Small-aperture monovision and the Pulfrich experience: absence of neural adaptation effects
To explore whether adaptation reduces the interocular visual latency differences and the induced Pulfrich effect caused by the anisocoria implicit in small-aperture monovision. Anisocoric vision was simulated in two adults by wearing in the non-dominant eye for 7 successive days, while awake, an opa...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | PloS one 2013-10, Vol.8 (10), p.e75987-e75987 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , , , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | e75987 |
---|---|
container_issue | 10 |
container_start_page | e75987 |
container_title | PloS one |
container_volume | 8 |
creator | Plainis, Sotiris Petratou, Dionysia Giannakopoulou, Trisevgeni Radhakrishnan, Hema Pallikaris, Ioannis G Charman, W Neil |
description | To explore whether adaptation reduces the interocular visual latency differences and the induced Pulfrich effect caused by the anisocoria implicit in small-aperture monovision.
Anisocoric vision was simulated in two adults by wearing in the non-dominant eye for 7 successive days, while awake, an opaque soft contact lens (CL) with a small, central, circular aperture. This was repeated with aperture diameters of 1.5 and 2.5 mm. Each day, monocular and binocular pattern-reversal Visual Evoked Potentials (VEP) were recorded. Additionally, the Pulfrich effect was measured: the task of the subject was to state whether a a 2-deg spot appeared in front or behind the plane of a central cross when moved left-to-right or right-to-left on a display screen. The retinal illuminance of the dominant eye was varied using neutral density (ND) filters to establish the ND value which eliminated the Pulfrich effect for each lens. All experiments were performed at luminance levels of 5 and 30 cd/m(2).
Interocular differences in monocular VEP latency (at 30 cd/m(2)) rose to about 12-15 ms and 20-25 ms when the CL aperture was 2.5 and 1.5 mm, respectively. The effect was more pronounced at 5 cd/m(2) (i.e. with larger natural pupils). A strong Pulfrich effect was observed under all conditions, with the effect being less striking for the 2.5 mm aperture. No neural adaptation appeared to occur: neither the interocular differences in VEP latency nor the ND value required to null the Pulfrich effect reduced over each 7-day period of anisocoric vision.
Small-aperture monovision produced marked interocular differences in visual latency and a Pulfrich experience. These were not reduced by adaptation, perhaps because the natural pupil diameter of the dominant eye was continually changing throughout the day due to varying illumination and other factors, making adaptation difficult. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1371/journal.pone.0075987 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>gale_plos_</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_plos_journals_1441864779</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><galeid>A478272652</galeid><doaj_id>oai_doaj_org_article_4f543ed4d3d14c76a77abf3dee8b1b0a</doaj_id><sourcerecordid>A478272652</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c692t-2a37c7515d023f6fd76659305fd0d3c55f007b57a8f405040436f4435ab0b7883</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqNk8tu1DAUhiMEoqXwBggiISFYzGDHt4QFUlVxGalSEQU2LCzHlxmPnHiwnaq8PU4nrSaoC5SFLfv7_-Nzck5RPIdgCRGD77Z-CL1wy53v9RIARpqaPSiOYYOqBa0AeniwPyqexLgFgKCa0sfFUYUhIXUNj4tfl51wbiF2OqQh6LLzvb-y0fq-FL0q00aXXwdngpWbUl9nyupe6velaOO4Kb0pez0E4UqhxC6JNCq1MVqm-LR4ZISL-tm0nhQ_Pn38fvZlcX7xeXV2er6QtKnSohKISUYgUaBChhrFKCUNAsQooJAkxOTsWsJEbTAgAAOMqMEYEdGCltU1Oile7n13zkc-1SVyiDGsKWasycRqTygvtnwXbCfCH-6F5TcHPqy5CMlKpzk2BCOtsEIKYsmoYEy0Bimt6xa2QGSvD1O0oe20krpPOf2Z6fymtxu-9lccsYYSVGWDN5NB8L8HHRPvbJTaOdFrP9y8mzQQVwRl9NU_6P3ZTdRa5ARsb3yOK0dTfopZXbGKkjHs8h4qf0p3VuYmMjafzwRvZ4LMJH2d1mKIka8uv_0_e_Fzzr4-YDdauLSJ3g1j68Q5iPegDD7GoM1dkSHg4wzcVoOPM8CnGciyF4c_6E502_ToLw8HATw</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Website</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1441864779</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Small-aperture monovision and the Pulfrich experience: absence of neural adaptation effects</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</source><source>Public Library of Science (PLoS) Journals Open Access</source><source>EZB-FREE-00999 freely available EZB journals</source><source>PubMed Central</source><source>Free Full-Text Journals in Chemistry</source><creator>Plainis, Sotiris ; Petratou, Dionysia ; Giannakopoulou, Trisevgeni ; Radhakrishnan, Hema ; Pallikaris, Ioannis G ; Charman, W Neil</creator><contributor>Vavvas, Demetrios</contributor><creatorcontrib>Plainis, Sotiris ; Petratou, Dionysia ; Giannakopoulou, Trisevgeni ; Radhakrishnan, Hema ; Pallikaris, Ioannis G ; Charman, W Neil ; Vavvas, Demetrios</creatorcontrib><description>To explore whether adaptation reduces the interocular visual latency differences and the induced Pulfrich effect caused by the anisocoria implicit in small-aperture monovision.
Anisocoric vision was simulated in two adults by wearing in the non-dominant eye for 7 successive days, while awake, an opaque soft contact lens (CL) with a small, central, circular aperture. This was repeated with aperture diameters of 1.5 and 2.5 mm. Each day, monocular and binocular pattern-reversal Visual Evoked Potentials (VEP) were recorded. Additionally, the Pulfrich effect was measured: the task of the subject was to state whether a a 2-deg spot appeared in front or behind the plane of a central cross when moved left-to-right or right-to-left on a display screen. The retinal illuminance of the dominant eye was varied using neutral density (ND) filters to establish the ND value which eliminated the Pulfrich effect for each lens. All experiments were performed at luminance levels of 5 and 30 cd/m(2).
Interocular differences in monocular VEP latency (at 30 cd/m(2)) rose to about 12-15 ms and 20-25 ms when the CL aperture was 2.5 and 1.5 mm, respectively. The effect was more pronounced at 5 cd/m(2) (i.e. with larger natural pupils). A strong Pulfrich effect was observed under all conditions, with the effect being less striking for the 2.5 mm aperture. No neural adaptation appeared to occur: neither the interocular differences in VEP latency nor the ND value required to null the Pulfrich effect reduced over each 7-day period of anisocoric vision.
Small-aperture monovision produced marked interocular differences in visual latency and a Pulfrich experience. These were not reduced by adaptation, perhaps because the natural pupil diameter of the dominant eye was continually changing throughout the day due to varying illumination and other factors, making adaptation difficult.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0075987</identifier><identifier>PMID: 24155881</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: Public Library of Science</publisher><subject>Adaptation ; Adaptation, Ocular - physiology ; Adult ; Adults ; Binocular vision ; Cataracts ; Contact Lenses ; Cornea ; Electrodes ; Evoked Potentials, Visual - physiology ; Eye ; Eye lens ; Humans ; Illuminance ; Latency ; Life sciences ; Luminance ; Luminescence ; Presbyopia ; Pupils ; Reaction Time - physiology ; Retina ; Time Factors ; Vision ; Vision, Monocular - physiology ; Visual evoked potentials</subject><ispartof>PloS one, 2013-10, Vol.8 (10), p.e75987-e75987</ispartof><rights>COPYRIGHT 2013 Public Library of Science</rights><rights>2013 Plainis et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.</rights><rights>2013 Plainis et al 2013 Plainis et al</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c692t-2a37c7515d023f6fd76659305fd0d3c55f007b57a8f405040436f4435ab0b7883</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c692t-2a37c7515d023f6fd76659305fd0d3c55f007b57a8f405040436f4435ab0b7883</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3796532/pdf/$$EPDF$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$Hfree_for_read</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3796532/$$EHTML$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$Hfree_for_read</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,723,776,780,860,881,2095,2914,23846,27903,27904,53769,53771,79346,79347</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24155881$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><contributor>Vavvas, Demetrios</contributor><creatorcontrib>Plainis, Sotiris</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Petratou, Dionysia</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Giannakopoulou, Trisevgeni</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Radhakrishnan, Hema</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Pallikaris, Ioannis G</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Charman, W Neil</creatorcontrib><title>Small-aperture monovision and the Pulfrich experience: absence of neural adaptation effects</title><title>PloS one</title><addtitle>PLoS One</addtitle><description>To explore whether adaptation reduces the interocular visual latency differences and the induced Pulfrich effect caused by the anisocoria implicit in small-aperture monovision.
Anisocoric vision was simulated in two adults by wearing in the non-dominant eye for 7 successive days, while awake, an opaque soft contact lens (CL) with a small, central, circular aperture. This was repeated with aperture diameters of 1.5 and 2.5 mm. Each day, monocular and binocular pattern-reversal Visual Evoked Potentials (VEP) were recorded. Additionally, the Pulfrich effect was measured: the task of the subject was to state whether a a 2-deg spot appeared in front or behind the plane of a central cross when moved left-to-right or right-to-left on a display screen. The retinal illuminance of the dominant eye was varied using neutral density (ND) filters to establish the ND value which eliminated the Pulfrich effect for each lens. All experiments were performed at luminance levels of 5 and 30 cd/m(2).
Interocular differences in monocular VEP latency (at 30 cd/m(2)) rose to about 12-15 ms and 20-25 ms when the CL aperture was 2.5 and 1.5 mm, respectively. The effect was more pronounced at 5 cd/m(2) (i.e. with larger natural pupils). A strong Pulfrich effect was observed under all conditions, with the effect being less striking for the 2.5 mm aperture. No neural adaptation appeared to occur: neither the interocular differences in VEP latency nor the ND value required to null the Pulfrich effect reduced over each 7-day period of anisocoric vision.
Small-aperture monovision produced marked interocular differences in visual latency and a Pulfrich experience. These were not reduced by adaptation, perhaps because the natural pupil diameter of the dominant eye was continually changing throughout the day due to varying illumination and other factors, making adaptation difficult.</description><subject>Adaptation</subject><subject>Adaptation, Ocular - physiology</subject><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Adults</subject><subject>Binocular vision</subject><subject>Cataracts</subject><subject>Contact Lenses</subject><subject>Cornea</subject><subject>Electrodes</subject><subject>Evoked Potentials, Visual - physiology</subject><subject>Eye</subject><subject>Eye lens</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Illuminance</subject><subject>Latency</subject><subject>Life sciences</subject><subject>Luminance</subject><subject>Luminescence</subject><subject>Presbyopia</subject><subject>Pupils</subject><subject>Reaction Time - physiology</subject><subject>Retina</subject><subject>Time Factors</subject><subject>Vision</subject><subject>Vision, Monocular - physiology</subject><subject>Visual evoked potentials</subject><issn>1932-6203</issn><issn>1932-6203</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2013</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>ABUWG</sourceid><sourceid>AFKRA</sourceid><sourceid>AZQEC</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>CCPQU</sourceid><sourceid>DWQXO</sourceid><sourceid>GNUQQ</sourceid><sourceid>DOA</sourceid><recordid>eNqNk8tu1DAUhiMEoqXwBggiISFYzGDHt4QFUlVxGalSEQU2LCzHlxmPnHiwnaq8PU4nrSaoC5SFLfv7_-Nzck5RPIdgCRGD77Z-CL1wy53v9RIARpqaPSiOYYOqBa0AeniwPyqexLgFgKCa0sfFUYUhIXUNj4tfl51wbiF2OqQh6LLzvb-y0fq-FL0q00aXXwdngpWbUl9nyupe6velaOO4Kb0pez0E4UqhxC6JNCq1MVqm-LR4ZISL-tm0nhQ_Pn38fvZlcX7xeXV2er6QtKnSohKISUYgUaBChhrFKCUNAsQooJAkxOTsWsJEbTAgAAOMqMEYEdGCltU1Oile7n13zkc-1SVyiDGsKWasycRqTygvtnwXbCfCH-6F5TcHPqy5CMlKpzk2BCOtsEIKYsmoYEy0Bimt6xa2QGSvD1O0oe20krpPOf2Z6fymtxu-9lccsYYSVGWDN5NB8L8HHRPvbJTaOdFrP9y8mzQQVwRl9NU_6P3ZTdRa5ARsb3yOK0dTfopZXbGKkjHs8h4qf0p3VuYmMjafzwRvZ4LMJH2d1mKIka8uv_0_e_Fzzr4-YDdauLSJ3g1j68Q5iPegDD7GoM1dkSHg4wzcVoOPM8CnGciyF4c_6E502_ToLw8HATw</recordid><startdate>20131014</startdate><enddate>20131014</enddate><creator>Plainis, Sotiris</creator><creator>Petratou, Dionysia</creator><creator>Giannakopoulou, Trisevgeni</creator><creator>Radhakrishnan, Hema</creator><creator>Pallikaris, Ioannis G</creator><creator>Charman, W Neil</creator><general>Public Library of Science</general><general>Public Library of Science (PLoS)</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>IOV</scope><scope>ISR</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>7QL</scope><scope>7QO</scope><scope>7RV</scope><scope>7SN</scope><scope>7SS</scope><scope>7T5</scope><scope>7TG</scope><scope>7TM</scope><scope>7U9</scope><scope>7X2</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8C1</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FE</scope><scope>8FG</scope><scope>8FH</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABJCF</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AEUYN</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ARAPS</scope><scope>ATCPS</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BBNVY</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BGLVJ</scope><scope>BHPHI</scope><scope>C1K</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>D1I</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>H94</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>KB.</scope><scope>KB0</scope><scope>KL.</scope><scope>L6V</scope><scope>LK8</scope><scope>M0K</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M7N</scope><scope>M7P</scope><scope>M7S</scope><scope>NAPCQ</scope><scope>P5Z</scope><scope>P62</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PATMY</scope><scope>PDBOC</scope><scope>PIMPY</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PTHSS</scope><scope>PYCSY</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope><scope>DOA</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20131014</creationdate><title>Small-aperture monovision and the Pulfrich experience: absence of neural adaptation effects</title><author>Plainis, Sotiris ; Petratou, Dionysia ; Giannakopoulou, Trisevgeni ; Radhakrishnan, Hema ; Pallikaris, Ioannis G ; Charman, W Neil</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c692t-2a37c7515d023f6fd76659305fd0d3c55f007b57a8f405040436f4435ab0b7883</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2013</creationdate><topic>Adaptation</topic><topic>Adaptation, Ocular - physiology</topic><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Adults</topic><topic>Binocular vision</topic><topic>Cataracts</topic><topic>Contact Lenses</topic><topic>Cornea</topic><topic>Electrodes</topic><topic>Evoked Potentials, Visual - physiology</topic><topic>Eye</topic><topic>Eye lens</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Illuminance</topic><topic>Latency</topic><topic>Life sciences</topic><topic>Luminance</topic><topic>Luminescence</topic><topic>Presbyopia</topic><topic>Pupils</topic><topic>Reaction Time - physiology</topic><topic>Retina</topic><topic>Time Factors</topic><topic>Vision</topic><topic>Vision, Monocular - physiology</topic><topic>Visual evoked potentials</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Plainis, Sotiris</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Petratou, Dionysia</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Giannakopoulou, Trisevgeni</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Radhakrishnan, Hema</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Pallikaris, Ioannis G</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Charman, W Neil</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Gale In Context: Opposing Viewpoints</collection><collection>Gale In Context: Science</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B)</collection><collection>Biotechnology Research Abstracts</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Database</collection><collection>Ecology Abstracts</collection><collection>Entomology Abstracts (Full archive)</collection><collection>Immunology Abstracts</collection><collection>Meteorological & Geoastrophysical Abstracts</collection><collection>Nucleic Acids Abstracts</collection><collection>Virology and AIDS Abstracts</collection><collection>Agricultural Science Collection</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Public Health Database</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>ProQuest SciTech Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Technology Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Materials Science & Engineering Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Sustainability</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>Agricultural & Environmental Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Technology Collection</collection><collection>Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Materials Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Materials Science Database</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Meteorological & Geoastrophysical Abstracts - Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest Engineering Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>Agricultural Science Database</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C)</collection><collection>Biological Science Database</collection><collection>Engineering Database</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Premium</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>Environmental Science Database</collection><collection>Materials Science Collection</collection><collection>Publicly Available Content Database</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>Engineering Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Science Collection</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><collection>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</collection><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Plainis, Sotiris</au><au>Petratou, Dionysia</au><au>Giannakopoulou, Trisevgeni</au><au>Radhakrishnan, Hema</au><au>Pallikaris, Ioannis G</au><au>Charman, W Neil</au><au>Vavvas, Demetrios</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Small-aperture monovision and the Pulfrich experience: absence of neural adaptation effects</atitle><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle><addtitle>PLoS One</addtitle><date>2013-10-14</date><risdate>2013</risdate><volume>8</volume><issue>10</issue><spage>e75987</spage><epage>e75987</epage><pages>e75987-e75987</pages><issn>1932-6203</issn><eissn>1932-6203</eissn><abstract>To explore whether adaptation reduces the interocular visual latency differences and the induced Pulfrich effect caused by the anisocoria implicit in small-aperture monovision.
Anisocoric vision was simulated in two adults by wearing in the non-dominant eye for 7 successive days, while awake, an opaque soft contact lens (CL) with a small, central, circular aperture. This was repeated with aperture diameters of 1.5 and 2.5 mm. Each day, monocular and binocular pattern-reversal Visual Evoked Potentials (VEP) were recorded. Additionally, the Pulfrich effect was measured: the task of the subject was to state whether a a 2-deg spot appeared in front or behind the plane of a central cross when moved left-to-right or right-to-left on a display screen. The retinal illuminance of the dominant eye was varied using neutral density (ND) filters to establish the ND value which eliminated the Pulfrich effect for each lens. All experiments were performed at luminance levels of 5 and 30 cd/m(2).
Interocular differences in monocular VEP latency (at 30 cd/m(2)) rose to about 12-15 ms and 20-25 ms when the CL aperture was 2.5 and 1.5 mm, respectively. The effect was more pronounced at 5 cd/m(2) (i.e. with larger natural pupils). A strong Pulfrich effect was observed under all conditions, with the effect being less striking for the 2.5 mm aperture. No neural adaptation appeared to occur: neither the interocular differences in VEP latency nor the ND value required to null the Pulfrich effect reduced over each 7-day period of anisocoric vision.
Small-aperture monovision produced marked interocular differences in visual latency and a Pulfrich experience. These were not reduced by adaptation, perhaps because the natural pupil diameter of the dominant eye was continually changing throughout the day due to varying illumination and other factors, making adaptation difficult.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>Public Library of Science</pub><pmid>24155881</pmid><doi>10.1371/journal.pone.0075987</doi><tpages>e75987</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 1932-6203 |
ispartof | PloS one, 2013-10, Vol.8 (10), p.e75987-e75987 |
issn | 1932-6203 1932-6203 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_plos_journals_1441864779 |
source | MEDLINE; DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals; Public Library of Science (PLoS) Journals Open Access; EZB-FREE-00999 freely available EZB journals; PubMed Central; Free Full-Text Journals in Chemistry |
subjects | Adaptation Adaptation, Ocular - physiology Adult Adults Binocular vision Cataracts Contact Lenses Cornea Electrodes Evoked Potentials, Visual - physiology Eye Eye lens Humans Illuminance Latency Life sciences Luminance Luminescence Presbyopia Pupils Reaction Time - physiology Retina Time Factors Vision Vision, Monocular - physiology Visual evoked potentials |
title | Small-aperture monovision and the Pulfrich experience: absence of neural adaptation effects |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-26T12%3A42%3A00IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-gale_plos_&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Small-aperture%20monovision%20and%20the%20Pulfrich%20experience:%20absence%20of%20neural%20adaptation%20effects&rft.jtitle=PloS%20one&rft.au=Plainis,%20Sotiris&rft.date=2013-10-14&rft.volume=8&rft.issue=10&rft.spage=e75987&rft.epage=e75987&rft.pages=e75987-e75987&rft.issn=1932-6203&rft.eissn=1932-6203&rft_id=info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0075987&rft_dat=%3Cgale_plos_%3EA478272652%3C/gale_plos_%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1441864779&rft_id=info:pmid/24155881&rft_galeid=A478272652&rft_doaj_id=oai_doaj_org_article_4f543ed4d3d14c76a77abf3dee8b1b0a&rfr_iscdi=true |