Evoked emotions predict food choice
In the current study we show that non-verbal food-evoked emotion scores significantly improve food choice prediction over merely liking scores. Previous research has shown that liking measures correlate with choice. However, liking is no strong predictor for food choice in real life environments. Th...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | PloS one 2014-12, Vol.9 (12), p.e115388-e115388 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , , , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | e115388 |
---|---|
container_issue | 12 |
container_start_page | e115388 |
container_title | PloS one |
container_volume | 9 |
creator | Dalenberg, Jelle R Gutjar, Swetlana Ter Horst, Gert J de Graaf, Kees Renken, Remco J Jager, Gerry |
description | In the current study we show that non-verbal food-evoked emotion scores significantly improve food choice prediction over merely liking scores. Previous research has shown that liking measures correlate with choice. However, liking is no strong predictor for food choice in real life environments. Therefore, the focus within recent studies shifted towards using emotion-profiling methods that successfully can discriminate between products that are equally liked. However, it is unclear how well scores from emotion-profiling methods predict actual food choice and/or consumption. To test this, we proposed to decompose emotion scores into valence and arousal scores using Principal Component Analysis (PCA) and apply Multinomial Logit Models (MLM) to estimate food choice using liking, valence, and arousal as possible predictors. For this analysis, we used an existing data set comprised of liking and food-evoked emotions scores from 123 participants, who rated 7 unlabeled breakfast drinks. Liking scores were measured using a 100-mm visual analogue scale, while food-evoked emotions were measured using 2 existing emotion-profiling methods: a verbal and a non-verbal method (EsSense Profile and PrEmo, respectively). After 7 days, participants were asked to choose 1 breakfast drink from the experiment to consume during breakfast in a simulated restaurant environment. Cross validation showed that we were able to correctly predict individualized food choice (1 out of 7 products) for over 50% of the participants. This number increased to nearly 80% when looking at the top 2 candidates. Model comparisons showed that evoked emotions better predict food choice than perceived liking alone. However, the strongest predictive strength was achieved by the combination of evoked emotions and liking. Furthermore we showed that non-verbal food-evoked emotion scores more accurately predict food choice than verbal food-evoked emotions scores. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1371/journal.pone.0115388 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>gale_plos_</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_plos_journals_1638003889</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><galeid>A418138939</galeid><doaj_id>oai_doaj_org_article_a8a970735d7d43b980fbf07df2dff52a</doaj_id><sourcerecordid>A418138939</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c743t-3e68e9e0cf9fe702bf0263f10f8e70658be2e65512faa803009d156af89681a13</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqNk11v0zAUhiMEYmPwDxBUmoTgosUfseNwgTRNAypNmsTXreU4x61Lamd2ssG_x2mzqUG7QJFix3nOm_OenJNlLzFaYFrg9xvfB6eaResdLBDGjArxKDvGJSVzThB9fLA_yp7FuEEoMZw_zY4IYwRTRo6z04sb_wvqGWx9Z72LszZAbXU3M97XM732VsPz7IlRTYQX43qS_fh08f38y_zy6vPy_OxyroucdnMKXEAJSJvSQIFIZRDh1GBkRHrkTFRAgDOGiVFKIIpQWWPGlRElF1hhepK93uu2jY9y9Bcl5lQglNyViVjuidqrjWyD3arwR3pl5e7Ah5VUobO6AamEKgtUUFYXdU6rUiCTEipqQ2pjGFFJ68Ne61atwFmXbtKpoG3cCTa2CoP4bR-ka4al7asoc4ERIyn445hqX22h1uC6oJpJRtM3zq7lyt_InKSk-ODk7SgQ_HUPsZNbGzU0jXLg-53psiwEw0VCT_9BHy7NSK1Ucm-d8em7ehCVZzkWmCZmoBYPUOmqYWt16iRj0_kk4N0kIDEd_O5Wqo9RLr99_X_26ueUfXPArkE13Tr6pt814RTM96AOPsYA5r7IGMlhEO6qIYdBkOMgpLBXhz_oPuiu8-lfM40CVA</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Website</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1638003889</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Evoked emotions predict food choice</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</source><source>Elektronische Zeitschriftenbibliothek - Frei zugängliche E-Journals</source><source>PubMed Central</source><source>Free Full-Text Journals in Chemistry</source><source>Public Library of Science (PLoS)</source><creator>Dalenberg, Jelle R ; Gutjar, Swetlana ; Ter Horst, Gert J ; de Graaf, Kees ; Renken, Remco J ; Jager, Gerry</creator><contributor>Friese, Malte</contributor><creatorcontrib>Dalenberg, Jelle R ; Gutjar, Swetlana ; Ter Horst, Gert J ; de Graaf, Kees ; Renken, Remco J ; Jager, Gerry ; Friese, Malte</creatorcontrib><description>In the current study we show that non-verbal food-evoked emotion scores significantly improve food choice prediction over merely liking scores. Previous research has shown that liking measures correlate with choice. However, liking is no strong predictor for food choice in real life environments. Therefore, the focus within recent studies shifted towards using emotion-profiling methods that successfully can discriminate between products that are equally liked. However, it is unclear how well scores from emotion-profiling methods predict actual food choice and/or consumption. To test this, we proposed to decompose emotion scores into valence and arousal scores using Principal Component Analysis (PCA) and apply Multinomial Logit Models (MLM) to estimate food choice using liking, valence, and arousal as possible predictors. For this analysis, we used an existing data set comprised of liking and food-evoked emotions scores from 123 participants, who rated 7 unlabeled breakfast drinks. Liking scores were measured using a 100-mm visual analogue scale, while food-evoked emotions were measured using 2 existing emotion-profiling methods: a verbal and a non-verbal method (EsSense Profile and PrEmo, respectively). After 7 days, participants were asked to choose 1 breakfast drink from the experiment to consume during breakfast in a simulated restaurant environment. Cross validation showed that we were able to correctly predict individualized food choice (1 out of 7 products) for over 50% of the participants. This number increased to nearly 80% when looking at the top 2 candidates. Model comparisons showed that evoked emotions better predict food choice than perceived liking alone. However, the strongest predictive strength was achieved by the combination of evoked emotions and liking. Furthermore we showed that non-verbal food-evoked emotion scores more accurately predict food choice than verbal food-evoked emotions scores.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0115388</identifier><identifier>PMID: 25521352</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: Public Library of Science</publisher><subject>Adolescent ; Adult ; Arousal ; Behavior ; Beverages ; Biology and Life Sciences ; Choice Behavior ; Computer simulation ; Consumers ; consumption ; Correlation analysis ; Emotions ; faces ; Female ; Food ; Food consumption ; Food Preferences - psychology ; Food production ; Food products ; Food science ; Humans ; Language ; Logit models ; Male ; Medical imaging ; Medicine and Health Sciences ; Nutrition ; Predictions ; Principal components analysis ; Restaurants ; Social Sciences ; Studies ; words</subject><ispartof>PloS one, 2014-12, Vol.9 (12), p.e115388-e115388</ispartof><rights>COPYRIGHT 2014 Public Library of Science</rights><rights>2014 Dalenberg et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.</rights><rights>2014 Dalenberg et al 2014 Dalenberg et al</rights><rights>Wageningen University & Research</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c743t-3e68e9e0cf9fe702bf0263f10f8e70658be2e65512faa803009d156af89681a13</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c743t-3e68e9e0cf9fe702bf0263f10f8e70658be2e65512faa803009d156af89681a13</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4270769/pdf/$$EPDF$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$Hfree_for_read</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4270769/$$EHTML$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$Hfree_for_read</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,723,776,780,860,881,2096,2915,23845,27901,27902,53766,53768,79343,79344</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25521352$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><contributor>Friese, Malte</contributor><creatorcontrib>Dalenberg, Jelle R</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Gutjar, Swetlana</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Ter Horst, Gert J</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>de Graaf, Kees</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Renken, Remco J</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Jager, Gerry</creatorcontrib><title>Evoked emotions predict food choice</title><title>PloS one</title><addtitle>PLoS One</addtitle><description>In the current study we show that non-verbal food-evoked emotion scores significantly improve food choice prediction over merely liking scores. Previous research has shown that liking measures correlate with choice. However, liking is no strong predictor for food choice in real life environments. Therefore, the focus within recent studies shifted towards using emotion-profiling methods that successfully can discriminate between products that are equally liked. However, it is unclear how well scores from emotion-profiling methods predict actual food choice and/or consumption. To test this, we proposed to decompose emotion scores into valence and arousal scores using Principal Component Analysis (PCA) and apply Multinomial Logit Models (MLM) to estimate food choice using liking, valence, and arousal as possible predictors. For this analysis, we used an existing data set comprised of liking and food-evoked emotions scores from 123 participants, who rated 7 unlabeled breakfast drinks. Liking scores were measured using a 100-mm visual analogue scale, while food-evoked emotions were measured using 2 existing emotion-profiling methods: a verbal and a non-verbal method (EsSense Profile and PrEmo, respectively). After 7 days, participants were asked to choose 1 breakfast drink from the experiment to consume during breakfast in a simulated restaurant environment. Cross validation showed that we were able to correctly predict individualized food choice (1 out of 7 products) for over 50% of the participants. This number increased to nearly 80% when looking at the top 2 candidates. Model comparisons showed that evoked emotions better predict food choice than perceived liking alone. However, the strongest predictive strength was achieved by the combination of evoked emotions and liking. Furthermore we showed that non-verbal food-evoked emotion scores more accurately predict food choice than verbal food-evoked emotions scores.</description><subject>Adolescent</subject><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Arousal</subject><subject>Behavior</subject><subject>Beverages</subject><subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject><subject>Choice Behavior</subject><subject>Computer simulation</subject><subject>Consumers</subject><subject>consumption</subject><subject>Correlation analysis</subject><subject>Emotions</subject><subject>faces</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Food</subject><subject>Food consumption</subject><subject>Food Preferences - psychology</subject><subject>Food production</subject><subject>Food products</subject><subject>Food science</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Language</subject><subject>Logit models</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Medical imaging</subject><subject>Medicine and Health Sciences</subject><subject>Nutrition</subject><subject>Predictions</subject><subject>Principal components analysis</subject><subject>Restaurants</subject><subject>Social Sciences</subject><subject>Studies</subject><subject>words</subject><issn>1932-6203</issn><issn>1932-6203</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2014</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>DOA</sourceid><recordid>eNqNk11v0zAUhiMEYmPwDxBUmoTgosUfseNwgTRNAypNmsTXreU4x61Lamd2ssG_x2mzqUG7QJFix3nOm_OenJNlLzFaYFrg9xvfB6eaResdLBDGjArxKDvGJSVzThB9fLA_yp7FuEEoMZw_zY4IYwRTRo6z04sb_wvqGWx9Z72LszZAbXU3M97XM732VsPz7IlRTYQX43qS_fh08f38y_zy6vPy_OxyroucdnMKXEAJSJvSQIFIZRDh1GBkRHrkTFRAgDOGiVFKIIpQWWPGlRElF1hhepK93uu2jY9y9Bcl5lQglNyViVjuidqrjWyD3arwR3pl5e7Ah5VUobO6AamEKgtUUFYXdU6rUiCTEipqQ2pjGFFJ68Ne61atwFmXbtKpoG3cCTa2CoP4bR-ka4al7asoc4ERIyn445hqX22h1uC6oJpJRtM3zq7lyt_InKSk-ODk7SgQ_HUPsZNbGzU0jXLg-53psiwEw0VCT_9BHy7NSK1Ucm-d8em7ehCVZzkWmCZmoBYPUOmqYWt16iRj0_kk4N0kIDEd_O5Wqo9RLr99_X_26ueUfXPArkE13Tr6pt814RTM96AOPsYA5r7IGMlhEO6qIYdBkOMgpLBXhz_oPuiu8-lfM40CVA</recordid><startdate>20141218</startdate><enddate>20141218</enddate><creator>Dalenberg, Jelle R</creator><creator>Gutjar, Swetlana</creator><creator>Ter Horst, Gert J</creator><creator>de Graaf, Kees</creator><creator>Renken, Remco J</creator><creator>Jager, Gerry</creator><general>Public Library of Science</general><general>Public Library of Science (PLoS)</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>IOV</scope><scope>ISR</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>7QL</scope><scope>7QO</scope><scope>7RV</scope><scope>7SN</scope><scope>7SS</scope><scope>7T5</scope><scope>7TG</scope><scope>7TM</scope><scope>7U9</scope><scope>7X2</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8C1</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FE</scope><scope>8FG</scope><scope>8FH</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABJCF</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AEUYN</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ARAPS</scope><scope>ATCPS</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BBNVY</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BGLVJ</scope><scope>BHPHI</scope><scope>C1K</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>D1I</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>H94</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>KB.</scope><scope>KB0</scope><scope>KL.</scope><scope>L6V</scope><scope>LK8</scope><scope>M0K</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M7N</scope><scope>M7P</scope><scope>M7S</scope><scope>NAPCQ</scope><scope>P5Z</scope><scope>P62</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PATMY</scope><scope>PDBOC</scope><scope>PIMPY</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PTHSS</scope><scope>PYCSY</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope><scope>QVL</scope><scope>DOA</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20141218</creationdate><title>Evoked emotions predict food choice</title><author>Dalenberg, Jelle R ; Gutjar, Swetlana ; Ter Horst, Gert J ; de Graaf, Kees ; Renken, Remco J ; Jager, Gerry</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c743t-3e68e9e0cf9fe702bf0263f10f8e70658be2e65512faa803009d156af89681a13</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2014</creationdate><topic>Adolescent</topic><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Arousal</topic><topic>Behavior</topic><topic>Beverages</topic><topic>Biology and Life Sciences</topic><topic>Choice Behavior</topic><topic>Computer simulation</topic><topic>Consumers</topic><topic>consumption</topic><topic>Correlation analysis</topic><topic>Emotions</topic><topic>faces</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Food</topic><topic>Food consumption</topic><topic>Food Preferences - psychology</topic><topic>Food production</topic><topic>Food products</topic><topic>Food science</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Language</topic><topic>Logit models</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Medical imaging</topic><topic>Medicine and Health Sciences</topic><topic>Nutrition</topic><topic>Predictions</topic><topic>Principal components analysis</topic><topic>Restaurants</topic><topic>Social Sciences</topic><topic>Studies</topic><topic>words</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Dalenberg, Jelle R</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Gutjar, Swetlana</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Ter Horst, Gert J</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>de Graaf, Kees</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Renken, Remco J</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Jager, Gerry</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Opposing Viewpoints in Context (Gale)</collection><collection>Gale In Context: Science</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B)</collection><collection>Biotechnology Research Abstracts</collection><collection>Proquest Nursing & Allied Health Source</collection><collection>Ecology Abstracts</collection><collection>Entomology Abstracts (Full archive)</collection><collection>Immunology Abstracts</collection><collection>Meteorological & Geoastrophysical Abstracts</collection><collection>Nucleic Acids Abstracts</collection><collection>Virology and AIDS Abstracts</collection><collection>Agricultural Science Collection</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Public Health Database</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>ProQuest SciTech Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Technology Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Materials Science & Engineering Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Sustainability</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>Agricultural & Environmental Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Technology Collection</collection><collection>Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Materials Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Materials Science Database</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Meteorological & Geoastrophysical Abstracts - Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest Engineering Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>Agricultural Science Database</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C)</collection><collection>Biological Science Database</collection><collection>Engineering Database</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Premium</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>Environmental Science Database</collection><collection>Materials Science Collection</collection><collection>Publicly Available Content Database</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>Engineering Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Science Collection</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><collection>NARCIS:Publications</collection><collection>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</collection><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Dalenberg, Jelle R</au><au>Gutjar, Swetlana</au><au>Ter Horst, Gert J</au><au>de Graaf, Kees</au><au>Renken, Remco J</au><au>Jager, Gerry</au><au>Friese, Malte</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Evoked emotions predict food choice</atitle><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle><addtitle>PLoS One</addtitle><date>2014-12-18</date><risdate>2014</risdate><volume>9</volume><issue>12</issue><spage>e115388</spage><epage>e115388</epage><pages>e115388-e115388</pages><issn>1932-6203</issn><eissn>1932-6203</eissn><abstract>In the current study we show that non-verbal food-evoked emotion scores significantly improve food choice prediction over merely liking scores. Previous research has shown that liking measures correlate with choice. However, liking is no strong predictor for food choice in real life environments. Therefore, the focus within recent studies shifted towards using emotion-profiling methods that successfully can discriminate between products that are equally liked. However, it is unclear how well scores from emotion-profiling methods predict actual food choice and/or consumption. To test this, we proposed to decompose emotion scores into valence and arousal scores using Principal Component Analysis (PCA) and apply Multinomial Logit Models (MLM) to estimate food choice using liking, valence, and arousal as possible predictors. For this analysis, we used an existing data set comprised of liking and food-evoked emotions scores from 123 participants, who rated 7 unlabeled breakfast drinks. Liking scores were measured using a 100-mm visual analogue scale, while food-evoked emotions were measured using 2 existing emotion-profiling methods: a verbal and a non-verbal method (EsSense Profile and PrEmo, respectively). After 7 days, participants were asked to choose 1 breakfast drink from the experiment to consume during breakfast in a simulated restaurant environment. Cross validation showed that we were able to correctly predict individualized food choice (1 out of 7 products) for over 50% of the participants. This number increased to nearly 80% when looking at the top 2 candidates. Model comparisons showed that evoked emotions better predict food choice than perceived liking alone. However, the strongest predictive strength was achieved by the combination of evoked emotions and liking. Furthermore we showed that non-verbal food-evoked emotion scores more accurately predict food choice than verbal food-evoked emotions scores.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>Public Library of Science</pub><pmid>25521352</pmid><doi>10.1371/journal.pone.0115388</doi><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 1932-6203 |
ispartof | PloS one, 2014-12, Vol.9 (12), p.e115388-e115388 |
issn | 1932-6203 1932-6203 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_plos_journals_1638003889 |
source | MEDLINE; DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals; Elektronische Zeitschriftenbibliothek - Frei zugängliche E-Journals; PubMed Central; Free Full-Text Journals in Chemistry; Public Library of Science (PLoS) |
subjects | Adolescent Adult Arousal Behavior Beverages Biology and Life Sciences Choice Behavior Computer simulation Consumers consumption Correlation analysis Emotions faces Female Food Food consumption Food Preferences - psychology Food production Food products Food science Humans Language Logit models Male Medical imaging Medicine and Health Sciences Nutrition Predictions Principal components analysis Restaurants Social Sciences Studies words |
title | Evoked emotions predict food choice |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-30T06%3A47%3A10IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-gale_plos_&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Evoked%20emotions%20predict%20food%20choice&rft.jtitle=PloS%20one&rft.au=Dalenberg,%20Jelle%20R&rft.date=2014-12-18&rft.volume=9&rft.issue=12&rft.spage=e115388&rft.epage=e115388&rft.pages=e115388-e115388&rft.issn=1932-6203&rft.eissn=1932-6203&rft_id=info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0115388&rft_dat=%3Cgale_plos_%3EA418138939%3C/gale_plos_%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1638003889&rft_id=info:pmid/25521352&rft_galeid=A418138939&rft_doaj_id=oai_doaj_org_article_a8a970735d7d43b980fbf07df2dff52a&rfr_iscdi=true |