Collecting psycholinguistic response time data using Amazon mechanical Turk
Researchers in linguistics and related fields have recently begun exploiting online crowd-sourcing tools, like Amazon Mechanical Turk (AMT), to gather behavioral data. While this method has been successfully validated for various offline measures--grammaticality judgment or other forced-choice tasks...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | PloS one 2015-03, Vol.10 (3), p.e0116946-e0116946 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | e0116946 |
---|---|
container_issue | 3 |
container_start_page | e0116946 |
container_title | PloS one |
container_volume | 10 |
creator | Enochson, Kelly Culbertson, Jennifer |
description | Researchers in linguistics and related fields have recently begun exploiting online crowd-sourcing tools, like Amazon Mechanical Turk (AMT), to gather behavioral data. While this method has been successfully validated for various offline measures--grammaticality judgment or other forced-choice tasks--its use for mainstream psycholinguistic research remains limited. This is because psycholinguistic effects are often dependent on relatively small differences in response times, and there remains some doubt as to whether precise timing measurements can be gathered over the web. Here we show that three classic psycholinguistic effects can in fact be replicated using AMT in combination with open-source software for gathering response times client-side. Specifically, we find reliable effects of subject definiteness, filler-gap dependency processing, and agreement attraction in self-paced reading tasks using approximately the same numbers of participants and/or trials as similar laboratory studies. Our results suggest that psycholinguists can and should be taking advantage of AMT and similar online crowd-sourcing marketplaces as a fast, low-resource alternative to traditional laboratory research. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1371/journal.pone.0116946 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>gale_plos_</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_plos_journals_1667654549</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><galeid>A419036168</galeid><doaj_id>oai_doaj_org_article_afdca9f9a85946adb5193a839ba6f342</doaj_id><sourcerecordid>A419036168</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c692t-b2ecee694102dc74295a2be24b1e5998cb699ef865bb1270022f85db4c05f9393</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqNkl1v0zAUhiMEYmPwDxBEQkJw0eKPxLVvkKqKj4pJk2Bwa504J62LE3dxghi_HmfNpgbtAvkilvOc9_i8fpPkOSVzyhf03c73bQNuvvcNzgmlQmXiQXJKFWczwQh_eLQ_SZ6EsCMk51KIx8kJyyVjPJOnyZeVdw5NZ5tNug_XZutd3PY2dNakLYaoHjDtbI1pCR2kfRjIZQ1_fJPWaLbQWAMuvezbn0-TRxW4gM_G71ny_eOHy9Xn2fnFp_VqeT4zQrFuVjA0iPG6lLDSLDKmcmAFsqygmCslTSGUwkqKvCgoWxDCWCXzssgMySvFFT9LXh50984HPfoQNBViIfIszwZifSBKDzu9b20N7bX2YPXNgW83Gto4oUMNVWlAVQpkHg2EssijayC5KkBUPGNR6_3YrS9qLA02XQtuIjr909it3vhfOuMLGUWjwJtRoPVXPYZO1zYYdA4a9P3NvSXjQpCh16t_0PunG6kNxAFsU_nY1wyieplRRbigQkZqfg8VV4m1NTE0lY3nk4K3k4LIdPi720Afgl5_-_r_7MWPKfv6iN0iuG4bvOs7G7M1BbMDaFofQovVncmU6CHzt27oIfN6zHwse3H8QHdFtyHnfwExgfwZ</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Website</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1667654549</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Collecting psycholinguistic response time data using Amazon mechanical Turk</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</source><source>Elektronische Zeitschriftenbibliothek - Frei zugängliche E-Journals</source><source>Public Library of Science (PLoS)</source><source>PubMed Central</source><source>Free Full-Text Journals in Chemistry</source><creator>Enochson, Kelly ; Culbertson, Jennifer</creator><contributor>Eriksson, Kimmo</contributor><creatorcontrib>Enochson, Kelly ; Culbertson, Jennifer ; Eriksson, Kimmo</creatorcontrib><description>Researchers in linguistics and related fields have recently begun exploiting online crowd-sourcing tools, like Amazon Mechanical Turk (AMT), to gather behavioral data. While this method has been successfully validated for various offline measures--grammaticality judgment or other forced-choice tasks--its use for mainstream psycholinguistic research remains limited. This is because psycholinguistic effects are often dependent on relatively small differences in response times, and there remains some doubt as to whether precise timing measurements can be gathered over the web. Here we show that three classic psycholinguistic effects can in fact be replicated using AMT in combination with open-source software for gathering response times client-side. Specifically, we find reliable effects of subject definiteness, filler-gap dependency processing, and agreement attraction in self-paced reading tasks using approximately the same numbers of participants and/or trials as similar laboratory studies. Our results suggest that psycholinguists can and should be taking advantage of AMT and similar online crowd-sourcing marketplaces as a fast, low-resource alternative to traditional laboratory research.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0116946</identifier><identifier>PMID: 25822348</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: Public Library of Science</publisher><subject>Adult ; Cognition & reasoning ; Cognitive psychology ; Crowdsourcing - methods ; Dependence ; Electronic commerce ; Experiments ; Humans ; Hypothesis testing ; Internet ; Laboratories ; Language ; Linguistics ; Memory ; Open source software ; Psycholinguistics ; Reaction Time ; Reading ; Researchers ; Response time ; Source code ; Sourcing ; Young Adult</subject><ispartof>PloS one, 2015-03, Vol.10 (3), p.e0116946-e0116946</ispartof><rights>COPYRIGHT 2015 Public Library of Science</rights><rights>2015 Enochson, Culbertson. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.</rights><rights>2015 Enochson, Culbertson 2015 Enochson, Culbertson</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c692t-b2ecee694102dc74295a2be24b1e5998cb699ef865bb1270022f85db4c05f9393</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c692t-b2ecee694102dc74295a2be24b1e5998cb699ef865bb1270022f85db4c05f9393</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4378859/pdf/$$EPDF$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$Hfree_for_read</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4378859/$$EHTML$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$Hfree_for_read</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,727,780,784,864,885,2102,2928,23866,27924,27925,53791,53793,79600,79601</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25822348$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><contributor>Eriksson, Kimmo</contributor><creatorcontrib>Enochson, Kelly</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Culbertson, Jennifer</creatorcontrib><title>Collecting psycholinguistic response time data using Amazon mechanical Turk</title><title>PloS one</title><addtitle>PLoS One</addtitle><description>Researchers in linguistics and related fields have recently begun exploiting online crowd-sourcing tools, like Amazon Mechanical Turk (AMT), to gather behavioral data. While this method has been successfully validated for various offline measures--grammaticality judgment or other forced-choice tasks--its use for mainstream psycholinguistic research remains limited. This is because psycholinguistic effects are often dependent on relatively small differences in response times, and there remains some doubt as to whether precise timing measurements can be gathered over the web. Here we show that three classic psycholinguistic effects can in fact be replicated using AMT in combination with open-source software for gathering response times client-side. Specifically, we find reliable effects of subject definiteness, filler-gap dependency processing, and agreement attraction in self-paced reading tasks using approximately the same numbers of participants and/or trials as similar laboratory studies. Our results suggest that psycholinguists can and should be taking advantage of AMT and similar online crowd-sourcing marketplaces as a fast, low-resource alternative to traditional laboratory research.</description><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Cognition & reasoning</subject><subject>Cognitive psychology</subject><subject>Crowdsourcing - methods</subject><subject>Dependence</subject><subject>Electronic commerce</subject><subject>Experiments</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Hypothesis testing</subject><subject>Internet</subject><subject>Laboratories</subject><subject>Language</subject><subject>Linguistics</subject><subject>Memory</subject><subject>Open source software</subject><subject>Psycholinguistics</subject><subject>Reaction Time</subject><subject>Reading</subject><subject>Researchers</subject><subject>Response time</subject><subject>Source code</subject><subject>Sourcing</subject><subject>Young Adult</subject><issn>1932-6203</issn><issn>1932-6203</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2015</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>ABUWG</sourceid><sourceid>AFKRA</sourceid><sourceid>AZQEC</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>CCPQU</sourceid><sourceid>DWQXO</sourceid><sourceid>GNUQQ</sourceid><sourceid>DOA</sourceid><recordid>eNqNkl1v0zAUhiMEYmPwDxBEQkJw0eKPxLVvkKqKj4pJk2Bwa504J62LE3dxghi_HmfNpgbtAvkilvOc9_i8fpPkOSVzyhf03c73bQNuvvcNzgmlQmXiQXJKFWczwQh_eLQ_SZ6EsCMk51KIx8kJyyVjPJOnyZeVdw5NZ5tNug_XZutd3PY2dNakLYaoHjDtbI1pCR2kfRjIZQ1_fJPWaLbQWAMuvezbn0-TRxW4gM_G71ny_eOHy9Xn2fnFp_VqeT4zQrFuVjA0iPG6lLDSLDKmcmAFsqygmCslTSGUwkqKvCgoWxDCWCXzssgMySvFFT9LXh50984HPfoQNBViIfIszwZifSBKDzu9b20N7bX2YPXNgW83Gto4oUMNVWlAVQpkHg2EssijayC5KkBUPGNR6_3YrS9qLA02XQtuIjr909it3vhfOuMLGUWjwJtRoPVXPYZO1zYYdA4a9P3NvSXjQpCh16t_0PunG6kNxAFsU_nY1wyieplRRbigQkZqfg8VV4m1NTE0lY3nk4K3k4LIdPi720Afgl5_-_r_7MWPKfv6iN0iuG4bvOs7G7M1BbMDaFofQovVncmU6CHzt27oIfN6zHwse3H8QHdFtyHnfwExgfwZ</recordid><startdate>20150330</startdate><enddate>20150330</enddate><creator>Enochson, Kelly</creator><creator>Culbertson, Jennifer</creator><general>Public Library of Science</general><general>Public Library of Science (PLoS)</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>IOV</scope><scope>ISR</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>7QL</scope><scope>7QO</scope><scope>7RV</scope><scope>7SN</scope><scope>7SS</scope><scope>7T5</scope><scope>7TG</scope><scope>7TM</scope><scope>7U9</scope><scope>7X2</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8C1</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FE</scope><scope>8FG</scope><scope>8FH</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABJCF</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ARAPS</scope><scope>ATCPS</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BBNVY</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BGLVJ</scope><scope>BHPHI</scope><scope>C1K</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>D1I</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>H94</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>KB.</scope><scope>KB0</scope><scope>KL.</scope><scope>L6V</scope><scope>LK8</scope><scope>M0K</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M7N</scope><scope>M7P</scope><scope>M7S</scope><scope>NAPCQ</scope><scope>P5Z</scope><scope>P62</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PATMY</scope><scope>PDBOC</scope><scope>PIMPY</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PTHSS</scope><scope>PYCSY</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope><scope>DOA</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20150330</creationdate><title>Collecting psycholinguistic response time data using Amazon mechanical Turk</title><author>Enochson, Kelly ; Culbertson, Jennifer</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c692t-b2ecee694102dc74295a2be24b1e5998cb699ef865bb1270022f85db4c05f9393</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2015</creationdate><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Cognition & reasoning</topic><topic>Cognitive psychology</topic><topic>Crowdsourcing - methods</topic><topic>Dependence</topic><topic>Electronic commerce</topic><topic>Experiments</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Hypothesis testing</topic><topic>Internet</topic><topic>Laboratories</topic><topic>Language</topic><topic>Linguistics</topic><topic>Memory</topic><topic>Open source software</topic><topic>Psycholinguistics</topic><topic>Reaction Time</topic><topic>Reading</topic><topic>Researchers</topic><topic>Response time</topic><topic>Source code</topic><topic>Sourcing</topic><topic>Young Adult</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Enochson, Kelly</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Culbertson, Jennifer</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Opposing Viewpoints in Context (Gale)</collection><collection>Gale In Context: Science</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B)</collection><collection>Biotechnology Research Abstracts</collection><collection>Proquest Nursing & Allied Health Source</collection><collection>Ecology Abstracts</collection><collection>Entomology Abstracts (Full archive)</collection><collection>Immunology Abstracts</collection><collection>Meteorological & Geoastrophysical Abstracts</collection><collection>Nucleic Acids Abstracts</collection><collection>Virology and AIDS Abstracts</collection><collection>Agricultural Science Collection</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Public Health Database</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>ProQuest SciTech Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Technology Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Materials Science & Engineering Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>Agricultural & Environmental Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Technology Collection</collection><collection>Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Materials Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Materials Science Database</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Meteorological & Geoastrophysical Abstracts - Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest Engineering Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>Agricultural Science Database</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C)</collection><collection>Biological Science Database</collection><collection>Engineering Database</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Premium</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>Environmental Science Database</collection><collection>Materials Science Collection</collection><collection>Publicly Available Content Database</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>Engineering Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Science Collection</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><collection>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</collection><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Enochson, Kelly</au><au>Culbertson, Jennifer</au><au>Eriksson, Kimmo</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Collecting psycholinguistic response time data using Amazon mechanical Turk</atitle><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle><addtitle>PLoS One</addtitle><date>2015-03-30</date><risdate>2015</risdate><volume>10</volume><issue>3</issue><spage>e0116946</spage><epage>e0116946</epage><pages>e0116946-e0116946</pages><issn>1932-6203</issn><eissn>1932-6203</eissn><abstract>Researchers in linguistics and related fields have recently begun exploiting online crowd-sourcing tools, like Amazon Mechanical Turk (AMT), to gather behavioral data. While this method has been successfully validated for various offline measures--grammaticality judgment or other forced-choice tasks--its use for mainstream psycholinguistic research remains limited. This is because psycholinguistic effects are often dependent on relatively small differences in response times, and there remains some doubt as to whether precise timing measurements can be gathered over the web. Here we show that three classic psycholinguistic effects can in fact be replicated using AMT in combination with open-source software for gathering response times client-side. Specifically, we find reliable effects of subject definiteness, filler-gap dependency processing, and agreement attraction in self-paced reading tasks using approximately the same numbers of participants and/or trials as similar laboratory studies. Our results suggest that psycholinguists can and should be taking advantage of AMT and similar online crowd-sourcing marketplaces as a fast, low-resource alternative to traditional laboratory research.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>Public Library of Science</pub><pmid>25822348</pmid><doi>10.1371/journal.pone.0116946</doi><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 1932-6203 |
ispartof | PloS one, 2015-03, Vol.10 (3), p.e0116946-e0116946 |
issn | 1932-6203 1932-6203 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_plos_journals_1667654549 |
source | MEDLINE; DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals; Elektronische Zeitschriftenbibliothek - Frei zugängliche E-Journals; Public Library of Science (PLoS); PubMed Central; Free Full-Text Journals in Chemistry |
subjects | Adult Cognition & reasoning Cognitive psychology Crowdsourcing - methods Dependence Electronic commerce Experiments Humans Hypothesis testing Internet Laboratories Language Linguistics Memory Open source software Psycholinguistics Reaction Time Reading Researchers Response time Source code Sourcing Young Adult |
title | Collecting psycholinguistic response time data using Amazon mechanical Turk |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-06T22%3A58%3A03IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-gale_plos_&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Collecting%20psycholinguistic%20response%20time%20data%20using%20Amazon%20mechanical%20Turk&rft.jtitle=PloS%20one&rft.au=Enochson,%20Kelly&rft.date=2015-03-30&rft.volume=10&rft.issue=3&rft.spage=e0116946&rft.epage=e0116946&rft.pages=e0116946-e0116946&rft.issn=1932-6203&rft.eissn=1932-6203&rft_id=info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0116946&rft_dat=%3Cgale_plos_%3EA419036168%3C/gale_plos_%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1667654549&rft_id=info:pmid/25822348&rft_galeid=A419036168&rft_doaj_id=oai_doaj_org_article_afdca9f9a85946adb5193a839ba6f342&rfr_iscdi=true |