You say yes, I say no: Investigating the link between meaning and form in response particles

Response particles, like English ‘yes’ and ‘no’, are used to respond to polar questions or assertions and are found in all languages. However, the number of particles and the specific meanings they convey vary across languages. For example, in some languages particles mainly convey whether the respo...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Glossa (London) 2023-03, Vol.8 (1), p.1
Hauptverfasser: Maldonado, Mora, Culbertson, Jennifer
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page
container_issue 1
container_start_page 1
container_title Glossa (London)
container_volume 8
creator Maldonado, Mora
Culbertson, Jennifer
description Response particles, like English ‘yes’ and ‘no’, are used to respond to polar questions or assertions and are found in all languages. However, the number of particles and the specific meanings they convey vary across languages. For example, in some languages particles mainly convey whether the response itself is positive or negative, while in others they convey whether the response is agreeing or disagreeing with previous discourse. Further, some languages have two response particles, while others have three, or even four. Recent work suggests that how meanings tend to be mapped to forms cross-linguistically might nevertheless be constrained. Roelofsen & Farkas (2015) suggest that indicating disagreement with a negative question or assertion (e.g., A: ‘Ally doesn’t eat meat.’ B: ‘Yes, he does.’) is more marked than indicating agreement with a positive assertion (e.g., A: ‘Ally eats meat.’ B: ‘Yes, he does’.). This difference in semantic markedness is argued to lead to a difference in form: more marked meanings are mapped to more specialized forms. Here we investigate this hypothesis in a series of behavioral experiments. Across our experiments, we find that participants are indeed sensitive to the differences in meaning that particles can convey. However, not all of the differences implicated by the hierarchy hypothesized in Roelofsen & Farkas (2015) are supported by our results, and we find evidence highlighting an unexpected special role for Positive Agreement—the least marked meaning.
doi_str_mv 10.16995/glossa.9185
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>gale_doaj_</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_hal_primary_oai_HAL_hal_04408197v1</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><galeid>A776459509</galeid><doaj_id>oai_doaj_org_article_49e7e09a82784024a0c59901582ad8b6</doaj_id><sourcerecordid>A776459509</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c369t-e45e5def57ba5117ab25fc94fa35219cd2b17fa8b2b8d14af0f3ba53554fe9173</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNpVkUFrGzEQhZeSQEOaW36AroXY1WglS-rNhKYxGHppD4GAmN0dbZSuJSNtU_zvu_aGkjCHGZ7efCN4VXUNfAkra9WXfkil4NKCUR-qC1FbvQBTq7M388fqqpRnzjlYAWDgonp8SH9YwQM7ULlhm9MY01e2iS9UxtDjGGLPxidiQ4i_WUPjX6LIdoTx-ICxYz7lHQuRZSr7FAuxPeYxtAOVT9W5x6HQ1Wu_rH7dfft5e7_Y_vi-uV1vF229suOCpCLVkVe6QQWgsRHKt1Z6rJUA23aiAe3RNKIxHUj03NeTs1ZKerKg68tqM3O7hM9un8MO88ElDO4kpNy71y85aUkTt2iENpILibxV1nJQRmBnmtXE-jyznnB4h7pfb91R41JyA1a_wORdzt4eJ3SIPo0Z26k62oU2RfJh0tdar6Syittp4WZeaPOUVSb__wJwd0rRzSm6Y4r1P1iVj5E</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Website</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype></control><display><type>article</type><title>You say yes, I say no: Investigating the link between meaning and form in response particles</title><source>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</source><source>EZB-FREE-00999 freely available EZB journals</source><source>Alma/SFX Local Collection</source><creator>Maldonado, Mora ; Culbertson, Jennifer</creator><creatorcontrib>Maldonado, Mora ; Culbertson, Jennifer</creatorcontrib><description>Response particles, like English ‘yes’ and ‘no’, are used to respond to polar questions or assertions and are found in all languages. However, the number of particles and the specific meanings they convey vary across languages. For example, in some languages particles mainly convey whether the response itself is positive or negative, while in others they convey whether the response is agreeing or disagreeing with previous discourse. Further, some languages have two response particles, while others have three, or even four. Recent work suggests that how meanings tend to be mapped to forms cross-linguistically might nevertheless be constrained. Roelofsen &amp; Farkas (2015) suggest that indicating disagreement with a negative question or assertion (e.g., A: ‘Ally doesn’t eat meat.’ B: ‘Yes, he does.’) is more marked than indicating agreement with a positive assertion (e.g., A: ‘Ally eats meat.’ B: ‘Yes, he does’.). This difference in semantic markedness is argued to lead to a difference in form: more marked meanings are mapped to more specialized forms. Here we investigate this hypothesis in a series of behavioral experiments. Across our experiments, we find that participants are indeed sensitive to the differences in meaning that particles can convey. However, not all of the differences implicated by the hierarchy hypothesized in Roelofsen &amp; Farkas (2015) are supported by our results, and we find evidence highlighting an unexpected special role for Positive Agreement—the least marked meaning.</description><identifier>ISSN: 2397-1835</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 2397-1835</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.16995/glossa.9185</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Open Library of Humanities</publisher><subject>artificial language learning ; Cognitive science ; Evaluation ; Linguistics ; Meaning (Philosophy) ; response particles ; semantics ; syntax ; typology</subject><ispartof>Glossa (London), 2023-03, Vol.8 (1), p.1</ispartof><rights>COPYRIGHT 2023 Open Library of Humanities</rights><rights>Distributed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c369t-e45e5def57ba5117ab25fc94fa35219cd2b17fa8b2b8d14af0f3ba53554fe9173</cites><orcidid>0000-0002-1737-6296 ; 0000-0003-4421-2614</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>230,314,778,782,862,883,2098,27907,27908</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://hal.science/hal-04408197$$DView record in HAL$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Maldonado, Mora</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Culbertson, Jennifer</creatorcontrib><title>You say yes, I say no: Investigating the link between meaning and form in response particles</title><title>Glossa (London)</title><description>Response particles, like English ‘yes’ and ‘no’, are used to respond to polar questions or assertions and are found in all languages. However, the number of particles and the specific meanings they convey vary across languages. For example, in some languages particles mainly convey whether the response itself is positive or negative, while in others they convey whether the response is agreeing or disagreeing with previous discourse. Further, some languages have two response particles, while others have three, or even four. Recent work suggests that how meanings tend to be mapped to forms cross-linguistically might nevertheless be constrained. Roelofsen &amp; Farkas (2015) suggest that indicating disagreement with a negative question or assertion (e.g., A: ‘Ally doesn’t eat meat.’ B: ‘Yes, he does.’) is more marked than indicating agreement with a positive assertion (e.g., A: ‘Ally eats meat.’ B: ‘Yes, he does’.). This difference in semantic markedness is argued to lead to a difference in form: more marked meanings are mapped to more specialized forms. Here we investigate this hypothesis in a series of behavioral experiments. Across our experiments, we find that participants are indeed sensitive to the differences in meaning that particles can convey. However, not all of the differences implicated by the hierarchy hypothesized in Roelofsen &amp; Farkas (2015) are supported by our results, and we find evidence highlighting an unexpected special role for Positive Agreement—the least marked meaning.</description><subject>artificial language learning</subject><subject>Cognitive science</subject><subject>Evaluation</subject><subject>Linguistics</subject><subject>Meaning (Philosophy)</subject><subject>response particles</subject><subject>semantics</subject><subject>syntax</subject><subject>typology</subject><issn>2397-1835</issn><issn>2397-1835</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2023</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>DOA</sourceid><recordid>eNpVkUFrGzEQhZeSQEOaW36AroXY1WglS-rNhKYxGHppD4GAmN0dbZSuJSNtU_zvu_aGkjCHGZ7efCN4VXUNfAkra9WXfkil4NKCUR-qC1FbvQBTq7M388fqqpRnzjlYAWDgonp8SH9YwQM7ULlhm9MY01e2iS9UxtDjGGLPxidiQ4i_WUPjX6LIdoTx-ICxYz7lHQuRZSr7FAuxPeYxtAOVT9W5x6HQ1Wu_rH7dfft5e7_Y_vi-uV1vF229suOCpCLVkVe6QQWgsRHKt1Z6rJUA23aiAe3RNKIxHUj03NeTs1ZKerKg68tqM3O7hM9un8MO88ElDO4kpNy71y85aUkTt2iENpILibxV1nJQRmBnmtXE-jyznnB4h7pfb91R41JyA1a_wORdzt4eJ3SIPo0Z26k62oU2RfJh0tdar6Syittp4WZeaPOUVSb__wJwd0rRzSm6Y4r1P1iVj5E</recordid><startdate>20230302</startdate><enddate>20230302</enddate><creator>Maldonado, Mora</creator><creator>Culbertson, Jennifer</creator><general>Open Library of Humanities</general><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>IAO</scope><scope>1XC</scope><scope>VOOES</scope><scope>DOA</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1737-6296</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4421-2614</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>20230302</creationdate><title>You say yes, I say no: Investigating the link between meaning and form in response particles</title><author>Maldonado, Mora ; Culbertson, Jennifer</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c369t-e45e5def57ba5117ab25fc94fa35219cd2b17fa8b2b8d14af0f3ba53554fe9173</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2023</creationdate><topic>artificial language learning</topic><topic>Cognitive science</topic><topic>Evaluation</topic><topic>Linguistics</topic><topic>Meaning (Philosophy)</topic><topic>response particles</topic><topic>semantics</topic><topic>syntax</topic><topic>typology</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Maldonado, Mora</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Culbertson, Jennifer</creatorcontrib><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Gale Academic OneFile</collection><collection>Hyper Article en Ligne (HAL)</collection><collection>Hyper Article en Ligne (HAL) (Open Access)</collection><collection>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</collection><jtitle>Glossa (London)</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Maldonado, Mora</au><au>Culbertson, Jennifer</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>You say yes, I say no: Investigating the link between meaning and form in response particles</atitle><jtitle>Glossa (London)</jtitle><date>2023-03-02</date><risdate>2023</risdate><volume>8</volume><issue>1</issue><spage>1</spage><pages>1-</pages><issn>2397-1835</issn><eissn>2397-1835</eissn><abstract>Response particles, like English ‘yes’ and ‘no’, are used to respond to polar questions or assertions and are found in all languages. However, the number of particles and the specific meanings they convey vary across languages. For example, in some languages particles mainly convey whether the response itself is positive or negative, while in others they convey whether the response is agreeing or disagreeing with previous discourse. Further, some languages have two response particles, while others have three, or even four. Recent work suggests that how meanings tend to be mapped to forms cross-linguistically might nevertheless be constrained. Roelofsen &amp; Farkas (2015) suggest that indicating disagreement with a negative question or assertion (e.g., A: ‘Ally doesn’t eat meat.’ B: ‘Yes, he does.’) is more marked than indicating agreement with a positive assertion (e.g., A: ‘Ally eats meat.’ B: ‘Yes, he does’.). This difference in semantic markedness is argued to lead to a difference in form: more marked meanings are mapped to more specialized forms. Here we investigate this hypothesis in a series of behavioral experiments. Across our experiments, we find that participants are indeed sensitive to the differences in meaning that particles can convey. However, not all of the differences implicated by the hierarchy hypothesized in Roelofsen &amp; Farkas (2015) are supported by our results, and we find evidence highlighting an unexpected special role for Positive Agreement—the least marked meaning.</abstract><pub>Open Library of Humanities</pub><doi>10.16995/glossa.9185</doi><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1737-6296</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4421-2614</orcidid><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 2397-1835
ispartof Glossa (London), 2023-03, Vol.8 (1), p.1
issn 2397-1835
2397-1835
language eng
recordid cdi_hal_primary_oai_HAL_hal_04408197v1
source DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals; EZB-FREE-00999 freely available EZB journals; Alma/SFX Local Collection
subjects artificial language learning
Cognitive science
Evaluation
Linguistics
Meaning (Philosophy)
response particles
semantics
syntax
typology
title You say yes, I say no: Investigating the link between meaning and form in response particles
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-16T15%3A57%3A11IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-gale_doaj_&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=You%20say%20yes,%20I%20say%20no:%20Investigating%20the%20link%20between%20meaning%20and%20form%20in%20response%20particles&rft.jtitle=Glossa%20(London)&rft.au=Maldonado,%20Mora&rft.date=2023-03-02&rft.volume=8&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=1&rft.pages=1-&rft.issn=2397-1835&rft.eissn=2397-1835&rft_id=info:doi/10.16995/glossa.9185&rft_dat=%3Cgale_doaj_%3EA776459509%3C/gale_doaj_%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_id=info:pmid/&rft_galeid=A776459509&rft_doaj_id=oai_doaj_org_article_49e7e09a82784024a0c59901582ad8b6&rfr_iscdi=true