Lifelong exposure to multilingualism: new evidence to support cognitive reserve hypothesis

Investigate the protective effect of multilingualism on cognition in seniors. As part of the MemoVie study conducted on 232 non-demented volunteers aged 65 and more, neurogeriatric and neuropsychological evaluations were performed. Participants were classified as presenting either cognitive impairme...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:PloS one 2013-04, Vol.8 (4), p.e62030-e62030
Hauptverfasser: Perquin, Magali, Vaillant, Michel, Schuller, Anne-Marie, Pastore, Jessica, Dartigues, Jean-François, Lair, Marie-Lise, Diederich, Nico
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page e62030
container_issue 4
container_start_page e62030
container_title PloS one
container_volume 8
creator Perquin, Magali
Vaillant, Michel
Schuller, Anne-Marie
Pastore, Jessica
Dartigues, Jean-François
Lair, Marie-Lise
Diederich, Nico
description Investigate the protective effect of multilingualism on cognition in seniors. As part of the MemoVie study conducted on 232 non-demented volunteers aged 65 and more, neurogeriatric and neuropsychological evaluations were performed. Participants were classified as presenting either cognitive impairment without dementia (CIND) or being free of any cognitive impairment (CIND-free). Language practices, socio-demographic data and lifestyle habits were recorded. In this retrospective nested case-control design, we used as proxies of multilingualism: number of languages practiced, age of acquisition and duration of practice, emphasizing the temporal pattern of acquisition, and the resulting practice of several languages sequentially or concomitantly during various periods of life. This special angle on the matter offered to our work a dimension particularly original and innovative. 44 subjects (19%) had CIND, the others were cognitively normal. All practiced from 2 to 7 languages. When compared with bilinguals, participants who practiced more than 2 languages presented a lower risk of CIND, after adjustment for education and age (odds ratio (OR) = 0.30, 95% confidence limits (95%CL) = [0.10-0.92]). Progressing from 2 to 3 languages, instead of staying bilingual, was associated with a 7-fold protection against CIND (OR = 0.14, 95%CL = [0.04-0.45], p = 0.0010). A one year delay to reach multilingualism (3 languages practiced being the threshold) multiplied the risk of CIND by 1.022 (OR = 1.022, 95%CL = [1.01-1.04], p = 0.0044). Also noteworthy, just as for multilingualism, an impact of cognitively stimulating activities on the occurrence of CIND was found as well (OR = 0.979, 95%CL = [0.961-0.998], p = 0.033). The study did not show independence of multilingualism and CIND. Rather it seems to show a strong association toward a protection against CIND. Practicing multilingualism from early life on, and/or learning it at a fast pace is even more efficient. This protection might be related to the enhancement of cognitive reserve and brain plasticity, thereby preserving brain functions from alterations during aging.
doi_str_mv 10.1371/journal.pone.0062030
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_plos_</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_plos_journals_1347594723</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><doaj_id>oai_doaj_org_article_692a069390f345a2a52adafbd2050d68</doaj_id><sourcerecordid>1349095780</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c592t-f3c715d200797035d7bc47561af8273ec85995473da54e910a35ac7e4f66b48f3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNptUk1v1DAQjRCIlsI_QBCJSy-7jO3YjjkgoapApZW4wIWL5XUmWa-8drCThf57srtp1SJO44_33rwZvaJ4TWBJmCTvt3FMwfhlHwMuAQQFBk-Kc6IYXRwuTx-cz4oXOW8BOKuFeF6cUSYqQQg7L36uXIs-hq7EP33MY8JyiOVu9IPzLnSj8S7vPpQBf5e4dw0GewTkse9jGkobu-AGt8cyYcY01c1tH4cNZpdfFs9a4zO-mutF8ePz9ferr4vVty83V59WC8sVHRYts5LwhgJIJYHxRq5tJbkgpq2pZGhrrhSvJGsMr1ARMIwbK7FqhVhXdcsuircn3d7HrOe1ZE3YpKIqSdmEuDkhmmi2uk9uZ9Ktjsbp40NMnTZpcNajFooaEIopaFnFDTWcmsa068kfh0bUk9bHudu43mFjMQzJ-Eeij3-C2-gu7vW0cgCqJoHLWSDFXyPmQe9ctui9CRjHo28FissaJui7f6D_n646oWyKOSds780Q0Ieo3LH0ISp6jspEe_NwkHvSXTbYXwvUvXY</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Website</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1347594723</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Lifelong exposure to multilingualism: new evidence to support cognitive reserve hypothesis</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</source><source>Elektronische Zeitschriftenbibliothek - Frei zugängliche E-Journals</source><source>PubMed Central</source><source>Free Full-Text Journals in Chemistry</source><source>Public Library of Science (PLoS)</source><creator>Perquin, Magali ; Vaillant, Michel ; Schuller, Anne-Marie ; Pastore, Jessica ; Dartigues, Jean-François ; Lair, Marie-Lise ; Diederich, Nico</creator><creatorcontrib>Perquin, Magali ; Vaillant, Michel ; Schuller, Anne-Marie ; Pastore, Jessica ; Dartigues, Jean-François ; Lair, Marie-Lise ; Diederich, Nico ; MemoVie Group ; on behalf of the MemoVie Group</creatorcontrib><description>Investigate the protective effect of multilingualism on cognition in seniors. As part of the MemoVie study conducted on 232 non-demented volunteers aged 65 and more, neurogeriatric and neuropsychological evaluations were performed. Participants were classified as presenting either cognitive impairment without dementia (CIND) or being free of any cognitive impairment (CIND-free). Language practices, socio-demographic data and lifestyle habits were recorded. In this retrospective nested case-control design, we used as proxies of multilingualism: number of languages practiced, age of acquisition and duration of practice, emphasizing the temporal pattern of acquisition, and the resulting practice of several languages sequentially or concomitantly during various periods of life. This special angle on the matter offered to our work a dimension particularly original and innovative. 44 subjects (19%) had CIND, the others were cognitively normal. All practiced from 2 to 7 languages. When compared with bilinguals, participants who practiced more than 2 languages presented a lower risk of CIND, after adjustment for education and age (odds ratio (OR) = 0.30, 95% confidence limits (95%CL) = [0.10-0.92]). Progressing from 2 to 3 languages, instead of staying bilingual, was associated with a 7-fold protection against CIND (OR = 0.14, 95%CL = [0.04-0.45], p = 0.0010). A one year delay to reach multilingualism (3 languages practiced being the threshold) multiplied the risk of CIND by 1.022 (OR = 1.022, 95%CL = [1.01-1.04], p = 0.0044). Also noteworthy, just as for multilingualism, an impact of cognitively stimulating activities on the occurrence of CIND was found as well (OR = 0.979, 95%CL = [0.961-0.998], p = 0.033). The study did not show independence of multilingualism and CIND. Rather it seems to show a strong association toward a protection against CIND. Practicing multilingualism from early life on, and/or learning it at a fast pace is even more efficient. This protection might be related to the enhancement of cognitive reserve and brain plasticity, thereby preserving brain functions from alterations during aging.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0062030</identifier><identifier>PMID: 23646113</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: Public Library of Science</publisher><subject>Aged ; Aged, 80 and over ; Aging ; Alzheimer Disease - prevention &amp; control ; Alzheimer's disease ; Bilingualism ; Biology ; Brain ; Cognition ; Cognition Disorders - prevention &amp; control ; Cognitive ability ; Cognitive Reserve ; Confidence limits ; Confounding Factors (Epidemiology) ; Dementia ; Dementia - prevention &amp; control ; Dementia disorders ; Demographics ; Education ; Habits ; Humans ; Hypotheses ; Impairment ; Languages ; Leisure ; Medical screening ; Medicine ; Multilingualism ; Neuropsychological Tests ; Odds Ratio ; Retrospective Studies ; Risk Factors ; Social and Behavioral Sciences ; Studies</subject><ispartof>PloS one, 2013-04, Vol.8 (4), p.e62030-e62030</ispartof><rights>2013 Perquin et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.</rights><rights>2013 Perquin et al 2013 Perquin et al</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c592t-f3c715d200797035d7bc47561af8273ec85995473da54e910a35ac7e4f66b48f3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c592t-f3c715d200797035d7bc47561af8273ec85995473da54e910a35ac7e4f66b48f3</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3640029/pdf/$$EPDF$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$Hfree_for_read</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3640029/$$EHTML$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$Hfree_for_read</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,723,776,780,860,881,2096,2915,23845,27901,27902,53766,53768,79342,79343</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23646113$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Perquin, Magali</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Vaillant, Michel</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Schuller, Anne-Marie</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Pastore, Jessica</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Dartigues, Jean-François</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Lair, Marie-Lise</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Diederich, Nico</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>MemoVie Group</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>on behalf of the MemoVie Group</creatorcontrib><title>Lifelong exposure to multilingualism: new evidence to support cognitive reserve hypothesis</title><title>PloS one</title><addtitle>PLoS One</addtitle><description>Investigate the protective effect of multilingualism on cognition in seniors. As part of the MemoVie study conducted on 232 non-demented volunteers aged 65 and more, neurogeriatric and neuropsychological evaluations were performed. Participants were classified as presenting either cognitive impairment without dementia (CIND) or being free of any cognitive impairment (CIND-free). Language practices, socio-demographic data and lifestyle habits were recorded. In this retrospective nested case-control design, we used as proxies of multilingualism: number of languages practiced, age of acquisition and duration of practice, emphasizing the temporal pattern of acquisition, and the resulting practice of several languages sequentially or concomitantly during various periods of life. This special angle on the matter offered to our work a dimension particularly original and innovative. 44 subjects (19%) had CIND, the others were cognitively normal. All practiced from 2 to 7 languages. When compared with bilinguals, participants who practiced more than 2 languages presented a lower risk of CIND, after adjustment for education and age (odds ratio (OR) = 0.30, 95% confidence limits (95%CL) = [0.10-0.92]). Progressing from 2 to 3 languages, instead of staying bilingual, was associated with a 7-fold protection against CIND (OR = 0.14, 95%CL = [0.04-0.45], p = 0.0010). A one year delay to reach multilingualism (3 languages practiced being the threshold) multiplied the risk of CIND by 1.022 (OR = 1.022, 95%CL = [1.01-1.04], p = 0.0044). Also noteworthy, just as for multilingualism, an impact of cognitively stimulating activities on the occurrence of CIND was found as well (OR = 0.979, 95%CL = [0.961-0.998], p = 0.033). The study did not show independence of multilingualism and CIND. Rather it seems to show a strong association toward a protection against CIND. Practicing multilingualism from early life on, and/or learning it at a fast pace is even more efficient. This protection might be related to the enhancement of cognitive reserve and brain plasticity, thereby preserving brain functions from alterations during aging.</description><subject>Aged</subject><subject>Aged, 80 and over</subject><subject>Aging</subject><subject>Alzheimer Disease - prevention &amp; control</subject><subject>Alzheimer's disease</subject><subject>Bilingualism</subject><subject>Biology</subject><subject>Brain</subject><subject>Cognition</subject><subject>Cognition Disorders - prevention &amp; control</subject><subject>Cognitive ability</subject><subject>Cognitive Reserve</subject><subject>Confidence limits</subject><subject>Confounding Factors (Epidemiology)</subject><subject>Dementia</subject><subject>Dementia - prevention &amp; control</subject><subject>Dementia disorders</subject><subject>Demographics</subject><subject>Education</subject><subject>Habits</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Hypotheses</subject><subject>Impairment</subject><subject>Languages</subject><subject>Leisure</subject><subject>Medical screening</subject><subject>Medicine</subject><subject>Multilingualism</subject><subject>Neuropsychological Tests</subject><subject>Odds Ratio</subject><subject>Retrospective Studies</subject><subject>Risk Factors</subject><subject>Social and Behavioral Sciences</subject><subject>Studies</subject><issn>1932-6203</issn><issn>1932-6203</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2013</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>DOA</sourceid><recordid>eNptUk1v1DAQjRCIlsI_QBCJSy-7jO3YjjkgoapApZW4wIWL5XUmWa-8drCThf57srtp1SJO44_33rwZvaJ4TWBJmCTvt3FMwfhlHwMuAQQFBk-Kc6IYXRwuTx-cz4oXOW8BOKuFeF6cUSYqQQg7L36uXIs-hq7EP33MY8JyiOVu9IPzLnSj8S7vPpQBf5e4dw0GewTkse9jGkobu-AGt8cyYcY01c1tH4cNZpdfFs9a4zO-mutF8ePz9ferr4vVty83V59WC8sVHRYts5LwhgJIJYHxRq5tJbkgpq2pZGhrrhSvJGsMr1ARMIwbK7FqhVhXdcsuircn3d7HrOe1ZE3YpKIqSdmEuDkhmmi2uk9uZ9Ktjsbp40NMnTZpcNajFooaEIopaFnFDTWcmsa068kfh0bUk9bHudu43mFjMQzJ-Eeij3-C2-gu7vW0cgCqJoHLWSDFXyPmQe9ctui9CRjHo28FissaJui7f6D_n646oWyKOSds780Q0Ieo3LH0ISp6jspEe_NwkHvSXTbYXwvUvXY</recordid><startdate>20130430</startdate><enddate>20130430</enddate><creator>Perquin, Magali</creator><creator>Vaillant, Michel</creator><creator>Schuller, Anne-Marie</creator><creator>Pastore, Jessica</creator><creator>Dartigues, Jean-François</creator><creator>Lair, Marie-Lise</creator><creator>Diederich, Nico</creator><general>Public Library of Science</general><general>Public Library of Science (PLoS)</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>7QL</scope><scope>7QO</scope><scope>7RV</scope><scope>7SN</scope><scope>7SS</scope><scope>7T5</scope><scope>7TG</scope><scope>7TM</scope><scope>7U9</scope><scope>7X2</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8C1</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FE</scope><scope>8FG</scope><scope>8FH</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABJCF</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AEUYN</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ARAPS</scope><scope>ATCPS</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BBNVY</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BGLVJ</scope><scope>BHPHI</scope><scope>C1K</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>D1I</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>H94</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>KB.</scope><scope>KB0</scope><scope>KL.</scope><scope>L6V</scope><scope>LK8</scope><scope>M0K</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M7N</scope><scope>M7P</scope><scope>M7S</scope><scope>NAPCQ</scope><scope>P5Z</scope><scope>P62</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PATMY</scope><scope>PDBOC</scope><scope>PIMPY</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PTHSS</scope><scope>PYCSY</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope><scope>DOA</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20130430</creationdate><title>Lifelong exposure to multilingualism: new evidence to support cognitive reserve hypothesis</title><author>Perquin, Magali ; Vaillant, Michel ; Schuller, Anne-Marie ; Pastore, Jessica ; Dartigues, Jean-François ; Lair, Marie-Lise ; Diederich, Nico</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c592t-f3c715d200797035d7bc47561af8273ec85995473da54e910a35ac7e4f66b48f3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2013</creationdate><topic>Aged</topic><topic>Aged, 80 and over</topic><topic>Aging</topic><topic>Alzheimer Disease - prevention &amp; control</topic><topic>Alzheimer's disease</topic><topic>Bilingualism</topic><topic>Biology</topic><topic>Brain</topic><topic>Cognition</topic><topic>Cognition Disorders - prevention &amp; control</topic><topic>Cognitive ability</topic><topic>Cognitive Reserve</topic><topic>Confidence limits</topic><topic>Confounding Factors (Epidemiology)</topic><topic>Dementia</topic><topic>Dementia - prevention &amp; control</topic><topic>Dementia disorders</topic><topic>Demographics</topic><topic>Education</topic><topic>Habits</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Hypotheses</topic><topic>Impairment</topic><topic>Languages</topic><topic>Leisure</topic><topic>Medical screening</topic><topic>Medicine</topic><topic>Multilingualism</topic><topic>Neuropsychological Tests</topic><topic>Odds Ratio</topic><topic>Retrospective Studies</topic><topic>Risk Factors</topic><topic>Social and Behavioral Sciences</topic><topic>Studies</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Perquin, Magali</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Vaillant, Michel</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Schuller, Anne-Marie</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Pastore, Jessica</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Dartigues, Jean-François</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Lair, Marie-Lise</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Diederich, Nico</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>MemoVie Group</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>on behalf of the MemoVie Group</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B)</collection><collection>Biotechnology Research Abstracts</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Database</collection><collection>Ecology Abstracts</collection><collection>Entomology Abstracts (Full archive)</collection><collection>Immunology Abstracts</collection><collection>Meteorological &amp; Geoastrophysical Abstracts</collection><collection>Nucleic Acids Abstracts</collection><collection>Virology and AIDS Abstracts</collection><collection>Agricultural Science Collection</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Public Health Database</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>ProQuest SciTech Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Technology Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Materials Science &amp; Engineering Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Sustainability</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies &amp; Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>Agricultural &amp; Environmental Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Technology Collection</collection><collection>Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Materials Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Health &amp; Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Materials Science Database</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Meteorological &amp; Geoastrophysical Abstracts - Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest Engineering Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>Agricultural Science Database</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C)</collection><collection>Biological Science Database</collection><collection>Engineering Database</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Premium</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies &amp; Aerospace Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Advanced Technologies &amp; Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>Environmental Science Database</collection><collection>Materials Science Collection</collection><collection>Publicly Available Content Database</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>Engineering Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Science Collection</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><collection>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</collection><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Perquin, Magali</au><au>Vaillant, Michel</au><au>Schuller, Anne-Marie</au><au>Pastore, Jessica</au><au>Dartigues, Jean-François</au><au>Lair, Marie-Lise</au><au>Diederich, Nico</au><aucorp>MemoVie Group</aucorp><aucorp>on behalf of the MemoVie Group</aucorp><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Lifelong exposure to multilingualism: new evidence to support cognitive reserve hypothesis</atitle><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle><addtitle>PLoS One</addtitle><date>2013-04-30</date><risdate>2013</risdate><volume>8</volume><issue>4</issue><spage>e62030</spage><epage>e62030</epage><pages>e62030-e62030</pages><issn>1932-6203</issn><eissn>1932-6203</eissn><abstract>Investigate the protective effect of multilingualism on cognition in seniors. As part of the MemoVie study conducted on 232 non-demented volunteers aged 65 and more, neurogeriatric and neuropsychological evaluations were performed. Participants were classified as presenting either cognitive impairment without dementia (CIND) or being free of any cognitive impairment (CIND-free). Language practices, socio-demographic data and lifestyle habits were recorded. In this retrospective nested case-control design, we used as proxies of multilingualism: number of languages practiced, age of acquisition and duration of practice, emphasizing the temporal pattern of acquisition, and the resulting practice of several languages sequentially or concomitantly during various periods of life. This special angle on the matter offered to our work a dimension particularly original and innovative. 44 subjects (19%) had CIND, the others were cognitively normal. All practiced from 2 to 7 languages. When compared with bilinguals, participants who practiced more than 2 languages presented a lower risk of CIND, after adjustment for education and age (odds ratio (OR) = 0.30, 95% confidence limits (95%CL) = [0.10-0.92]). Progressing from 2 to 3 languages, instead of staying bilingual, was associated with a 7-fold protection against CIND (OR = 0.14, 95%CL = [0.04-0.45], p = 0.0010). A one year delay to reach multilingualism (3 languages practiced being the threshold) multiplied the risk of CIND by 1.022 (OR = 1.022, 95%CL = [1.01-1.04], p = 0.0044). Also noteworthy, just as for multilingualism, an impact of cognitively stimulating activities on the occurrence of CIND was found as well (OR = 0.979, 95%CL = [0.961-0.998], p = 0.033). The study did not show independence of multilingualism and CIND. Rather it seems to show a strong association toward a protection against CIND. Practicing multilingualism from early life on, and/or learning it at a fast pace is even more efficient. This protection might be related to the enhancement of cognitive reserve and brain plasticity, thereby preserving brain functions from alterations during aging.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>Public Library of Science</pub><pmid>23646113</pmid><doi>10.1371/journal.pone.0062030</doi><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 1932-6203
ispartof PloS one, 2013-04, Vol.8 (4), p.e62030-e62030
issn 1932-6203
1932-6203
language eng
recordid cdi_plos_journals_1347594723
source MEDLINE; DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals; Elektronische Zeitschriftenbibliothek - Frei zugängliche E-Journals; PubMed Central; Free Full-Text Journals in Chemistry; Public Library of Science (PLoS)
subjects Aged
Aged, 80 and over
Aging
Alzheimer Disease - prevention & control
Alzheimer's disease
Bilingualism
Biology
Brain
Cognition
Cognition Disorders - prevention & control
Cognitive ability
Cognitive Reserve
Confidence limits
Confounding Factors (Epidemiology)
Dementia
Dementia - prevention & control
Dementia disorders
Demographics
Education
Habits
Humans
Hypotheses
Impairment
Languages
Leisure
Medical screening
Medicine
Multilingualism
Neuropsychological Tests
Odds Ratio
Retrospective Studies
Risk Factors
Social and Behavioral Sciences
Studies
title Lifelong exposure to multilingualism: new evidence to support cognitive reserve hypothesis
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-08T07%3A51%3A58IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_plos_&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Lifelong%20exposure%20to%20multilingualism:%20new%20evidence%20to%20support%20cognitive%20reserve%20hypothesis&rft.jtitle=PloS%20one&rft.au=Perquin,%20Magali&rft.aucorp=MemoVie%20Group&rft.date=2013-04-30&rft.volume=8&rft.issue=4&rft.spage=e62030&rft.epage=e62030&rft.pages=e62030-e62030&rft.issn=1932-6203&rft.eissn=1932-6203&rft_id=info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0062030&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_plos_%3E1349095780%3C/proquest_plos_%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1347594723&rft_id=info:pmid/23646113&rft_doaj_id=oai_doaj_org_article_692a069390f345a2a52adafbd2050d68&rfr_iscdi=true