Developmental changes in impulse control: Trial‐by‐trial EMG dissociates the evolution of impulse strength from its subsequent suppression
Goal‐oriented behavior can be disrupted by irrelevant information that automatically activates incorrect responses. While behavioral errors reveal response capture in such situations, they are only the tip of the iceberg. Additional subliminal activations of the incorrect responses (partial errors)...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Developmental science 2022-11, Vol.25 (6), p.e13273-n/a |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | n/a |
---|---|
container_issue | 6 |
container_start_page | e13273 |
container_title | Developmental science |
container_volume | 25 |
creator | Śmigasiewicz, Kamila Ambrosi, Solène Blaye, Agnès Burle, Boris |
description | Goal‐oriented behavior can be disrupted by irrelevant information that automatically activates incorrect responses. While behavioral errors reveal response capture in such situations, they are only the tip of the iceberg. Additional subliminal activations of the incorrect responses (partial errors) can be revealed on correctly responded trials thanks to electromyography (EMG). In the current study, for the first time, EMG recorded in children was combined with distributional analyses. This allowed to investigate the properties of incorrect response activations and to highlight developmental changes in impulse control. A sample of 114 children aged 6–14 years was studied. Children performed a Simon task in which the irrelevant stimulus‐position automatically activates a response that might be compatible or incompatible with the correct one. On incompatible trials, the automatic response activation must be overcome by controlled response selection. As previously observed in adults, our approach revealed the presence of an automatic EMG activation of the incorrect response elicited by the irrelevant stimulus dimension. Further, it revealed another independent source at the origin of incorrect response activations: the tendency to guess for response alternation. Both sources increased the frequency of early incorrect EMG activations, indicating impulsive responding. In addition, the influence of both sources decreased with increasing age. Thus, development is marked by improved ability to manage distractibility on the one hand and decreased tendency to rely on a guessing strategy on the other. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1111/desc.13273 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_hal_p</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_hal_primary_oai_HAL_hal_03660303v1</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>2729439146</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c4273-37449f8e3acbe3bc3559cf11d2e96cb64b922991383c73495e8159ae7fe812cc3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9kc1u1DAUhSNERUthwwMgS2wAKcWOY3vMrppOf6RBLCgSO8vx3HRcOXFqO4NmxxMgnrFPgqdpZ8ECL-xr6zvH9-oUxRuCT0hen1YQzQmhlaDPiiNSc1EKPhPPc005LQVjPw6LlzHeYoxrismL4pCyWmBG-FHx-ww24PzQQZ-0Q2at-xuIyPbIdsPoIiDj-xS8-4yug9Xu_tefZpu3tLugxZcLtLIxemN1yrK0BgQb78ZkfY98uzeJKUB_k9aoDb5DNkUUxybC3Zi_zeUwBIgxa14VB63OgteP53Hx_XxxPb8sl18vruany9LUecySirqW7QyoNg3QxlDGpGkJWVUguWl43ciqkpLQGTWC1pLBjDCpQbS5qIyhx8WHyXetnRqC7XTYKq-tujxdqt0bppxjiumGZPb9xA7B54ZjUp2NBpzTPfgxqoozxjhhFc7ou3_QWz-GPk-iKlHJmsocT6Y-TpQJPsYA7b4DgtUuUbVLVD0kmuG3j5Zj08Fqjz5FmAEyAT-tg-1_rNTZ4tt8Mv0LhICu2Q</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Access Repository</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2729439146</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Developmental changes in impulse control: Trial‐by‐trial EMG dissociates the evolution of impulse strength from its subsequent suppression</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>Wiley Online Library All Journals</source><creator>Śmigasiewicz, Kamila ; Ambrosi, Solène ; Blaye, Agnès ; Burle, Boris</creator><creatorcontrib>Śmigasiewicz, Kamila ; Ambrosi, Solène ; Blaye, Agnès ; Burle, Boris</creatorcontrib><description>Goal‐oriented behavior can be disrupted by irrelevant information that automatically activates incorrect responses. While behavioral errors reveal response capture in such situations, they are only the tip of the iceberg. Additional subliminal activations of the incorrect responses (partial errors) can be revealed on correctly responded trials thanks to electromyography (EMG). In the current study, for the first time, EMG recorded in children was combined with distributional analyses. This allowed to investigate the properties of incorrect response activations and to highlight developmental changes in impulse control. A sample of 114 children aged 6–14 years was studied. Children performed a Simon task in which the irrelevant stimulus‐position automatically activates a response that might be compatible or incompatible with the correct one. On incompatible trials, the automatic response activation must be overcome by controlled response selection. As previously observed in adults, our approach revealed the presence of an automatic EMG activation of the incorrect response elicited by the irrelevant stimulus dimension. Further, it revealed another independent source at the origin of incorrect response activations: the tendency to guess for response alternation. Both sources increased the frequency of early incorrect EMG activations, indicating impulsive responding. In addition, the influence of both sources decreased with increasing age. Thus, development is marked by improved ability to manage distractibility on the one hand and decreased tendency to rely on a guessing strategy on the other.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1363-755X</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1467-7687</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1111/desc.13273</identifier><identifier>PMID: 35470516</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>England: Wiley Subscription Services, Inc</publisher><subject>Adult ; Attention ; Child ; Children ; Cognitive science ; distributional analysis ; Electromyography ; EMG ; Humans ; impulse control ; Impulsive behavior ; Impulsive Behavior - physiology ; Impulsivity ; Neuroscience ; partial errors ; Psychology ; Psychomotor Performance - physiology ; Reaction Time - physiology ; response transition ; susceptibility to interference</subject><ispartof>Developmental science, 2022-11, Vol.25 (6), p.e13273-n/a</ispartof><rights>2022 The Authors. published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd.</rights><rights>2022 The Authors. Developmental Science published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd.</rights><rights>2022. This article is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.</rights><rights>Distributed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License</rights><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c4273-37449f8e3acbe3bc3559cf11d2e96cb64b922991383c73495e8159ae7fe812cc3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c4273-37449f8e3acbe3bc3559cf11d2e96cb64b922991383c73495e8159ae7fe812cc3</cites><orcidid>0000-0001-8179-9034 ; 0000-0001-9475-0221 ; 0000-0002-4514-6519</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/pdf/10.1111%2Fdesc.13273$$EPDF$$P50$$Gwiley$$Hfree_for_read</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1111%2Fdesc.13273$$EHTML$$P50$$Gwiley$$Hfree_for_read</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,315,781,785,886,1418,27925,27926,45575,45576</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35470516$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://hal.science/hal-03660303$$DView record in HAL$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Śmigasiewicz, Kamila</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Ambrosi, Solène</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Blaye, Agnès</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Burle, Boris</creatorcontrib><title>Developmental changes in impulse control: Trial‐by‐trial EMG dissociates the evolution of impulse strength from its subsequent suppression</title><title>Developmental science</title><addtitle>Dev Sci</addtitle><description>Goal‐oriented behavior can be disrupted by irrelevant information that automatically activates incorrect responses. While behavioral errors reveal response capture in such situations, they are only the tip of the iceberg. Additional subliminal activations of the incorrect responses (partial errors) can be revealed on correctly responded trials thanks to electromyography (EMG). In the current study, for the first time, EMG recorded in children was combined with distributional analyses. This allowed to investigate the properties of incorrect response activations and to highlight developmental changes in impulse control. A sample of 114 children aged 6–14 years was studied. Children performed a Simon task in which the irrelevant stimulus‐position automatically activates a response that might be compatible or incompatible with the correct one. On incompatible trials, the automatic response activation must be overcome by controlled response selection. As previously observed in adults, our approach revealed the presence of an automatic EMG activation of the incorrect response elicited by the irrelevant stimulus dimension. Further, it revealed another independent source at the origin of incorrect response activations: the tendency to guess for response alternation. Both sources increased the frequency of early incorrect EMG activations, indicating impulsive responding. In addition, the influence of both sources decreased with increasing age. Thus, development is marked by improved ability to manage distractibility on the one hand and decreased tendency to rely on a guessing strategy on the other.</description><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Attention</subject><subject>Child</subject><subject>Children</subject><subject>Cognitive science</subject><subject>distributional analysis</subject><subject>Electromyography</subject><subject>EMG</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>impulse control</subject><subject>Impulsive behavior</subject><subject>Impulsive Behavior - physiology</subject><subject>Impulsivity</subject><subject>Neuroscience</subject><subject>partial errors</subject><subject>Psychology</subject><subject>Psychomotor Performance - physiology</subject><subject>Reaction Time - physiology</subject><subject>response transition</subject><subject>susceptibility to interference</subject><issn>1363-755X</issn><issn>1467-7687</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2022</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>24P</sourceid><sourceid>WIN</sourceid><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><recordid>eNp9kc1u1DAUhSNERUthwwMgS2wAKcWOY3vMrppOf6RBLCgSO8vx3HRcOXFqO4NmxxMgnrFPgqdpZ8ECL-xr6zvH9-oUxRuCT0hen1YQzQmhlaDPiiNSc1EKPhPPc005LQVjPw6LlzHeYoxrismL4pCyWmBG-FHx-ww24PzQQZ-0Q2at-xuIyPbIdsPoIiDj-xS8-4yug9Xu_tefZpu3tLugxZcLtLIxemN1yrK0BgQb78ZkfY98uzeJKUB_k9aoDb5DNkUUxybC3Zi_zeUwBIgxa14VB63OgteP53Hx_XxxPb8sl18vruany9LUecySirqW7QyoNg3QxlDGpGkJWVUguWl43ciqkpLQGTWC1pLBjDCpQbS5qIyhx8WHyXetnRqC7XTYKq-tujxdqt0bppxjiumGZPb9xA7B54ZjUp2NBpzTPfgxqoozxjhhFc7ou3_QWz-GPk-iKlHJmsocT6Y-TpQJPsYA7b4DgtUuUbVLVD0kmuG3j5Zj08Fqjz5FmAEyAT-tg-1_rNTZ4tt8Mv0LhICu2Q</recordid><startdate>202211</startdate><enddate>202211</enddate><creator>Śmigasiewicz, Kamila</creator><creator>Ambrosi, Solène</creator><creator>Blaye, Agnès</creator><creator>Burle, Boris</creator><general>Wiley Subscription Services, Inc</general><general>Wiley</general><scope>24P</scope><scope>WIN</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7QP</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>1XC</scope><scope>VOOES</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8179-9034</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9475-0221</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4514-6519</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>202211</creationdate><title>Developmental changes in impulse control: Trial‐by‐trial EMG dissociates the evolution of impulse strength from its subsequent suppression</title><author>Śmigasiewicz, Kamila ; Ambrosi, Solène ; Blaye, Agnès ; Burle, Boris</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c4273-37449f8e3acbe3bc3559cf11d2e96cb64b922991383c73495e8159ae7fe812cc3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2022</creationdate><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Attention</topic><topic>Child</topic><topic>Children</topic><topic>Cognitive science</topic><topic>distributional analysis</topic><topic>Electromyography</topic><topic>EMG</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>impulse control</topic><topic>Impulsive behavior</topic><topic>Impulsive Behavior - physiology</topic><topic>Impulsivity</topic><topic>Neuroscience</topic><topic>partial errors</topic><topic>Psychology</topic><topic>Psychomotor Performance - physiology</topic><topic>Reaction Time - physiology</topic><topic>response transition</topic><topic>susceptibility to interference</topic><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Śmigasiewicz, Kamila</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Ambrosi, Solène</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Blaye, Agnès</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Burle, Boris</creatorcontrib><collection>Wiley Online Library Open Access</collection><collection>Wiley Online Library Free Content</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>Hyper Article en Ligne (HAL)</collection><collection>Hyper Article en Ligne (HAL) (Open Access)</collection><jtitle>Developmental science</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Śmigasiewicz, Kamila</au><au>Ambrosi, Solène</au><au>Blaye, Agnès</au><au>Burle, Boris</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Developmental changes in impulse control: Trial‐by‐trial EMG dissociates the evolution of impulse strength from its subsequent suppression</atitle><jtitle>Developmental science</jtitle><addtitle>Dev Sci</addtitle><date>2022-11</date><risdate>2022</risdate><volume>25</volume><issue>6</issue><spage>e13273</spage><epage>n/a</epage><pages>e13273-n/a</pages><issn>1363-755X</issn><eissn>1467-7687</eissn><abstract>Goal‐oriented behavior can be disrupted by irrelevant information that automatically activates incorrect responses. While behavioral errors reveal response capture in such situations, they are only the tip of the iceberg. Additional subliminal activations of the incorrect responses (partial errors) can be revealed on correctly responded trials thanks to electromyography (EMG). In the current study, for the first time, EMG recorded in children was combined with distributional analyses. This allowed to investigate the properties of incorrect response activations and to highlight developmental changes in impulse control. A sample of 114 children aged 6–14 years was studied. Children performed a Simon task in which the irrelevant stimulus‐position automatically activates a response that might be compatible or incompatible with the correct one. On incompatible trials, the automatic response activation must be overcome by controlled response selection. As previously observed in adults, our approach revealed the presence of an automatic EMG activation of the incorrect response elicited by the irrelevant stimulus dimension. Further, it revealed another independent source at the origin of incorrect response activations: the tendency to guess for response alternation. Both sources increased the frequency of early incorrect EMG activations, indicating impulsive responding. In addition, the influence of both sources decreased with increasing age. Thus, development is marked by improved ability to manage distractibility on the one hand and decreased tendency to rely on a guessing strategy on the other.</abstract><cop>England</cop><pub>Wiley Subscription Services, Inc</pub><pmid>35470516</pmid><doi>10.1111/desc.13273</doi><tpages>16</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8179-9034</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9475-0221</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4514-6519</orcidid><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 1363-755X |
ispartof | Developmental science, 2022-11, Vol.25 (6), p.e13273-n/a |
issn | 1363-755X 1467-7687 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_hal_primary_oai_HAL_hal_03660303v1 |
source | MEDLINE; Wiley Online Library All Journals |
subjects | Adult Attention Child Children Cognitive science distributional analysis Electromyography EMG Humans impulse control Impulsive behavior Impulsive Behavior - physiology Impulsivity Neuroscience partial errors Psychology Psychomotor Performance - physiology Reaction Time - physiology response transition susceptibility to interference |
title | Developmental changes in impulse control: Trial‐by‐trial EMG dissociates the evolution of impulse strength from its subsequent suppression |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-18T14%3A58%3A50IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_hal_p&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Developmental%20changes%20in%20impulse%20control:%20Trial%E2%80%90by%E2%80%90trial%20EMG%20dissociates%20the%20evolution%20of%20impulse%20strength%20from%20its%20subsequent%20suppression&rft.jtitle=Developmental%20science&rft.au=%C5%9Amigasiewicz,%20Kamila&rft.date=2022-11&rft.volume=25&rft.issue=6&rft.spage=e13273&rft.epage=n/a&rft.pages=e13273-n/a&rft.issn=1363-755X&rft.eissn=1467-7687&rft_id=info:doi/10.1111/desc.13273&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_hal_p%3E2729439146%3C/proquest_hal_p%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2729439146&rft_id=info:pmid/35470516&rfr_iscdi=true |