Priming plural ambiguities

•Distributive/cumulative ambiguity gives rise to semantic priming.•The nature of the priming effect is asymmetric: only distributivity is primed trial-to-trial.•Distributivity involves specific mental mechanisms that can be primed. Sentences that involve two or more plural expressions, such as numer...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Journal of memory and language 2017-08, Vol.95, p.89-101
Hauptverfasser: Maldonado, Mora, Chemla, Emmanuel, Spector, Benjamin
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 101
container_issue
container_start_page 89
container_title Journal of memory and language
container_volume 95
creator Maldonado, Mora
Chemla, Emmanuel
Spector, Benjamin
description •Distributive/cumulative ambiguity gives rise to semantic priming.•The nature of the priming effect is asymmetric: only distributivity is primed trial-to-trial.•Distributivity involves specific mental mechanisms that can be primed. Sentences that involve two or more plural expressions, such as numerical expressions, give rise to systematic ambiguities. For example, the sentence Two boys have three balloons can either mean that there are two boys who, between them, have three balloons (cumulative reading) or that there are two boys who each have three balloons (distributive reading). In this paper, we report the results of three experiments which show that the distributive/cumulative ambiguity can give rise to priming effects. That is, when subjects perform a sentence-picture matching task which creates a strong bias towards one of the two types of readings, they are more likely to access the very same type of interpretation when subsequently presented with a different sentence-picture pair which does not create the same bias. This finding suggests that the abstract constructs that linguists posit to account for different types of readings describe some real features of mental representations.
doi_str_mv 10.1016/j.jml.2017.02.002
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>elsevier_hal_p</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_hal_primary_oai_HAL_hal_02474191v1</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><els_id>S0749596X17300098</els_id><sourcerecordid>S0749596X17300098</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c374t-e4d0eb53ac16ba1383bf06e6b58b656f8746199d294e3273adb2e6bdb3bebcbe3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9kE1LAzEQhoMoWD9-gJ569bDrTJJNdvFUilqhoAcFbyHJztYs2w-StuC_N6Xi0dPAzPsMMw9jNwglAqr7vuyXQ8kBdQm8BOAnbITQqAJqjqdsBFo2RdWoz3N2kVIPgFhpPmK3bzEsw2ox3gy7aIexXbqw2IVtoHTFzjo7JLr-rZfs4-nxfTor5q_PL9PJvPBCy21BsgVylbAelbMoauE6UKRcVTtVqa7WUmHTtLyRJLgWtnU8T1snHDnvSFyyu-PeLzuYTT7Hxm-ztsHMJnNz6AGXWmKDe8xZPGZ9XKcUqfsDEMxBhOlNFmEOIjJnsojMPBwZyk_sA0WTfKCVpzZE8lvTrsM_9A9wAmTa</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Access Repository</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype></control><display><type>article</type><title>Priming plural ambiguities</title><source>Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals Complete</source><creator>Maldonado, Mora ; Chemla, Emmanuel ; Spector, Benjamin</creator><creatorcontrib>Maldonado, Mora ; Chemla, Emmanuel ; Spector, Benjamin</creatorcontrib><description>•Distributive/cumulative ambiguity gives rise to semantic priming.•The nature of the priming effect is asymmetric: only distributivity is primed trial-to-trial.•Distributivity involves specific mental mechanisms that can be primed. Sentences that involve two or more plural expressions, such as numerical expressions, give rise to systematic ambiguities. For example, the sentence Two boys have three balloons can either mean that there are two boys who, between them, have three balloons (cumulative reading) or that there are two boys who each have three balloons (distributive reading). In this paper, we report the results of three experiments which show that the distributive/cumulative ambiguity can give rise to priming effects. That is, when subjects perform a sentence-picture matching task which creates a strong bias towards one of the two types of readings, they are more likely to access the very same type of interpretation when subsequently presented with a different sentence-picture pair which does not create the same bias. This finding suggests that the abstract constructs that linguists posit to account for different types of readings describe some real features of mental representations.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0749-596X</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1096-0821</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2017.02.002</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Elsevier Inc</publisher><subject>Cognitive science ; Distributivity ; Plural ambiguities ; Priming ; Semantics</subject><ispartof>Journal of memory and language, 2017-08, Vol.95, p.89-101</ispartof><rights>2017 Elsevier Inc.</rights><rights>Distributed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c374t-e4d0eb53ac16ba1383bf06e6b58b656f8746199d294e3273adb2e6bdb3bebcbe3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c374t-e4d0eb53ac16ba1383bf06e6b58b656f8746199d294e3273adb2e6bdb3bebcbe3</cites><orcidid>0000-0003-0468-5986 ; 0000-0002-8423-5880</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2017.02.002$$EHTML$$P50$$Gelsevier$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,780,784,885,3550,27924,27925,45995</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://hal.science/hal-02474191$$DView record in HAL$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Maldonado, Mora</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Chemla, Emmanuel</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Spector, Benjamin</creatorcontrib><title>Priming plural ambiguities</title><title>Journal of memory and language</title><description>•Distributive/cumulative ambiguity gives rise to semantic priming.•The nature of the priming effect is asymmetric: only distributivity is primed trial-to-trial.•Distributivity involves specific mental mechanisms that can be primed. Sentences that involve two or more plural expressions, such as numerical expressions, give rise to systematic ambiguities. For example, the sentence Two boys have three balloons can either mean that there are two boys who, between them, have three balloons (cumulative reading) or that there are two boys who each have three balloons (distributive reading). In this paper, we report the results of three experiments which show that the distributive/cumulative ambiguity can give rise to priming effects. That is, when subjects perform a sentence-picture matching task which creates a strong bias towards one of the two types of readings, they are more likely to access the very same type of interpretation when subsequently presented with a different sentence-picture pair which does not create the same bias. This finding suggests that the abstract constructs that linguists posit to account for different types of readings describe some real features of mental representations.</description><subject>Cognitive science</subject><subject>Distributivity</subject><subject>Plural ambiguities</subject><subject>Priming</subject><subject>Semantics</subject><issn>0749-596X</issn><issn>1096-0821</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2017</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><recordid>eNp9kE1LAzEQhoMoWD9-gJ569bDrTJJNdvFUilqhoAcFbyHJztYs2w-StuC_N6Xi0dPAzPsMMw9jNwglAqr7vuyXQ8kBdQm8BOAnbITQqAJqjqdsBFo2RdWoz3N2kVIPgFhpPmK3bzEsw2ox3gy7aIexXbqw2IVtoHTFzjo7JLr-rZfs4-nxfTor5q_PL9PJvPBCy21BsgVylbAelbMoauE6UKRcVTtVqa7WUmHTtLyRJLgWtnU8T1snHDnvSFyyu-PeLzuYTT7Hxm-ztsHMJnNz6AGXWmKDe8xZPGZ9XKcUqfsDEMxBhOlNFmEOIjJnsojMPBwZyk_sA0WTfKCVpzZE8lvTrsM_9A9wAmTa</recordid><startdate>201708</startdate><enddate>201708</enddate><creator>Maldonado, Mora</creator><creator>Chemla, Emmanuel</creator><creator>Spector, Benjamin</creator><general>Elsevier Inc</general><general>Elsevier</general><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>1XC</scope><scope>VOOES</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-0468-5986</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-8423-5880</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>201708</creationdate><title>Priming plural ambiguities</title><author>Maldonado, Mora ; Chemla, Emmanuel ; Spector, Benjamin</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c374t-e4d0eb53ac16ba1383bf06e6b58b656f8746199d294e3273adb2e6bdb3bebcbe3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2017</creationdate><topic>Cognitive science</topic><topic>Distributivity</topic><topic>Plural ambiguities</topic><topic>Priming</topic><topic>Semantics</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Maldonado, Mora</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Chemla, Emmanuel</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Spector, Benjamin</creatorcontrib><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Hyper Article en Ligne (HAL)</collection><collection>Hyper Article en Ligne (HAL) (Open Access)</collection><jtitle>Journal of memory and language</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Maldonado, Mora</au><au>Chemla, Emmanuel</au><au>Spector, Benjamin</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Priming plural ambiguities</atitle><jtitle>Journal of memory and language</jtitle><date>2017-08</date><risdate>2017</risdate><volume>95</volume><spage>89</spage><epage>101</epage><pages>89-101</pages><issn>0749-596X</issn><eissn>1096-0821</eissn><abstract>•Distributive/cumulative ambiguity gives rise to semantic priming.•The nature of the priming effect is asymmetric: only distributivity is primed trial-to-trial.•Distributivity involves specific mental mechanisms that can be primed. Sentences that involve two or more plural expressions, such as numerical expressions, give rise to systematic ambiguities. For example, the sentence Two boys have three balloons can either mean that there are two boys who, between them, have three balloons (cumulative reading) or that there are two boys who each have three balloons (distributive reading). In this paper, we report the results of three experiments which show that the distributive/cumulative ambiguity can give rise to priming effects. That is, when subjects perform a sentence-picture matching task which creates a strong bias towards one of the two types of readings, they are more likely to access the very same type of interpretation when subsequently presented with a different sentence-picture pair which does not create the same bias. This finding suggests that the abstract constructs that linguists posit to account for different types of readings describe some real features of mental representations.</abstract><pub>Elsevier Inc</pub><doi>10.1016/j.jml.2017.02.002</doi><tpages>13</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-0468-5986</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-8423-5880</orcidid><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0749-596X
ispartof Journal of memory and language, 2017-08, Vol.95, p.89-101
issn 0749-596X
1096-0821
language eng
recordid cdi_hal_primary_oai_HAL_hal_02474191v1
source Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals Complete
subjects Cognitive science
Distributivity
Plural ambiguities
Priming
Semantics
title Priming plural ambiguities
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-29T11%3A16%3A32IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-elsevier_hal_p&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Priming%20plural%20ambiguities&rft.jtitle=Journal%20of%20memory%20and%20language&rft.au=Maldonado,%20Mora&rft.date=2017-08&rft.volume=95&rft.spage=89&rft.epage=101&rft.pages=89-101&rft.issn=0749-596X&rft.eissn=1096-0821&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/j.jml.2017.02.002&rft_dat=%3Celsevier_hal_p%3ES0749596X17300098%3C/elsevier_hal_p%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_id=info:pmid/&rft_els_id=S0749596X17300098&rfr_iscdi=true