0179552-e0179552&rft.issn=1932-6203&rft.eissn=1932-6203&rft_id=info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0179552&rft_dat=%3Cgale_plos_%3EA496023729%3C/gale_plos_%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1911580640&rft_id=info:pmid/28632746&rft_galeid=A496023729&rft_doaj_id=oai_doaj_org_article_1543783ff9db4b55a2d1f0cac0eb7d1a&rfr_iscdi=true